Neurogenesis and Neuroplasticity in Major Depression: Its Therapeutic Implication

  • First Online: 09 April 2021

Cite this chapter

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  • Michel Bourin 7  

Part of the book series: Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology ((AEMB,volume 1305))

3757 Accesses

7 Citations

The neurochemical model of depression, based on monoaminergic theories, does not allow on its own to understand the mechanism of action of antidepressants. This approach does not explain the gap between the immediate biochemical modulations induced by antidepressants and the time required for their clinical action. Several hypotheses have been developed to try to explain more precisely the action of these molecules, each of them involving mechanisms of receptor regulation. At the same time, data on the neuroanatomy of depression converge toward the existence of specific lesions of this pathology. This chapter aims to provide an overview of recent advances in understanding the mechanisms of neural plasticity involved in pathophysiology depression and in its treatment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Altman J (1963) Differences in the utilization of tritiated leucine by single neurons in normal and exercised rats: an autoradiographic investigation with microdensitometry. Nature 199:777–780

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Altman J (1969) Autoradiographic and histological studies of postnatal neurogenesis. IV. Cell proliferation and migration in the anterior forebrain, with special reference to persisting neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb. J Comp Neurol 137:433–457

Altman J, Das GD (1965) Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. J Comp Neurol 124:319–335

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Kaplan MS, Hinds JW (1977) Neurogenesis in the adult rat: electron microscopic analysis of light radioautographs. Science 197:1092–1094

Nottebohm F (2002) Neuronal replacement in adult brain. Brain Res Bull 57:737–749

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Reynolds BA, Weiss S (1992) Generation of neurons and astrocytes from isolated cells of the adult mammalian central nervous system. Science 255:1707–1710

Ming GL, Song H (2005) Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian central nervous system. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:223–250

Toda T, Gage FH (2018) Review: adult neurogenesis contributes to hippocampal plasticity. Cell Tissue Res 373:693–709

Oakes P, Loukas M, Oskouian RJ, Tubbs RS (2017) The neuroanatomy of depression: a review. Clin Anat 30:44–49

Liu W, Ge T, Leng Y, Pan Z, Fan J, Yang W, Cui R (2017) The role of neural plasticity in depression: from hippocampus to prefrontal cortex. Neural Plast 2017:6871089. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/6871089

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Campbell S, Macqueen G (2004) The role of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci 29:417–426

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Lyons DM, Yang C, Sawyer-Glover AM, Moseley ME, Schatzberg AF (2001) Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:1145–1151

Maller JJ, Broadhouse K, Rush AJ, Gordon E, Koslow S, Grieve SM (2018) Increased hippocampal tail volume predicts depression status and remission to anti-depressant medications in major depression. Mol Psychiatry 23:1737–1744

Zhong M, Wang X, Xiao J, Yi J, Zhu X, Liao J, Wang W, Yao S (2011) Amygdala hyperactivation and prefrontal hypoactivation in subjects with cognitive vulnerability to depression. Biol Psychol 88:233–242

Ruhé HG, Koster M, Booij J, van Herk M, Veltman DJ, Schene AH (2014) Occupancy of serotonin transporters in the amygdala by paroxetine in association with attenuation of left amygdala activation by negative faces in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res 221:155–161

Pittenger C, Duman RS (2008) Stress, depression, and neuroplasticity: a convergence of mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:88–109

Drevets WC (2004) Neuroplasticity in mood disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 6:199–216

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Goodwin GM (2016) Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal lobes in depression: 20 years on. J Psychopharmacol 30:1090–1094

Nelson BD, Kessel EM, Klein DN, Shankman SA (2018) Depression symptom dimensions and asymmetrical frontal cortical activity while anticipating reward. Psychophysiology 55(1):e12892. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12892

Article   Google Scholar  

Bond AM, Ming GL, Song H (2015) Adult mammalian neural stem cells and neurogenesis: five decades later. Cell Stem Cell 17:385–395

Bonaguidi MA, Wheeler MA, Shapiro JS, Stadel RP, Sun GJ, Ming GL, Song H (2011) In vivo clonal analysis reveals self-renewing and multipotent adult neural stem cell characteristics. Cell 145:1142–1155

Bonaguidi MA, Song J, Ming GL, Song H (2012) A unifying hypothesis on mammalian neural stem cell properties in the adult hippocampus. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:754–761

Pardal R, López BJ (2016) Mature neurons modulate neurogenesis through chemical signals acting on neural stem cells. Develop Growth Differ 58:456–462

Aimone JB, Li Y, Lee SW, Clemenson GD, Deng W, Gage FH (2014) Regulation and function of adult neurogenesis: from genes to cognition. Physiol Rev 94:991–1026

Crowther AJ, Song J (2014) Activity-dependent signaling mechanisms regulating adult hippocampal neural stem cells and their progeny. Neurosci Bull 30:542–556

Conover JC, Todd KL (2017) Development and aging of a brain neural stem cell niche. Exp Gerontol 94:9–13

Namba T, Huttner WB (2017) Neural progenitor cells and their role in the development and evolutionary expansion of the neocortex. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Dev Biol 6(1):e256. https://doi.org/10.1002/wdev.256

Capilla-Gonzalez V, Herranz-Pérez V, García-Verdugo JM (2015) The aged brain: genesis and fate of residual progenitor cells in the subventricular zone. Front Cell Neurosci 9:365. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00365

Martoncikova M, Fabianova K, Schreiberova A, Blasko J, Almasiova V, Racekova E (2014) Astrocytic and vascular scaffolding for neuroblast migration in the rostral migratory stream. Curr Neurovasc Res 11:321–329

Carleton A, Petreanu LT, Lansford R, Alvarez-Buylla A, Lledo PM (2003) Becoming a new neuron in the adult olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 6:507–518

Morales-Garcia JA, Echeverry-Alzate V, Alonso-Gil S, Sanz-SanCristobal M, Lopez-Moreno JA, Gil C, Martinez A, Santos A, Perez-Castillo A (2017) Phosphodiesterase7 inhibition activates adult neurogenesis in hippocampus and subventricular zone in vitro and in vivo. Stem Cells 35:458–472

Inta D, Cameron HA, Gass P (2015) New neurons in the adult striatum: from rodents to humans. Trends Neurosci 38:517–523

Li Q-Q, Qiao G-Q, Ma J, Fan H-W, Li Y-B (2015) Cortical neurogenesis in adult rats after ischemic brain injury: most new neurons fail to mature. Neural Regen Res 10:277–285

Lepousez G, Nissant A, Lledo PM (2015) Adult neurogenesis and the future of the rejuvenating brain circuits. Neuron 86:387–401

Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moal M (2005) Adult neurogenesis: from precursors to network and physiology. Physiol Rev 85:523–569

Lledo PM, Gheusi G (2006) Adult neurogenesis: from basic research to clinical applications. Bull Acad Natl Med 190:385–400

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Egeland M, Zunszain PA, Pariante CM (2015) Molecular mechanisms in the regulation of adult neurogenesis during stress. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:189–200

Sahay A, Scobie KN, Hill AS, O’Carroll CM, Kheirbek MA, Burghardt NS, Fenton AA, Dranovsky A, Hen R (2011) Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472:466–470

Aasebø IE, Blankvoort S, Tashiro A (2011) Critical maturational period of new neurons in adult dentate gyrus for their involvement in memory formation. Eur J Neurosci 33:1094–1100

Drapeau E, Nora AD (2008) Stem cell review series: role of neurogenesis in age-related memory disorders. Aging Cell 7:569–589

Dennis CV, Suh LS, Rodriguez ML, Kril JJ, Sutherland GT (2016) Human adult neurogenesis across the ages: an immunohistochemical study. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 42:621–638

Gray JD, Kogan JF, Marrocco J, McEwen BS (2017) Genomic and epigenomic mechanisms of glucocorticoids in the brain. Nat Rev Endocrinol 13:661–673

Peeters B, Langouche L, Van den Berghe G (2017) Adrenocortical stress response during the course of critical illness. Compr Physiol 8:283–298

Malberg JE, Duman RS (2003) Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by inescapable stress: reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1562–1571

Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Anderson GM, Luckenbaugh D, Anderson ER, Snow J, Staib LH, Charney DS, Bremner JD (2004) Hippocampal volume, memory, and cortisol status in major depressive disorder: effects of treatment. Biol Psychiatry 56:101–112

Koutmani Y, Politis PK, Elkouris M, Agrogiannis G, Kemerli M, Patsouris E, Remboutsika E, Karalis KP (2013) Corticotropin-releasing hormone exerts direct effects on neuronal progenitor cells: implications for neuroprotection. Mol Psychiatry 18:300–307

Bothwell M (2014) NGF, BDNF, NT3, and NT4. Handb Exp Pharmacol 220:3–15

Hing B, Sathyaputri L, Potash JB (2018) A comprehensive review of genetic and epigenetic mechanisms that regulate BDNF expression and function with relevance to major depressive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 177:143–167

Sandhya VK, Raju R, Verma R, Advani J, Sharma R, Radhakrishnan A, Nanjappa V, Narayana J, Somani BL, Mukherjee KK, Pandey A, Christopher R, Prasad TS (2013) A network map of BDNF/TRKB and BDNF/p75NTR signaling system. J Cell Commun Signal 7:301–307

Kojima M, Mizui T (2017) BDNF propeptide: a novel modulator of synaptic plasticity. Vitam Horm 104:19–28

Martin JL, Finsterwald C (2011) Cooperation between BDNF and glutamate in the regulation of synaptic transmission and neuronal development. Commun Integr Biol 4:14–16

Colino-Oliveira M, Rombo DM, Dias RB, Ribeiro JA, Sebastião AM (2016) BDNF-induced presynaptic facilitation of GABAergic transmission in the hippocampus of young adults is dependent of TrkB and adenosine A2A receptors. Purinergic Signal 12:283–294

Szapacs ME, Mathews TA, Tessarollo L, Ernest Lyons W, Mamounas LA, Andrews AM (2004) Exploring the relationship between serotonin and brain-derived neurotrophic factor: analysis of BDNF protein and extraneuronal 5-HT in mice with reduced serotonin transporter or BDNF expression. J Neurosci Methods 140:81–92

Narita M, Aoki K, Takagi M, Yajima Y, Suzuki T (2003) Implication of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the release of dopamine and dopamine-related behaviors induced by methamphetamine. Neuroscience 119:767–775

D’Sa C, Duman RS (2002) Antidepressants and neuroplasticity. Bipolar Disord 4:183–194

Khakpai F, Zarrindast MR, Nasehi M, Haeri-Rohani A, Eidi A (2013) The role of glutamatergic pathway between septum and hippocampus in the memory formation. EXCLI J 12:41–51. eCollection 2013

Joca SR, Ferreira FR, Guimarães FS (2007) Modulation of stress consequences by hippocampal monoaminergic, glutamatergic and nitrergic neurotransmitter systems. Stress 10:227–249

Manji HK, Quiroz JA, Sporn J, Payne JL, Denicoff KA, Gray N, Zarate CA Jr, Charney DS (2003) Enhancing neuronal plasticity and cellular resilience to develop novel, improved therapeutics for difficult-to-treat depression. Biol Psychiatry 53:707–742

McEwen BS (1997) Possible mechanisms for atrophy of the human hippocampus. Mol Psychiatry 2:255–262

Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L (2003) The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:1002–1012

Villa RF, Ferrari F, Moretti A (2018) Post-stroke depression: mechanisms and pharmacological treatment. Pharmacol Ther 184:131–144

Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R, Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R (2003) Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301:805–809

Cantone M, Bramanti A, Lanza G, Pennisi M, Bramanti P, Pennisi G, Bella R (2017) Cortical plasticity in depression. ASN Neuro 9(3):1759091417711512

Kraus C, Castrén E, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R (2017) Serotonin and neuroplasticity—links between molecular, functional and structural pathophysiology in depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 77:317–326

Liguz-Lecznar M, Lehner M, Kaliszewska A, Zakrzewska R, Sobolewska A, Kossut M (2015) Altered glutamate/GABA equilibrium in aged mice cortex influences cortical plasticity. Brain Struct Funct 220:1681–1693

Greger IH, Watson JF, Cull-Candy SG (2017) Structural and functional architecture of AMPA-type glutamate receptors and their auxiliary proteins. Neuron 94:713–730

Réus GZ, Abelaira HM, Tuon T, Titus SE, Ignácio ZM, Rodrigues AL, Quevedo J (2016) Glutamatergic NMDA receptor as therapeutic target for depression. Adv Protein Chem Struct Biol 103:169–202

Article   PubMed   CAS   Google Scholar  

Bourin M (2019) Why ketamine is a new treatment of resistant depression? SOJ Pharm Sci 6(2):1–3. https://doi.org/10.15226/2374-6866/6/2/00198

Levy MJF, Boulle F, Steinbusch HW, van den Hove DLA, Kenis G, Lanfumey L (2018) Neurotrophic factors and neuroplasticity pathways in the pathophysiology and treatment of depression. Psychopharmacology 235:2195–2220

Malberg JE (2004) Implications of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in antidepressant action. J Psychiatry Neurosci 29:196–205

Sahay A, Drew MR, Hen R (2007) Dentate gyrus neurogenesis and depression. Prog Brain Res 163:697–722

Sahay A, Hen R (2007) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat Neurosci 10:1110–1115

Castrén E, Kojima M (2017) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in mood disorders and antidepressant treatments. Neurobiol Dis 97(Pt B):119–126

Ostadhadi S, Ahangari M, Nikoui V, Norouzi-Javidan A, Zolfaghari S, Jazaeri F, Chamanara M, Akbarian R, Dehpour AR (2016) Pharmacological evidence for the involvement of the NMDA receptor and nitric oxide pathway in the antidepressant-like effect of lamotrigine in the mouse forced swimming test. Biomed Pharmacother 82:713–721

Bourin M, Hascoet M, Masse F (2005) Evidence of the activity of lamotrigine on 5-HT1A receptors in the mouse forced swimming test. J Psychiatry Neurosci 30:275–282

Okada M, Fukuyama K, Kawano Y, Shiroyama T, Ueda Y (2019) Memantine protects thalamocortical hyper-glutamatergic transmission induced by NMDA receptor antagonism via activation of system xc. Pharmacol Res Perspect 7(1):e00457. https://doi.org/10.1002/prp2.457

Khan AJ, LaCava S, Mehta M, Schiff D, Thandoni A, Jhawar S, Danish S, Haffty BG, Chen S (2019) The glutamate release inhibitor riluzole increases DNA damage and enhances cytotoxicity in human glioma cells, in vitro and in vivo. Oncotarget 10:2824–2834

Dutta A, McKie S, Deakin JFW (2015) Ketamine and other potential glutamate antidepressants. Psychiatry Res 225:1–13

Kanzari A, Bourcier-Lucas C, Freyssin A, Abrous DN, Haddjeri N, Lucas G (2018) Inducing a long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus is sufficient to produce rapid antidepressant-like effects. Mol Psychiatry 23:587–596

Eliwa H, Belzung C, Surget A (2017) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis: is it the alpha and omega of antidepressant action? Biochem Pharmacol 141:86–99

Strawbridge WJ, Deleger S, Roberts RE, Kaplan GA (2002) Physical activity reduces the risk of subsequent depression for older adults. Am J Epidemiol 156:328–334

Van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (1999) Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci 2:266–270

Bourin M (2018) Post-stroke depression and changes in behavior and personality. Arch Depress Anxiety 4(1):031–033

Google Scholar  

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Neurobiology of Mood Disorders, University of Nantes, Nantes, France

Michel Bourin

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michel Bourin .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Department of Psychiatry, Korea University Ansan Hospital, College of Medicine, Ansan, Korea (Republic of)

Yong-Ku Kim

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Bourin, M. (2021). Neurogenesis and Neuroplasticity in Major Depression: Its Therapeutic Implication. In: Kim, YK. (eds) Major Depressive Disorder. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, vol 1305. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-33-6044-0_10

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-33-6044-0_10

Published : 09 April 2021

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-33-6043-3

Online ISBN : 978-981-33-6044-0

eBook Packages : Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

The current state of the neurogenic theory of depression and anxiety

Affiliations.

  • 1 Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, Division of Integrative Neuroscience, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA.
  • 2 Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, Department of Pharmacology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, Division of Integrative Neuroscience, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA. Electronic address: [email protected].
  • PMID: 25240202
  • PMCID: PMC4293252
  • DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.012

Newborn neurons are continuously added to the adult hippocampus. Early studies found that adult neurogenesis is impaired in models of depression and anxiety and accelerated by antidepressant treatment. This led to the theory that depression results from impaired adult neurogenesis and restoration of adult neurogenesis leads to recovery. Follow up studies yielded a complex body of often inconsistent results, and the veracity of this theory is uncertain. We propose five criteria for acceptance of this theory, we review the recent evidence for each criterion, and we draw the following conclusions: Diverse animal models of depression and anxiety have impaired neurogenesis. Neurogenesis is consistently boosted by antidepressants in animal models only when animals are stressed. Ablation of neurogenesis in animal models impairs cognitive functions relevant to depression, but only a minority of studies find that ablation causes depression or anxiety. Recent human neuroimaging and postmortem studies are consistent with the neurogenic theory, but they are indirect. Finally, a novel drug developed based on the neurogenic theory is promising in animal models.

Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Publication types

  • Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
  • Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
  • Antidepressive Agents / therapeutic use
  • Anxiety / drug therapy
  • Anxiety / pathology*
  • Depression / drug therapy
  • Depression / pathology*
  • Disease Models, Animal
  • Hippocampus / pathology
  • Neurogenesis / drug effects
  • Neurogenesis / physiology*
  • Neurons / drug effects
  • Neurons / physiology*
  • Antidepressive Agents

Grants and funding

  • R25 MH086466/MH/NIMH NIH HHS/United States
  • R25 MH086466-03/MH/NIMH NIH HHS/United States
  • R01 AG043688/AG/NIA NIH HHS/United States
  • R01 NS081203/NS/NINDS NIH HHS/United States
  • R37 MH068542/MH/NIMH NIH HHS/United States
  • R01NS081203-01A1/NS/NINDS NIH HHS/United States

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here .

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

The reduction of adult neurogenesis in depression impairs the retrieval of new as well as remote episodic memory

Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

Affiliations Institute for Neural Computation, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany, Mercator Research Group “Structure of Memory”, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany

Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

Affiliation St. Elisabeth Hospital, Gütersloh, Germany

Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

* E-mail: [email protected]

Affiliations Institute for Neural Computation, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany, Mercator Research Group “Structure of Memory”, Ruhr University Bochum, Bochum, Germany

  • Jing Fang, 
  • Selver Demic, 

PLOS

  • Published: June 7, 2018
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406
  • Reader Comments

Fig 1

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with an impairment of episodic memory, but the mechanisms underlying this deficit remain unclear. Animal models of MDD find impaired adult neurogenesis (AN) in the dentate gyrus (DG), and AN in DG has been suggested to play a critical role in reducing the interference between overlapping memories through pattern separation. Here, we study the effect of reduced AN in MDD on the accuracy of episodic memory using computational modeling. We focus on how memory is affected when periods with a normal rate of AN (asymptomatic states) alternate with periods with a low rate (depressive episodes), which has never been studied before. Also, unlike previous models of adult neurogenesis, which consider memories as static patterns, we model episodic memory as sequences of neural activity patterns. In our model, AN adds additional random components to the memory patterns, which results in the decorrelation of similar patterns. Consistent with previous studies, higher rates of AN lead to higher memory accuracy in our model, which implies that memories stored in the depressive state are impaired. Intriguingly, our model makes the novel prediction that memories stored in an earlier asymptomatic state are also impaired by a later depressive episode. This retrograde effect exacerbates with increased duration of the depressive episode. Finally, pattern separation at the sensory processing stage does not improve, but rather worsens, the accuracy of episodic memory retrieval, suggesting an explanation for why AN is found in brain areas serving memory rather than sensory function. In conclusion, while cognitive retrieval biases might contribute to episodic memory deficits in MDD, our model suggests a mechanistic explanation that affects all episodic memories, regardless of emotional relevance.

Citation: Fang J, Demic S, Cheng S (2018) The reduction of adult neurogenesis in depression impairs the retrieval of new as well as remote episodic memory. PLoS ONE 13(6): e0198406. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406

Editor: Judith Homberg, Radboud University Medical Centre, NETHERLANDS

Received: October 10, 2017; Accepted: May 20, 2018; Published: June 7, 2018

Copyright: © 2018 Fang et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding: This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) through the SFB 874 project B2, grant 01GQ1506 and a grant from the Stiftung Mercator. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is the most common mood disorder, estimated to affect 20% of the population at some point of a person’s lifetime [ 1 – 3 ]. MDD is characterized by a constellation of behavioural, emotional and cognitive symptoms, especially in the domain of memory [ 4 ]. Numerous studies have reported a selective impairment of episodic memory during depressive episodes [ 5 – 8 ]. Some studies even find an almost linear relationship between scores on a depression rating scale and episodic memory performance [ 9 , 10 ]. Unlike episodic memory, however, semantic memory, the other type of declarative memory, is relatively intact in MDD patients [ 11 , 12 ].

The mechanisms underlying MDD are not understood. The neurogenic theory of depression suggests that impaired adult neurogenesis (AN) in the dentate gyrus (DG) triggers depression and that restoration of AN leads to recovery [ 13 ]. AN refers to the process that generates new neurons beyond development in adulthood. It occurs in only two regions of the mammalian brain, one of which is the DG. A number of experimental studies have observed a reduction of AN in animal models of MDD [ 14 – 18 ]. While there are no direct measurements of AN in brains of MDD patients, both post-mortem [ 19 ] and high-resolution MRI volumetric [ 20 , 21 ] studies consistently find smaller DG sizes in subjects who had suffered or were suffering from MDD. In addition, animal studies indicate that the rate of AN can be increased by antidepressant treatment [ 22 – 24 ] and ablating AN suppresses the antidepressant effect of the drug [ 14 , 25 ]. However, the clear picture painted by these studies is complicated by findings that even a complete reduction of AN [ 25 ] does not produce the behavioural symptoms of MDD, see [ 26 ] for a review. Nevertheless, even though the role of AN in the etiology of MDD remains uncertain, the evidence strongly suggests that there is a correlation between MDD and AN in DG.

The DG is a subregion of the hippocampus, which is heavily involved in the storage and retrieval of episodic memory [ 27 – 29 ]. Marr [ 30 ] suggested that memories are stored in an associative network that is implemented in the recurrent connections of hippocampal CA3. Computational studies suggest that memory patterns in CA3 have to be uncorrelated to avoid interference between memories. Since sensory inputs are highly correlated, the hippocampal network has to pre-proccess these input patterns to reduce the correlations before they can be stored in CA3 [ 31 ]. This process is called pattern separation and the DG, which receives inputs from the entorhinal cortex (EC) and sends direct projections to CA3, has been suggested to be especially suitable for this purpose [ 30 , 32 – 34 ]. There is mounting empirical support for the hypothesis that AN in DG plays a role in minimizing interference between overlapping memories. Animals with AN impairment show a deficit in spatial discrimination [ 35 – 37 ] and in learning overlapping odour pair discriminations [ 38 ]. On the other hand, increasing AN improves pattern separation [ 39 ]. An fMRI study in humans also shows that the presentation of objects that are very similar, but not identical, to previously learned objects increases BOLD activity in human DG/CA3 [ 40 ]. Linking MDD, AN, and pattern separation together, recent studies in humans found a negative correlation between depression scores and pattern separation performance [ 41 , 42 ]. Déry et al. also find that the memory deficit in depression is selective for a neurogenesis-dependent task, and does not occur in other hippocampus-dependent control tasks [ 41 ].

In contrast to the abundance of experimental and clinical studies on the link between MDD and cognitive deficits, there are few modelling studies on this topic. One example is the study by Becker et al., who proposed a functional cluster hypothesis in their theoretical model by which cells born at a particular time in the DG encode a context that binds together all memories formed in that context [ 43 ]. An AN deficit then causes deficits in contextual memory. By contrast, the vast majority of computational studies focus on the broader question of how AN contributes to normal learning and memory, see [ 44 ] for a review. AN is implemented either by replacing trained cells with new naïve cells or generate additional new cells. In simple feedforward architectures, neural replacement improves the encoding of new memories at the cost of losing previously stored memories [ 45 – 48 ]. By contrast, adding new neurons to the network can help avoid catastrophic interference [ 49 ] and can preserve old memories as well as store and retrieve new memories [ 50 ]. Aimone et al. emphasizes the role of newborn immature granule cells which are more broadly tuned to a wide range of inputs [ 51 ]. Their model suggests that immature neurons increase the similarity between contemporaneous events, but once they are mature, they separate events that occur in different time periods. Nonetheless, these computational studies do not account for the specific episodic memory deficits in MDD.

Finally, little is known about how dynamic changes in the rate of AN might affect episodic memories. The time course of MDD is highly dynamic [ 52 , 53 ] and involves transitions between depressive episodes, when the rate of AN is putatively low, and asymptomatic states, when the rate of AN is putatively higher. Although memory deficits in depressive patients have been reported in various episodic memory tasks, these studies generally examine the memories both formed and retrieved in the depressive state. The accuracy of memories formed during asymptomatic states and retrieved during depressive episodes, or vice versa, has not been studied using controlled experiments. Note that this cannot be achieved by asking depressive patients to recall auto-biographical memories stored in a previous asymptomatic state, since not all auto-biographical memory can be considered episodic memory [ 28 , 54 ].

In this study, we develop a computational model that accounts for episodic memory deficits in MDD by assuming that MDD leads to a reduction in DG AN, which in turn leads an impairment in pattern separation, which eventually impairs episodic memory retrieval. Unlike previous models of adult neurogenesis, which consider memories as static patterns, we model episodic memory as sequences of neural activity patterns. Also, we examine for the first time how episodic memories are affected by the dynamics of MDD. To simulate this dynamics, the model differentiates an asymptomatic state with a normal rate of AN and a depressive state with reduced rate of AN. We compare the retrieval of memories stored and retrieved in the same state as well as memories stored in one state and retrieved in another. We find that pattern separation indeed improves episodic memory retrieval as well as its robustness to the retrieval noise. Retrieval performance is significantly worse for memories stored and retrieved in the depressive state as compared to the asymptomatic state. Interestingly, our model predicts an retrograde effect of MDD on memories formed in an earlier asymptomatic state. This effect is a novel prediction of our model, which has not been previously reported by any study, experimental or computational.

Memory model

To study episodic memory storage and retrieval, we adopted a model that we proposed and studied in earlier work [ 55 ]. The model consists of three systems (the perceptual, semantic and episodic system), which are arranged hierarchically ( Fig 1A ). This model assumes that episodic memories are represented as sequences of activation patterns, which are stored in the hippocampus [ 28 , 29 , 56 ]. These activation patterns are the outputs of a semantic representation network in neocortex, which generates low dimensional semantic representations of high dimensional sensory input. In other word, episodic memory in the model is defined as sequences of semantic representations.

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

A. The relationship between systems involved episodic memory. B. Example of the input stimuli. Top: 300 × 300 black-and-white pixels; bottom: pattern scaled down to 30 × 30 greyscale pixels. C. Hierarchical network of slow feature analysis (SFA) as a model of the semantic system. The dots in each layer symbolize SFA nodes. The grey patches indicate the receptive field of each node, partially overlapping with the neighbouring nodes’ receptive fields. As an ensemble nodes in a given layer cover the full input space. Each node performs a number of processing steps as visualized on the right hand side. The activity in the top layer is taken as the output of the semantic system in our memory model. D. Example of the output of the semantic representation layer. The object in the input sequence i moves along the trajectory (yellow arrow) and rotates by 360 degrees (indicated by black arrows). Shown on the right are the four slowest features calculated by the SFA-network. The feature values at time t , y i , t (dashed line), form a semantic (more abstract) representation of the input. E. Sequence storage network (see main text in Methods for details).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g001

Input stimuli.

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

Test sequences were generated using a different statistics to ensure that our results are not selective to the specific input statistics used during training. In the test sequences, the object moved along a random walk trajectory, where the object can translate horizontally and vertically in each time step. The step sizes in the two directions were drawn independently from a normal distribution v ∼ N (5, 2.2). If a step would have taken the object beyond the boundary, the object was reflected on the boundary instead. The rotation of the object also followed a random walk, where the steps are drawn from δ φ ∼ N (0, (0.035 e ) 2 ).

Semantic representation network.

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

The semantic network consists of converging layers of SFA nodes. Information is first extracted locally and then integrated into more global and abstract features, see [ 55 ] for a more detailed description. In each SFA node, the same processing steps are performed ( Fig 1C , top right). The network was implemented using the MDP library [ 58 ]. It was trained sequentially from bottom to top on sequences of 10,000 images in each training session. Although SFA learns on sequences and the movement statistics determines which features are learned, SFA does not learn the movement statistics of the training sequences itself. In fact, the network learns to extract a semantic representation from a single input pattern, i.e., the extracted functions g ( i ) operate on single input patterns. This makes SFA fundamentally different from low-pass filtering.

Due to our particular choice of the object’s movement parameters in the training sequences (mainly the translation and rotation speeds), the four slowest features that emerged from the trained SFA network are related to the coordinates of the object’s center and its orientation [ 59 ]. To illustrate the SFA output, we used input sequences where the object moves along a trajectory and rotates by 360 degrees ( Fig 1D ). We refer to the vector y i , t of SFA features at a given time t in sequence i as the semantic, i.e., more abstract, representation of a single input image. After the semantic representation network had been trained, we used it to process sequences with different movement statistics. The temporal sequence of these semantic representation { y i ,1 , y i ,2 , y i ,3 , …}, describing the movement of the object in the input sequence i , is stored in the episodic memory system.

Sequence storage network.

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

Modeling the effect of adult neurogenesis in episodic memory storage

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

Top: Schematic of three stored sequences in the memory model, where the first elements in sequences 2 and i are similiar to each other. A: Without adult neurogenesis, the memory patterns are located in close proximity to each other in the memory space. B: In the asymptomatic state with a normal rate of adult neurogenesis, the augmentation with distinct pattern separation vectors distributes the sequences along an additional dimension in memory space. C: In the depressive state, the new sequence ( i ) is stored by re-using a pattern separation vector that had been assigned to a memory stored in a preceding asymptomatic state, based on the similarity of their first patterns. As a result, the two memory sequences, 2 and i , are more likely to interfere during retrieval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g002

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

Modeling memory storage and retrieval in different disease states in MDD

In this study, we limit ourselves to considering only the asymptomatic state (A) and the depressive state (D). Based on the experimental evidence discussed above, we assume that the rate of AN is normal in the asymptomatic state and zero in the depressive state. The latter assumption implies that no new pattern separation vectors are generated for new sequences in the depressed state and previously generated ones are re-used. Four cases can be distinguished in principle based on which of the two states a memory sequence was stored and retrieved in ( Fig 3 ). We use the notation “X|Y” to indicate that a memory was stored in state X and retrieved in state Y. The four possible cases are A|A, A|D, D|D, and D|A. We will only discuss the first three cases, because the D|A case can be decomposed into those memories for which A|A applies and those for which D|D applies. We return to this issue in the Discussion.

thumbnail

The rate of adult neurogenesis (AN) is normal in the asymptomatic state and reduced in the depressive state. The origin of the arrow indicates during which state the memory was stored, and the termination of the arrow indicates during which state the memory is retrieved. A|A: memories stored and retrieved in the asymptomatic state; A|D: memories stored in the asymptomatic state and retrieved in the depressive state; D|D: memories stored and retrieved in the depressive state.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g003

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

Pattern separation improves the robustness of memory retrieval

We study the effect of augmenting memory patterns y i , t with a pattern separation vector a i on pattern separation in our model. Since the Euclidean distance between patterns plays an important role in retrieval in our model, we quantified the dissimilarity of patterns using the Euclidean distance. We find that distance between augmented patterns D a are larger than the distance between the original target patterns D t ( Fig 4 ), indicating that pattern separation indeed occurs in our model. Furthermore, the effect of pattern separation is largest for highly similar patterns ( D t < 1) and for large variability in the pattern separation vector (large σ a ).

thumbnail

Left: The distance between pairs of augmented patterns ( D a ), i.e., containing the pattern separation vectors, against the distance between pairs of original patterns ( D t ). A curve above the diagonal means that the augmented vectors and more dissimilar than the original vectors, indicative of pattern separation. Right: Same data as left panel, but plotted to emphasize pattern separation (= D a − D t ).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g004

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

A: Example performance of single-pattern retrieval across different level of retrieval noise (raw data). B: Distribution of the distance between cued and retrieved patterns at different levels of pattern separation σ a (only for the data within the dashed rectangle in A) as indicated by different colors. The legend is given in panel C, the reference σ a = 0 is shown in dark blue. C: Average performance of single-pattern retrieval as a function of retrieval noise. D: Retrieval error for retrieval of sequences at different levels of σ a (100 stored sequences, σ n = 0.1, a : 2-D).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g005

Next, we analyzed how pattern separation affects the retrieval of memory sequences in a model that stored 100 sequences (random walk trajectory), each with an unique pattern separation vector. Consistent with the result for single pattern retrieval, introducing pattern separation into the memory network (0 ≤ σ a ≤ 1), increases the retrieval accuracy of memory sequences ( Fig 5D ). However, large amounts of pattern separation ( σ a > 1) do not yield further improvement of the retrieval performance, indicating that pattern separation cannot fully eliminate the retrieval error in our model. In our model, DG AN is modelled by the generation of pattern separation vectors, which is parametrized by σ a . Better memory performance for ( σ a > 0) therefore means that AN improves episodic memory.

Dynamics of memory retrieval in asymptomatic and depressive state

To test our hypothesis that a reduction of AN in DG induces pattern separation impairment, which in turn impairs episodic memory in depression, we study the retrieval quality of memories stored and retrieved in the asymptomatic and depressive states, respectively. Two hundred sequences are stored in each state. Specifically, we compared retrieval performance in the three cases: A|A, A|D and D|D for different levels of retrieval noise σ n and pattern separation σ a . At low levels of pattern separation σ a = 0.1, retrieval performance is comparable in the three cases ( Fig 6A ). Increasing the level of pattern separation (from left to right in Fig 6A ), while keeping the level of retrieval noise fixed, improves the retrieval performance in all three cases, but the degree of improvement differs. When memories are stored and retrieved in the asymptomatic state (A|A), memory performance is better than if memories were retrieved in the depressive phase (A|D), or stored and retrieved in the depressive phase (D|D). This finding indicates that depression impairs memory performance. The higher the level of retrieval noise is, the more pattern separation is required to yield an advantage of A|A, or conversely an negative impact of depression. For example, for σ n = 0.05, a difference is already apparent for σ a ≥ 0.1, while for σ n = 0.2, a difference is only apparent for σ a ≥ 0.5. If retrieval noise dominates, i.e., σ n ≥ 0.5, no amount of pattern separation yields a difference between A|A, A|D and D|D. We discuss these effects in more detail below. To rule out the probability that our results are specific to a particular input stimulus, we also studied the model with different objects (‘T’, ‘U’, ‘E’) as input stimuli and find very similar results (data not shown). Together, these results suggest that retrieval performance in our model depends on the mutual interaction between the retrieval noise and pattern separation and that a memory deficit in depression would not be expected in every case.

thumbnail

A: Retrieval performance for the three cases A|A, A|D, D|D (indicated by color) at different levels of pattern separation σ a (left to right) and retrieval noise σ n (top to bottom). B: The duration of depressive episode affects the retrieval performance of A|D and D|D. Duration is measured by the fraction of memories stored in the depressive episode k ( Eq 12 ). C: Increasing the number of stored sequences negatively impacts the retrieval performance in all cases, while the difference are preserved. D: Increasing the dimensionality of the pattern separation vector, up to a certain point, increases the difference between the A|A and the other cases. Values in B, C and D are calculated based on the 30 th element in the sequence ( σ a = 1, σ n = 0.1). For A,B,C: a :2-D; for A,C,D: k = 0.9; for A,B,D: 200 stored sequences in both asymptomatic and depressive state respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g006

Impact of depressive episode duration on retrieval performance.

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

The results in Fig 6A were obtained with k = 0.9. Across all values of k , we found that the retrieval performance in the case of A|A is the most accurate, while D|D is the worst. The difference between the two cases becomes more prominent for larger k ( Fig 6B ). We also find that for short duration of depression (small k ), the retrieval performance of A|D is as good as the performance of A|A and then converges to the same level as D|D as the duration of the depressive episode increases (larger k ). This indicates that even the remote memories formed in earlier asymptomatic state of the depressive patients are impaired as depression lingers.

The role of other model parameters.

We studied the influence of two other parameters that have a potentially important role in memory performance in our model. First, we studied the role of the memory load by storing larger numbers of sequences in the memory network. Retrieval performance for all three cases becomes worse for higher memory load. The difference, however, between depressive state and asymptomatic state is almost constant ( Fig 6C ). Second, we expected the dimensionality of the pattern separation vector to influence pattern separation, i.e., higher dimensionality leads to larger pattern separation effects. Indeed, our results show that the advantage of the A|A case is already apparent with only a one dimensional pattern separation vector ( Fig 6D ). The effect is stronger for larger numbers of dimensions. However, for this particular set of memory sequences, increasing the dimensionality beyond two has little effect on memory performance in each of the three cases.

Accounting for the pattern of retrieval errors.

Next, we explored how the difference in retrieval accuracy among the three cases arises. A retrieval error occurs when the retrieved pattern is different from the stored one, in other words, when retrieval jumps to an incorrect pattern. Intuitively, one might expect that the more frequently incorrect jumps occur, the larger the retrieval error is, but we found previously that the retrieval error is dominated by another process, namely the sequence divergence [ 55 ]. It refers to the tendency of two sequences that are close to each other at some point in time to diverge from each other over time. Since memory patterns are retrieved sequentially in our model, the movement along the sequence exacerbates the retrieval error, if the incorrect sequence diverges from the correct one. We therefore examined the sequence divergence as well as the probability of jumps to an incorrect pattern within the same sequences ( p w ) and between two sequences ( p b ) during retrieval. Sequence divergence is quantified by the increase in the distance between the subsequent elements of two sequences after two patterns in the respective sequences were the closest patterns to each other [ 55 ].

Three observations account for the differences in retrieval error seen in Fig 6 . First, increasing the retrieval noise leads to more faulty transitions both within and between sequences ( Fig 7A , from top to bottom), which accounts for the increase in the retrieval error with increasing retrieval noise. Second, with the same level of retrieval noise, increasing the level of pattern separation ( σ a ) reduces the rate of faulty transitions between sequences, but increases the faulty transition rate within sequences. This is expected since pattern separation in our model acts to make sequences more distinct from each other. As a result, incorrect patterns within the same sequence are more often the closest element to the retrieval cue for the next element. This effect is more apparent in the A|A case than in the other two cases due to the stronger effect of pattern separation. Since jumps between sequences lead to larger errors than jumps within sequences, the differences in p b between the three cases account for the differences in the respective retrieval errors ( Fig 6A ), except for the lack of a difference at high levels of retrieval noise ( σ n = 0.5).

thumbnail

A:left, probability of incorrect jumps between sequences ( p b ); right, probability of incorrect jumps within sequences ( p w ). B: Sequence divergence. For A, B: a :2-D, k = 0.2, 200 stored sequences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g007

The third observation fills this explanatory gap. Sequence divergence is maintained across different levels of pattern separation in the A|A case, while sequence divergence drops in the other two cases ( Fig 7B ). The latter effect is the result of reusing pattern separation vectors based on the similarity of the sequences in the A|D and D|D cases. Through this mechanism similar sequences become more clustered. Since pattern separation drives these clusters further apart, incorrect jumps go to similar sequences, thus reducing sequence divergence, when pattern separation is high. The lower sequence divergence offsets the higher jump probability p b in the A|D and D|D cases and therefore reduces the difference to the A|A case in the retrieval error, but only if the jump probability p b for the A|A case is not already close to zero. These conditions are satisfied for all levels of pattern separation, when σ n = 0.5, which explains why the A|A case performs no better in this noise regime.

Pattern separation at input stage

After showing that pattern separation improves episodic retrieval in our model, we asked whether pattern separation has to occur in the memory system or whether it could instead occur in the sensory system before the patterns are processed by the memory system. To study this question, we randomly flipped different numbers of pixels of each input image in the testing data ( Fig 8A ). No noise was added to the memory representations during storage. The way we added noise to the input patterns followed the same strategy that we used for pattern separation in the previous simulations. That is, the same pixels are flipped for all patterns within the same sequences, whereas different sets of pixels are flipped for the patterns in different sequences. Therefore, similar input patterns in different sequences should be separated. We tested whether this kind of pattern separation alleviates the interference between memories and facilitates the accuracy of memory retrieval.

thumbnail

A: Example of the manipulated input patterns. Top: same pattern as in Fig 1B , but with 5% pixels flipped (300 × 300 pixels); Bottom: the scaled version. B: Retrieval error as a function of the fraction of randomly flipped pixels in the input image ( σ n = 0.2, 200 stored sequences). Dashed curve: retrieval performance of the model with neurogenesis ( σ a = 1) for comparison. C: With the same amount of noise in the input (1% flipped pixels), retrieval error increases monotonically with increasing retrieval noise σ n . D: The difference between the retrieval error for original patterns and that for noisy input pattern gradually increases with input noise ( σ n = 0.2). Values in C and D are drawn from the 30 th element in the sequence.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.g008

Unlike what one might expect, we found that the retrieval performance is impaired by pattern separation in the sensory inputs ( Fig 8B and 8D ). Similar to previous results, retrieval noise impairs the retrieval performance ( Fig 8C ). These results indicate that pattern separation of sensory inputs does not necessarily mean the semantic representation patterns are separated as well, since the SFA network is performing a nonlinear operation. These results are consistent with our previous study [ 55 ], where we found that the episodic retrieval is more accurate when the semantic network is trained on the same image statistics that generates the inputs to be stored in memory, as compared to when the image statistics differ. Episodic retrieval is impaired when we add noise to the input, because by doing so we changed the input statistics after the semantic network had been trained. We therefore conclude that pattern separation at the sensory stage is not effective and therefore has to occur in the memory system.

We have developed a computational model to study episodic memory deficits in MDD. We assumed that MDD is associated with a reduction of AN in the DG, and that this reduction in AN impairs pattern separation. We hypothesized that the impairment of pattern separation in turn reduces the accuracy of episodic memory retrieval. In our model, episodic memories are encoded based on a semantic representation of the sensory inputs [ 55 ]. We investigated episodic memory deficit in MDD with an intact semantic system, which is consistent with observations that semantic memory is not affected in MDD [ 11 , 12 ]. Our model of episodic memory is built around the idea that episodic memories are best represented by sequences of neural activity patterns [ 28 , 29 , 56 , 63 ]. This aspect distinguishes our model from other models of neurogenesis, which only consider the storage and retrieval of static patterns.

Correspondence to neuronal mechanisms underlying pattern separation

Even though our model does not reflect the anatomy and physiology of the hippocampus, it nevertheless describes hippocampal function at an abstract level and the functions of our abstract model can be roughly mapped onto the hippocamal circuit. The hippocampus has been found to be essential for sequence memory [ 64 , 65 ] and we previously hypothesized that the hippocampal circuit is optimized for storing sequences of neural activity patterns [ 28 ]. During episodic memory storage, input patterns are mapped onto pre-existing intrinsic sequences of neural activity in CA3. In CA3, sequences are thought to be generated by the dynamics of its recurrent network, e.g. [ 66 ]. A given state of the network drives the next state through the recurrent synapses. In our model, we approximate this process in our sequence retrieval network, where the sequence elements are linked by associating each element with a retrieval key for the next element. Thus, the sequence elements y i , t correspond to activity patterns in CA3.

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

In our model, the pattern separation vector is identical for all patterns within the same sequence, while different pattern separation vectors are generated for different sequences. This assumption is consistent with the temporal tagging hypothesis [ 51 ]. It was proposed that memories formed at distinct times would be represented by different groups of neurons in DG since newborn cells continue to be integrated into the network. As a result, memories formed close in time would be associated by the same group of immature DG granule cells (pattern integration), while memories formed at times far apart would be represented by distinct sets of DG neurons. Similarly, the functional cluster hypothesis proposes that the same contexts are represented by DG cells that were born simultaneously [ 43 ]. We therefore conclude that our abstract model is firmly rooted in the neuronal mechanisms underlying pattern separation in the hippocampal formation.

Rate of adult neurogenesis and memory persistence

Empirical evidence suggests that increases in the rate of AN improves the performance on a variety of memory tasks [ 24 , 41 ]. Here, we find that increasing AN up to a certain level improves memory performance (Figs 5 and 6 ). Moreover, since retrieval performance in our model depends on the interaction between the retrieval noise and pattern separation, memory deficits would not be expected in every case of MDD. Indeed, some studies failed to find episodic memory deficits in depressed individuals [ 72 , 73 ]. We hypothesize that the retrieval error in our model is determined by task demands, the subject’s level of engagement, and neural processing. Pattern separation would be affected by the rate of DG AN, and the severity, and perhaps the duration, of the depressive phase. To test these predictions, future experimental studies could systematically vary the rate of AN and retrieval noise, and measure the affect of these manipulations on retrieval performance.

What is currently missing from our model is a detrimental effect of AN on memory. Experimental [ 47 , 74 ] and computational [ 47 , 75 ] studies have found that a high rate of AN leads to faster forgetting. Apparently, integrating new neurons into the hippocampal circuit affects memories that are already stored, because new cells and new connections compete with existing ones. In other words, there is trade-off between plasticity and stability.

Episodic memory deficits in MDD

Our model predicts that MDD has an retrograde effect on episodic memory retrieval ( Fig 6B ). That is, memories retrieved in a depressive state are less accurate, even if they had been stored in a preceding asymptomatic state (A|D), as compared to memories that were stored and retrieved in an asymptomatic state (A|A). Studies of auto-biographical memories, which we discuss below, appear to support a retrograde effect of MDD on previously formed memories. However, to the best of our knowledge, a retrograde effect has yet to be demonstrated under laboratory-controlled conditions. Moreover, we find that memory deficits depend on the duration of the depressive episode. The longer the depressive episode lasts, the more severe the memory performance becomes.

In addition to the three case discussed in our study (A|A, A|D, D|D), there is another possible scenario. A memory can be stored in the depressive state and retrieved in the asymptomatic state (D|A). While this case is distinct from the other three, we did not include it in our study because it can be viewed as a composite of two other cases. Memories stored in the depressive state are not assigned a distinct pattern separation vector, while memories stored in the subsequent asymptomatic state are. New memories would therefore rarely interfere with previously stored memories and the D|A case can be decomposed into those memories that fall under the A|A case (new memories) and those under the D|D case (old memories). Our model, therefore, predicts that the memory deficit is not rescued when the depressive state ends. In other words, the damage caused in the depressive state by interference in the memory system cannot be undone. By contrast, the A|D case cannot be decomposed, because the pattern separation vector generated during the asymptomatic phase are re-used during the depressive phase, which leads to retrograde interference.

We found that the type of error committed during memory retrieval differs during MDD ( Fig 7 ). According to our model during MDD patients might more frequently confuse memories formed at different timepoints than healthy controls. Somewhat paradoxically, it also predicts that controls incorrectly report events that occurred close in time more frequently than patients do. This novel prediction awaits testing in experimental studies.

Shifting from episodic to semantic memory in MDD

Apart from impairments in episodic memory, patients suffering from MDD also show over-general memories [ 12 , 76 – 78 ]. When subjects are asked to recall a particular event from their personal history related to a given cue, patients, more often than controls, retrieve rather general information that summarizes a category of events [ 12 , 77 ]. This is called the over-general memory effect. For instance, when cued with “enjoy” to recall an event, patients tend to produce generic answers, e.g., “I enjoy a good party”, whereas controls produce specific memories such as “I enjoyed Jane’s party last Saturday”. To account for this effect, Williams et al. [ 77 ] adopted the Conway and Pleydell-Pearce model [ 79 ], which suggests that autobiographical memories are arranged in a hierarchical structure with the general categories at the top, specific categories in the middle and specific event memories at the bottom. Autobiographical memories are retrieved by traversing this memory structure from top-to-bottom. Williams et al. suggest that MDD patients block the access to specific event memories in order to avoid retrieving painful memories and therefore end the retrieval process at an abstract level.

By contrast, we propose that the same episodic memory deficit that we studied here might be sufficient to account for over-general memories, too. Episodic memories together with personal semantic information forms autobiographical memory. Episodic memories are about specific events, whereas semantic memories refer to general facts. Therefore, over-general memory can be seen as a shift from the retrieval of episodic memories to the retrieval of semantic memories. If episodic memory retrieval is impaired during MDD, retrieval of autobiographical memories is more likely to result in a semantic memory which is mostly preserved during MDD. This shift from a reliance on episoidic memory to reliance on semantic memory appears as a shift from specific to over-general memories. This account is consistent with a previous suggestion that over-general memory could result from reduced episodic recall, increased semantic recall or the combination of both [ 12 ].

In conclusion, the model we present here might be able to account for both over-general memories and episodic memory deficits in MDD.

Supporting information

S1 file. python code..

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198406.s001

Acknowledgments

We thank Sonakchhi Shrestha for support in performing the computer simulations. This work was supported by grants from the Stiftung Mercator, from the German Research Foundation (DFG) through the SFB 874, project B2, and from the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF), grant 01GQ1506.

  • View Article
  • PubMed/NCBI
  • Google Scholar
  • 5. Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G. Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;.
  • 27. Amaral D, Lavenex P. Hippocampal Neuroanatomy. In: The Hippocampus Book. Oxford University Press; 2006. p. 37–114.
  • 54. Cheng S. Consolidation of Episodic Memory: An Epiphenomenon of Semantic Learning. In: Axmacher N, Rasch B, editors. Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory Consolidation. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing; 2017. p. 57–72.
  • 76. Conway M, Williams H. Autobiographical memory. In: Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference. Oxford:Elsevier Ltd; 2008. p. 893–909.

The Future of Depression Treatment: The Neurogenesis Theory

Some studies show that antidepressants act by triggering neurogenesis. Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:GFPneuron.png

Some studies show that antidepressants act by triggering neurogenesis. Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:GFPneuron.png

Nearly half of all clinically depressed patients fail to respond to available antidepressant medications (1).  Though antidepressants are effective for some depressed patients, this selective efficacy is still not fully understood. Professor Poul Videbech, a specialist at the Centre for Psychiatric Research at Aarhus University Hospital, has dedicated himself to researching the effects of depression to better understand the mechanism of antidepressants. (2)

In one project, Videbech scanned the brains of depressed patients to observe structural effects of the disorder.  Videbech concluded, “My review shows that a depression leaves its mark on the brain as it results in a ten percent reduction of the hippocampus… In some cases this reduction continues when the depression itself is over” (2).  Videbech believes that nerve reduction supports the neurogenesis theory of depression which posits that depression results in a cessation of neuron birth in the brain (3).  Support for this theory lies in the fact that, with extended use, antidepressants trigger neurogenesis by initiating the birth of new nerve cells.  Studies at the Centre for Psychiatric Research, where patients suffering from depression were followed for ten years using brain scans, demonstrate that shrinking of the hippocampus is reversible when depressed patients are treated.

People not suffering from depression have a balance in degradation and regeneration processes in the brain.  The degradation process refers to the breaking down of nerve cells, while regeneration refers to the formation of nerve cells (1).  Depressed patients show greater activity in the degradation system, which explains Videbech’s findings that brain structures are reduced in patients with depression.  The location of reduction cited by Videbech is the hippocampus, the structure of the brain responsible for the storage and retrieval of memories.  Hippocampal reduction explains the common symptom of memory problems in patients with depression.  With antidepressant use, and hence a return of neurogenesis, memory problems and depressive symptoms are reduced.  Meaning, boosting neurogenesis results in a returned balance between the degradation and regeneration processes. (2)

The most common form of antidepressants, serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), were believed to have their effect by boosting levels of serotonin in the brain.  However, scientists have proven that SSRIs take about a month to improve mood in depressed patients.  This delay in treatment suggests that another process, one influenced by serotonin, is involved.  The neurogenesis theory of depression explains that the delay in mood improvement is a result of the minimal effect serotonin has on neurogenesis.  Researchers have turned their focus to chemicals in the brain that promote neurogenesis and suggest that new treatments targeting said chemicals could be a more logical and effective treatment for depression. (1)

The neurogenesis theory has been supported with animal studies.  In one trial, researchers induced a depression-like condition in mice so they develop a depressive behavioral pattern.  The mice were then given antidepressants and normal behavior returned.  Then, when the mice were subjected to radiation treatment, a process known to terminate the formation of new nerve cells, the antidepressants stopped working and the mice returned to their depressed behaviors (1).  Videbech cites this study as proof that antidepressants are only effective because of their influence on neurogenesis and when that influence is eliminated, antidepressants no longer improve depressive symptoms (2).

A recent article in Nature Medicine cites a promising new line of research in the ceramide system.  In one study, after mice took Prozac-like antidepressants the levels of ceramide, a fat molecule in the brain, significantly decreased.  In the brain, ceramide blocks brain cell growth.  Meaning, Prozac affected ceramide levels which in turn increased neurogenesis.  Scientists believe that further research on molecules like ceramide will continue, and eventually result in, more direct and effective antidepressant treatments. (4)

References:

1. A. Maxmen, Psychol. Today. 64, 39-40 (2013).

2. S. Hildebrandt, Depression Can Damage the Brain (2011). Available at http://sciencenordic.com/depression-can-damage-brain (19 December 2013).

3. I. Dell, Depression: Neurogenesis and Depression (2010). Available at http://sites.lafayette.edu/neur401-sp10/applications-in-health-and-medicine/depression/ (19 December 2013).

4. E. Gulbins et al, Nat. Med. 19, 934-938 (2013).

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

  • Bovine Brain Damage: New Findings in the Field of Wildlife Neuroscience
  • The Heredity of Mental Disorders
  • Levels of Empathy in Apes and Humans
  • Stochastic Volatility Models and its Effect on the Asset Market
  • Corporate Psychopathy: Does Empathy Cripple Leaders?
  • Dementia Villages – Experimenting with Universal Design Treatment

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • My Account Login
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Correspondence
  • Open access
  • Published: 29 August 2022

Hippocampal neuroplasticity, major depression and, not to forget: ECT

  • Alexander Sartorius   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1243-3693 1 ,
  • Sebastian Karl   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-5406-7137 1 &
  • David Zilles-Wegner   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-7531-9732 2  

Molecular Psychiatry volume  29 ,  pages 1–2 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

2577 Accesses

4 Citations

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Diagnostic markers

To the Editor:

With this letter we would like to refer to the review on the topic of neuroplasticity, hippocampus and depression, in which, however, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) was overlooked [ 1 ]. In our opinion, no other specific form of psychiatric therapy plays a more important role for the neuroplasticity hypothesis of depression than ECT, which we would like to highlight by the following.

The neurotrophin/neuroplasticity hypothesis has historically evolved from the catecholamine hypothesis [ 2 ], which posits that that depletion of monoamines such as serotonin or norepinephrine can trigger depression. Normalization of the concentration of monoamines in the synaptic cleft (e.g., by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors) does not immediately lead to remission of depressive symptoms, which suggests delayed changes on the level of gene activation [ 3 ]. This finding led to the suggestion – almost 20 years ago – that development of new medications might focus more on downstream changes [ 4 ], where ECT was already prominently featured. Tartt et al. mention that the delayed action of selective serotonin and serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors increased the need for rapid acting antidepressants – ECT is exactly that, and likely because it directly induces downstream changes [ 5 , 6 ].

Findings from animal models

Since the authors describe magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) in the context of measuring GABA and glutamate concentrations [ 1 ], it seems noteworthy that in an animal model of depression, electroconvulsive shock (ECS, the analogue to ECT) led to a normalization of altered glutamate/GABA ratios within the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus [ 7 ].

ECS also leads to a dose-dependent increase of hippocampal dendritic arborization and dose-dependent cell proliferation in the subgranular region [ 8 , 9 , 10 ]. Further, ECS series and “maintenance” ECS induced a significant increase in newborn neurons in mice hippocampi, suggesting a cellular mechanism for the beneficial effect of ECT [ 10 , 11 ]. This finding was replicated [ 12 ] and extended for synaptogenesis indicating that neuronal survival is key to the efficacy of ECS [ 13 ]. ECS elevates hippocampal cell proliferation, while repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) does not [ 14 ].

For brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), ECS induces a tissue concentration increase in hippocampus and PFC while concentration of BDNF in peripheral serum takes longer (days) to come to a new equilibrium [ 15 ].

Patient findings

BDNF is lower in depressed patients’ serum and rises with ECT, both of which is supported by meta-analytical findings [ 16 , 17 ]. Additionally, like in the animal models, there is evidence that peripheral BDNF concentrations reach a new equilibrium with some delay after ECT [ 18 ]. Consequently, researchers looked for ECT induced hippocampal grey matter volume increases, which were first described by a Swedish group [ 19 ] and have since been replicated in large multisite samples [ 20 , 21 ]. While a mega-analysis did not find a positive association of hippocampal volume change and clinical outcome, a more recent smaller study did find larger hippocampal volume increases in ECT remitters vs. non-remitters [ 22 ].

Initial genetic findings corroborate an influence of ECT on e.g. DNA methylation: RAP-GEF2, a protein-encoding gene suggested to be involved in signal transmission and in BDNF receptor pathway signaling in depression is associated with ECT as well as FKBP5, a gene that is involved in stress hormone regulation [ 23 ].

MRS-studies in depressed patients treated with ECT also showed a normalization of glutamate levels in the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex which were associated with both ECT and symptom improvement [ 24 , 25 ].

Another relevant aspect mentioned in the introduction of Tartt et al. [ 1 ] concerns the assumed relationship between immunologic and neurotrophic processes in the hippocampus. Regarding ECT, decreased systemic levels of interleukins and cortisol after an ECT series have been described in a systematic review [ 26 ] and increased immune activation measured in the cerebrospinal fluid at baseline has been shown to predict better seizure quality [ 27 ] and better treatment response to ECT [ 28 ]. Given the inverse relationship between cortisol exposition and hippocampal volumes in both animal and human studies [ 29 ], the reduction of inflammatory processes by ECT may also contribute to increased hippocampal volumes after ECT [ 30 ] in addition to more direct neurotrophic effects. There is some evidence from ECT research that inflammatory activity can influence the relationship between BDNF and ECT treatment outcomes [ 31 ]. However, it may be that neuroplastic effects of ECT are necessary but not sufficient for a response. Other hypotheses include that ECT-induced seizures elicit a variety of processes in the brain, some of which having antidepressant effects, others having anticatatonic effects, and yet others leading to a grey matter increase [ 32 , 33 ].

To conclude, ECT research has had important influence on the development of the hippocampal neurotrophin/neuroplasticity hypothesis (and other hypotheses) of depression [ 33 ], which should not be overlooked. With the inclusion of severely ill patients and a large antidepressant effect size, ECT studies in particular offer optimal conditions to make contributions to the elucidation of the etiopathogenesis of depression.

Tartt AN, Mariani MB, Hen R, Mann JJ, Boldrini M. Dysregulation of adult hippocampal neuroplasticity in major depression: pathogenesis and therapeutic implications. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27:2689–99.

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Schildkraut JJ. The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: a review of supporting evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 1965;122:509–22.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Duman RS, Heninger GR, Nestler EJ. A molecular and cellular theory of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:597–606.

Henn FA, Vollmayr B, Sartorius A. Mechanisms of depression: the role of neurogenesis. Drug Discov Today: Dis Mechanisms. 2004;1:407–11.

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Husain MM, Rush AJ, Fink M, Knapp R, Petrides G, Rummans T, et al. Speed of response and remission in major depressive disorder with acute electroconvulsive therapy (ECT): a Consortium for Research in ECT (CORE) report. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65:485–91.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Vanicek T, Kranz GS, Vyssoki B, Komorowski A, Fugger G, Hoflich A, et al. Repetitive enhancement of serum BDNF subsequent to continuation ECT. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2019;140:426–34.

Sartorius A, Mahlstedt MM, Vollmayr B, Henn FA, Ende G. Elevated spectroscopic glutamate/gamma-amino butyric acid in rats bred for learned helplessness. Neuroreport. 2007;18:1469–73.

Smitha JS, Roopa R, Sagar BK, Kutty BM, Andrade C. Images in electroconvulsive therapy: ECS dose-dependently increases cell proliferation in the subgranular region of the rat hippocampus. J ECT. 2014;30:193–4.

Smitha JS, Roopa R, Khaleel N, Kutty BM, Andrade C. Images in electroconvulsive therapy: electroconvulsive shocks dose-dependently increase dendritic arborization in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus. J ECT. 2014;30:191–2.

Madsen TM, Treschow A, Bengzon J, Bolwig TG, Lindvall O, Tingstrom A. Increased neurogenesis in a model of electroconvulsive therapy. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:1043–9.

Weber T, Baier V, Lentz K, Herrmann E, Krumm B, Sartorius A, et al. Genetic fate mapping of type-1 stem cell-dependent increase in newborn hippocampal neurons after electroconvulsive seizures. Hippocampus. 2013;23:1321–30.

Olesen MV, Wortwein G, Folke J, Pakkenberg B. Electroconvulsive stimulation results in long-term survival of newly generated hippocampal neurons in rats. Hippocampus. 2017;27:52–60.

Jonckheere J, Deloulme JC, Dall’Igna G, Chauliac N, Pelluet A, Nguon AS, et al. Short- and long-term efficacy of electroconvulsive stimulation in animal models of depression: The essential role of neuronal survival. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:1336–47.

Zhang TR, Guilherme E, Kesici A, Ash AM, Vila-Rodriguez F, Snyder JS. Electroconvulsive shock, but not transcranial magnetic stimulation, transiently elevates cell proliferation in the adult mouse Hippocampus. Cells. 2021;10.

Sartorius A, Hellweg R, Litzke J, Vogt M, Dormann C, Vollmayr B, et al. Correlations and discrepancies between serum and brain tissue levels of neurotrophins after electroconvulsive treatment in rats. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2009;42:270–6.

Molendijk ML, Spinhoven P, Polak M, Bus BA, Penninx BW, Elzinga BM. Serum BDNF concentrations as peripheral manifestations of depression: evidence from a systematic review and meta-analyses on 179 associations ( N  = 9484). Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:791–800.

Rocha RB, Dondossola ER, Grande AJ, Colonetti T, Ceretta LB, Passos IC, et al. Increased BDNF levels after electroconvulsive therapy in patients with major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis study. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;83:47–53.

Bumb JM, Aksay SS, Janke C, Kranaster L, Geisel O, Gass P, et al. Focus on ECT seizure quality: serum BDNF as a peripheral biomarker in depressed patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2015;265:227–32.

Nordanskog P, Dahlstrand U, Larsson MR, Larsson EM, Knutsson L, Johanson A. Increase in hippocampal volume after electroconvulsive therapy in patients with depression: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. J ECT. 2010;26:62–7.

Sartorius A, Demirakca T, Bohringer A, Clemm von Hohenberg C, Aksay SS, Bumb JM, et al. Electroconvulsive therapy induced gray matter increase is not necessarily correlated with clinical data in depressed patients. Brain Stimul. 2019;12:335–43.

Oltedal L, Narr KL, Abbott C, Anand A, Argyelan M, Bartsch H, et al. Volume of the human Hippocampus and clinical response following Electroconvulsive Therapy. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84:574–81.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Takamiya A, Plitman E, Chung JK, Chakravarty M, Graff-Guerrero A, Mimura M, et al. Acute and long-term effects of electroconvulsive therapy on human dentate gyrus. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:1805–11.

Sirignano L, Frank J, Kranaster L, Witt SH, Streit F, Zillich L, et al. Methylome-wide change associated with response to electroconvulsive therapy in depressed patients. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11:347.

Zhang J, Narr KL, Woods RP, Phillips OR, Alger JR, Espinoza RT. Glutamate normalization with ECT treatment response in major depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:268–70.

Njau S, Joshi SH, Espinoza R, Leaver AM, Vasavada M, Marquina A, et al. Neurochemical correlates of rapid treatment response to electroconvulsive therapy in patients with major depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2017;42:6–16.

Yrondi A, Sporer M, Peran P, Schmitt L, Arbus C, Sauvaget A. Electroconvulsive therapy, depression, the immune system and inflammation: A systematic review. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:29–51.

Kranaster L, Hoyer C, Mindt S, Neumaier M, Muller N, Zill P, et al. The novel seizure quality index for the antidepressant outcome prediction in electroconvulsive therapy: association with biomarkers in the cerebrospinal fluid. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020;270:911–9.

Kranaster L, Hoyer C, Aksay SS, Bumb JM, Muller N, Zill P, et al. Antidepressant efficacy of electroconvulsive therapy is associated with a reduction of the innate cellular immune activity in the cerebrospinal fluid in patients with depression. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2018;19:379–89.

Nguyen DM, Yassa MA, Tustison NJ, Roberts JM, Kulikova A, Nakamura A, et al. The relationship between cumulative exogenous corticosteroid exposure and volumes of Hippocampal subfields and surrounding structures. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2019;39:653–7.

Belge JB, van Diermen L, Sabbe B, Parizel P, Morrens M, Coppens V, et al. Inflammation, Hippocampal volume, and therapeutic outcome following Electroconvulsive Therapy in depressive patients: A Pilot Study. Neuropsychobiology. 2020;79:222–32.

Loef D, Vansteelandt K, Oudega ML, van Eijndhoven P, Carlier A, van Exel E, et al. The ratio and interaction between neurotrophin and immune signaling during electroconvulsive therapy in late-life depression. Brain Behav Immun Health. 2021;18:100389.

Leaver AM, Espinoza R, Wade B, Narr KL. Parsing the network mechanisms of Electroconvulsive Therapy. Biol Psychiatry. 2022;92:193–203.

Sartorius A. Is seizure termination a key? Brain Stimul. 2021;14:1089–90.

Download references

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, J5, 68159, Mannheim, Germany

Alexander Sartorius & Sebastian Karl

Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Göttingen, von-Siebold-Str. 5, 37075, Göttingen, Germany

David Zilles-Wegner

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

All authors contributed equally to this letter.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastian Karl .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Sartorius, A., Karl, S. & Zilles-Wegner, D. Hippocampal neuroplasticity, major depression and, not to forget: ECT. Mol Psychiatry 29 , 1–2 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01746-w

Download citation

Received : 10 June 2022

Revised : 01 August 2022

Accepted : 11 August 2022

Published : 29 August 2022

Issue Date : January 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01746-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Electroconvulsive therapy—a shocking inducer of neuroplasticity.

  • Alexandria N. Tartt
  • Madeline Mariani
  • Maura Boldrini

Molecular Psychiatry (2023)

Wirkt EKT über eine Verbesserung dysfunktionaler Denkmuster?

  • Alexander Sartorius

InFo Neurologie + Psychiatrie (2023)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

IMAGES

  1. IJMS

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  2. Neurogenesis hypothesis of depression and antidepressant treatment.

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  3. The Serotonin Hypothesis and Neurogenesis

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  4. Table 1 from Understanding the pathophysiology of depression: From monoamines to the

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  5. Figure 1 from Orchestrating transcriptional control of adult neurogenesis.

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

  6. Antidepressant Agents

    neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

VIDEO

  1. Neurogenesis is the process by which

  2. Neurogenesis (Regrowing)

  3. Understanding the brain to treat depression

  4. Biological Theories of Major Depressive Disorder

  5. Depression and an Anti-Inflammatory Diet

  6. BPC-157 & THE BRAIN: Effects on Mood

COMMENTS

  1. Depression, Antidepressants, and Neurogenesis: A Critical Reappraisal

    The neurogenesis hypothesis of depression posits (1) that neurogenesis in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus is regulated negatively by stressful experiences and positively by treatment with antidepressant drugs and (2) that alterations in the rate of neurogenesis play a fundamental role in the pathology and treatment of major depression ...

  2. Adult Neurogenesis and Mental Illness

    Depression. The past decade and a half of research on the role of adult neurogenesis in mental disorders has focused mainly on depression. The neurogenesis hypothesis of depression (Duman et al ...

  3. Neurogenesis hypothesis of depression

    Adult neurogenesis is the process by which functional, mature neurons are produced from neural stem cells (NSCs) in the adult brain. In most mammals, including humans, it only occurs in the subgranular zone of the hippocampus, and in the olfactory bulb. [1] The neurogenesis hypothesis of depression proposes that major depressive disorder is ...

  4. Major depressive disorder: hypothesis, mechanism, prevention and

    Numerous investigations have demonstrated that 5-HT is intimately related to the pathophysiological process of major depression. The 5-HT hypothesis primarily ... promote neurogenesis and ...

  5. Depression, Antidepressants, and Neurogenesis: A Critical ...

    The neurogenesis hypothesis of depression posits (1) that neurogenesis in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus is regulated negatively by stressful experiences and positively by treatment ...

  6. The Current State of The Neurogenic Theory of Depression and Anxiety

    Abstract. Newborn neurons are continuously added to the adult hippocampus. Early studies found that adult neurogenesis is impaired in models of depression and anxiety and accelerated by antidepressant treatment. This led to the theory that depression results from impaired adult neurogenesis and restoration of adult neurogenesis leads to recovery.

  7. Understanding the pathophysiology of depression: From monoamines to the

    Understanding depression pathophysiology is challenging because varying depression symptomatology cannot be explained by single hypothesis. • Pathophysiologic mechanisms include: monoamine hypothesis, genetic, environmental, immunologic, endocrine factors and neurogenesis.

  8. Understanding the pathophysiology of depression: From ...

    A number of factors (biogenic amine deficiency, genetic, environmental, immunologic, endocrine factors and neurogenesis) have been identified as mechanisms which provide unitary explanations for the pathophysiology of depression. Rather than a unitary construct, the combination and linkage of these …

  9. Cell cycle regulation, neurogenesis, and depression

    The neurogenesis hypothesis of depression is attractive but highly controversial, as recently reviewed (7-9). Neurodegenerative processes in depression are supported by observations of decreased gray matter volume of hippocampus and frontal and temporal cortices (structural imaging) ( 10 , 11 ) and neuronal and glial pathology in depressed ...

  10. Neuroplasticity in cognitive and psychological mechanisms of depression

    These findings reveal alterations at the levels of intracellular signaling, gene expression, neurotrophic factors, neurogenesis, neuroinflammation, excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission, and synaptic number and function, and have been described in several brain regions implicated in depression 13-22. The signaling pathways and types of ...

  11. Neuromodulation and hippocampal neurogenesis in depression: A scoping

    The 'neurogenesis hypothesis of depression' emphasizes the importance of upregulated hippocampal neurogenesis for the efficacy of antidepressant treatment. Neuromodulation is a promising therapeutic method that stimulates neural circuitries to treat neuropsychiatric illnesses. We conducted a scoping review on the neurogenic and ...

  12. Neurogenesis and Neuroplasticity in Major Depression: Its ...

    The hypothesis of a close relationship among depression, antidepressant treatment, and neurogenesis is reinforced by other experiments showing that the rate of neurogenesis, decreased by glucocorticoid injections (rates equivalent to chronic stress), is normalized as a result of a disturbance in hippocampal neurogenesis would only represent one ...

  13. Dysregulation of adult hippocampal neuroplasticity in major depression

    The neurogenesis hypothesis of affective and anxiety disorders: are we mistaking the scaffolding for the building? Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:21-34. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

  14. Neuromodulation and hippocampal neurogenesis in depression: A ...

    The 'neurogenesis hypothesis of depression' emphasizes the importance of upregulated hippocampal neurogenesis for the efficacy of antidepressant treatment. Neuromodulation is a promising therapeutic method that stimulates neural circuitries to treat neuropsychiatric illnesses. We conducted a scoping review on the neurogenic and antidepressant ...

  15. The current state of the neurogenic theory of depression and anxiety

    Abstract. Newborn neurons are continuously added to the adult hippocampus. Early studies found that adult neurogenesis is impaired in models of depression and anxiety and accelerated by antidepressant treatment. This led to the theory that depression results from impaired adult neurogenesis and restoration of adult neurogenesis leads to recovery.

  16. Depression and Hippocampal Neurogenesis: A Road to Remission?

    Neurogenesis is a process, not a time point , a message emphasized in the road sign (inset) that condenses the journey from aNSC to GCL neuron. (D and E) The two branches of the neurogenic hypothesis of depression. (D) The first branch proposes ablation of adult neurogenesis does not greatly influence mood under normal, nonstressful conditions.

  17. The Neural Plasticity Theory of Depression: Assessing the Roles of

    Adult hippocampal neurogenesis has been hypothesized to play a potentially important role on the pathology and successful treatment of depression. A neurogenic hypothesis of depression has been postulated, which suggests that reduced adult hippocampal neurogenesis may underlie the pathoetiology of depression, while antidepressant efficacy ...

  18. Neurogenesis and depression: etiology or epiphenomenon?

    The concept that decreased neurogenesis might be the cause of depression is supported by the effects of stress on neurogenesis and the demonstration that neurogenesis seems to be necessary for antidepressant action. Data from the animal models tested to date show that decreasing the rate of neurogenesis does not lead to depressive behavior. Furthermore, evidence shows that an effective ...

  19. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression

    Statement of hypothesis. As mentioned above, in a variety of species, stress is one of the important controlling influences on neurogenesis. Stress is also believed to be the most significant ...

  20. The reduction of adult neurogenesis in depression impairs the ...

    The mechanisms underlying MDD are not understood. The neurogenic theory of depression suggests that impaired adult neurogenesis (AN) in the dentate gyrus (DG) triggers depression and that restoration of AN leads to recovery . AN refers to the process that generates new neurons beyond development in adulthood.

  21. The Future of Depression Treatment: The Neurogenesis Theory

    The neurogenesis theory of depression explains that the delay in mood improvement is a result of the minimal effect serotonin has on neurogenesis. Researchers have turned their focus to chemicals in the brain that promote neurogenesis and suggest that new treatments targeting said chemicals could be a more logical and effective treatment for ...

  22. Depression and Hippocampal Neurogenesis: A Road to Remission?

    In non-human primates, stress - a predisposing factor to depression in humans - decreased neurogenesis, and neurogenesis levels were normalized by antidepressants . These and many other studies gave the neurogenic hypothesis of depression its initial robust trajectory ( 2 ): adult-generated hippocampal neurons are needed for proper mood ...

  23. Reporting Psychiatric Disease Characteristics in Post-Mortem- and

    A major pathophysiological theory on the neurobiology of depression is dysfunctional neuroplasticity with reduced numbers of synapses. 1,2 Microglia are the main brain-resident macrophages and responsible for—among things—homeostasis of neuronal plasticity, by removing excessive proteins, dysfunctional synapses, and aberrant neurons. 3 In our transcriptomic profiling study of microglia in ...

  24. Hippocampal neuroplasticity, major depression and, not to ...

    A molecular and cellular theory of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:597-606. ... Treschow A, Bengzon J, Bolwig TG, Lindvall O, Tingstrom A. Increased neurogenesis in a model of ...