Flipped Learning Resources for Sociology A Level Courses.

Gender Differences in Crime – Chivalry Thesis

The chivalry thesis: examining gender differences in crime.

gender crime

What is the Chivalry Thesis?

The chivalry thesis proposes that because of their perceived vulnerability as victims of male violence, female criminals are treated differently than their male counterparts by criminal justice systems. It suggests that police officers, prosecutors, judges, and juries are all more likely to treat female offenders with leniency due to their gender . In other words, it implies that men face harsher punishments for the same crimes than women do.

‘Men hate to accuse women and thus send them to their punishment, police officers dislike to arrest them, district attorneys to prosecute them, judges and juries to find them guilty, and so on’ – Pollak (1950)

Evidence Against the Chivalry Thesis

Despite early evidence in favor of the chivalry thesis, researchers have since found plenty of data that refutes this hypothesis. For example, a 1998 study by Heidensohn et al., which looked at 35 countries including England and Wales found no evidence that female offenders were consistently treated more lightly than male offenders. Similarly, studies conducted by Carlen (1992) and Walklate (1998) both concluded that there was no significant difference between how men and women were dealt with by criminal justice systems.

In addition to these empirical findings, functionalist sex role theory has also been used to explain gender differences in crime. According to this perspective, differences in offending patterns between men and women can be explained by looking at how society rewards particular types of behavior for each gender based on traditional gender roles—for example, males are rewarded for aggression while females are rewarded for passivity and obedience. This theory suggests that instead of being treated differently because they are female, women commit fewer offenses because they do not receive social approval or rewards for aggressive behavior like men do.

Feminist Explanations for Female Crime

The feminist perspective provides an additional explanation for why women commit fewer crimes than men—namely, patriarchal power structures prevent them from having access to resources necessary to engage in certain types of criminal activities such as drug dealing or theft from businesses (Smart & Neale 2006). Because these activities require some degree of financial independence or freedom from supervision (both of which are largely denied to most women), they are less likely to engage in such crimes than their male counterparts who have greater access to resources and autonomy within society (Smart & Neale 2006). Furthermore, feminists argue that due to patriarchal power structures like sexism and racism which limit economic opportunities for minority women particularly those living in poverty-stricken neighborhoods- many resorting illegal activities as a means of survival.(Smart & Neale 2006).

Gender Differences in Crime

Overall it appears clear from our discussion today that while there may have been some truth behind Pollak’s original chivalry thesis when it was first proposed back in 1950; further research has revealed a much more complex picture regarding gender differences in crime today. As we have seen here there is strong evidence against the notion that criminal justice systems treat female offenders differently than male ones but also important insights provided by functionalist sex role theory and feminist explanations which cast light on why certain behaviors may be seen as ‘deviant’ or ‘criminal’ depending on one’s gender identity or socioeconomic background respectively . All these factors combined prove just how nuanced an issue like this really is – one which requires further exploration if we ever hope to fully understand its implications both now and into the future .

Why not check out our video on Why Women Commit Less Crime Than Men? ​​

Share this:.

  • Click to share on Twitter (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Facebook (Opens in new window)

Facebook

Copyright 2023 Precooked Sociology | Contact

You must be logged in to post a comment.

Criminology, Sociology and Policing at Hull

Student research journal, gender differences and sentencing: a critical literature review.

This review focuses on various pieces of literature that surrounds the perceived differences in sentencing gender. Also, literature examining the reasons why these differences are taking place between genders, and theories that could be applied when explaining these differences, will be scrutinised in order to give an indication as to whether a reason for gender differences in sentencing has been identified. The two theories that will be focused are the Chivalry theory (including Selective Chivalry) and the Double Deviance/ Evil Woman theory. Some other factors effects on sentencing, and the literature surrounding them, were also looked at as it would be negligent to say that only one factor could cause the perceived disparity between male and female sentencing. This review mainly focused on bodies of work based in the United States of America. This is because a large amount of research has been done in this area in the United States. Therefore, any questions answered will mostly be only applicable to that country due to cultural and legal differences in other parts of the world. Throughout the review a lot of bodies of research can be seen to be relatively supportive of the ideas that Double Deviance and Selective Chivalry has on the sentencing process, less so for regular chivalry. This is because various other factors seem to have some sort of effect as well as gender. Therefore, it is perhaps inaccurate to point to gender being the factor that decisively affects the sentencing outcomes. More research should be done in this area to fully grasp the relationship between gender and sentencing outcomes, while taking into account a larger number of relevant factors (legal and extra-legal) in order to not over attribute the outcomes to gender.

Author: Kieran Malon, April 2020

BA (Hons) Criminology with Psychology

1.   Introduction

A question that has long been discussed in various forms of academic literature is why there seems to be a difference in how genders are treated during the sentencing phase of trials. Within the United States the male population in prisons massively outnumber the female population. This may suggest a difference in how genders are treated at some stage during the criminal justice process in the United States. The stage that will be focused on within this review will be the sentencing phase of the system. A focus will also be made on two theories that have been looked at in various pieces of academic literature as well as other factors that may be contributing to this disparity in treatment. The literature will then be reviewed, and its validity will be looked at in relevancy and ability to test and explain the differences between genders.

The sentencing phase of the criminal justice process was chosen because there is evidence to suggest that this area is where the most difference in how the differing genders are treated in relation to what they are sentenced to do. This is suggested by academic literature like authors such as Steffensmeier et, al (1998), who used the gender effect as one of the factors that affect how an individual is sentenced, and Gelsthorpe (2013) who also looked at this factor as well as whether that number is justified in the crimes the crimes they commit. The justifiability may come in the form of whether the different genders get the same treatment when it comes to being sentenced to a crime that is similar in nature. If after looking at that justifiability differences do appear, we will then begin the process of looking at why that may be. Some theories that have been hypothesised will be looked at in relation to any differences in treatment found and the literature surrounding these theories will be reviewed and scrutinised in order to find if they have any relevant effects on modern sentencing outcomes.

The two theories that will be focused on when it comes to this topic are the Double Deviance theory and the Chivalry theory. These two are theories that have been discussed frequently in literature when it comes to this area of the criminal justice system. The literature surrounding these theories will be further discussed later in the text and their relevance, or lack of, will also be discussed further on. Beyond these two theories however, I will also be briefly looking at further factors that have been hypothesised to affect this stage as well as the gender of a defendant. This may include popular factors in research in this area such as race and age of a defendant. Also, as these factors may interrelate with each other it is important to discuss how these may be advantageous to some groups of people and disadvantageous to other groups. This may lead to different sentencing being given to different groups of people depending on characteristics that may be out of their control.

Review Structure

Within this review, three studies have been chosen as the focus for each of the theories (Chivalry and Double Deviance/ Evil Woman). These studies will be analysed as well as various other forms literature on these theories and relevant studies will be mentioned in reference to whether they support the studies in focus. An attempt at looking at a variety of different crimes has been made to investigate whether the theories apply or not across a spectrum of different sentencing events. In order to take into account other factors that may contribute to any differences, other than gender, in sentencing, two legal and two extra-legal factors have been chosen for further discussion. However, it should be noted that there are a range of other factors that will not be discussed in as much detail due to the large amount. But it is noted that these other factors exist that may influence sentencing decisions. This will be finished with a discussion on future studies that could be conducted as well as the limitations of this review and a conclusion on what can be found from the reviewed literature.

2. Chivalry, Selective Chivalry, and Paternalism Theory

This is an idea that was put forward by Otto Pollak (1950) to suggest that women within the criminal justice system are treated much more leniently than men due to the idea of chivalry towards women. Later Paternalism would be identified as something that frequently follows the chivalry aspect. It is suggested under this theory that law officials/judges see women as child-like and defenceless in their behaviour (Herzog and Oreg, 2008) and therefore are in need of protection, this leads to said leniency I favour of women. It can be said that Pollak’s research could be seen as outdated, we look at whether elements of the theory can be seen in today’s criminal justice system when looking at sentencing. Paternalism has led to modifications to the Chivalry theory however, this has been called Selective Chivalry. It is suggested by Farnworth and Teske (1995) that the leniency that comes with this chivalry idea is only open to white women and those who have wealthy backgrounds (Jeffries and Bond, 2013). There have been studies conducted on a range of different crimes which then have looked at how chivalry could possibly influence how sentencing decisions turn out the way in which they do.

Holland and Prohaska (2018) conducted a study in which they looked at whether females were more likely than males to receive shorter sentences while also controlling for relevant factors that could possibly affect the sentences also. One factor that they did want to account for and investigate further, in addition to gender, was race. Racial effects will be discussed in more depth further in the review, however they did want to see, as well as if there are differences between men and women, whether there are differences in sentences between women of different racial groups. This would allow a view into whether just gender could possibly effect sentencing between the males and females, or if other factors also need to be present to effect sentencing. Therefore, a second hypothesis was that white women would receive more lenient sentences than women of colour, which would support the work of Farnworth and Teske (2008) who suggested chivalry would only apply to white women. They also took into account geography when making various assumptions about what the results may show in their hypotheses. They hypothesised that women in the south of the country will be sentenced differently from women in other regions. The data collected looks at all federal cases that spanned the year between the 1 st of October 2014 and 30 th of September 2015. Also, due to the database containing information on a range of controls for legal and extra-legal factors, it means that an in depth analysis can be done to measure the various factors influence on the sentencing process and seeing how they could interact with the gender factor to lead to a sentencing outcome.

From the results, they found that their first hypothesis was proven correct. Women in general did receive shorter sentence lengths in comparison to males. This is also with legal factors considered and supports the chivalry theory. This supports the various bodies of work that have shown similar results through various experiments they have conducted (Doerner and Demuth, 2010; Rodriguez et al., 2006). This supports the idea that women are not seen by law officials as being as culpable for the crimes they committed as men are. However, the results contradicted the selective chivalry claims that would suggest that white women would mostly benefit from the leniency hypothesised in the chivalry theory. Hispanic and black women got shorter sentences when sentenced for federal drug crimes. This is surprising as the reverse has been shown in respect to selective chivalry as white women have benefitted the least from the suggested leniency shown in the results of this study. However, that may be because they have been adjudged to have been more out of line with traditional gender roles leading them to punished as doubly deviant rather then being viewed as needing protection.

There are some limitations on this study even if it does cover a population across a large area (Across multiple states). This is a problem in itself as guidelines differ across different states. This means that some judges will have more discretion than others, allowing them more freedom in decision making on sentences. Therefore, that has to be taken into account when looking at the results. Therefore, this may need to be built upon by more studies looking on a state by state basis accounting for those guidelines and taking them into account. There are also multiple other variables that could be explored. These may explain any contradiction with other forms of research that show support for selective chivalry as it does not have any detail on if these other factors could have had a effect on the sentencing process.

Embry and Lyons (2012) focused their study on the discrepancies in the way that male and female sex offenders are sentenced and how chivalry theory could possibly have an influence in these sentencing decisions. They give an initial idea of what they expect to find, which is based upon previous literature (Jeffries, Fletcher and Newbold, 2003; Curry, Lee and Rodriguez, 2004), that females receive more lenience than males do when it comes to sentencing. For their study, they collected data from the National Corrections Reporting program in order to do secondary data analysis. The sample they used was spread over ten years but they originally had more than this. They used the most recent ten years in order to get the most relevant results. This gave them a more modernised picture of what is affecting the current sentencing process as values and views can change over time. Also, using a large time frame allowed them to offset another problem which is the low number of female offenders who have been sentenced on being a sex offender. Therefore, looking at a data set over a larger period allows them to have a large sample of female offenders in the data to look at and analyse.

From the study, the evidence showed that although there is no difference in sentencing rates between men and women who commit sexual offences, men do tend to get harsher sentences. This shows that although judges can see that women should be charged (because they have committed a crime), they may not believe that they are as perhaps dangerous as male sex offenders are. Embry and Lyons (2012) earlier talk about this stereotypical image of a sex offender, which is usually a male offender with victim coming to mind as a young female according to them. This could be proven to have some accuracy if you look at the perceived leniency that could be inferred from the results of this study. Due to women not fitting the stereotypical image of a sex offender, even if they have in fact committed the crime, they may still be deemed as less dangerous as a male sex offender. This shows that although the decision that all genders need to be punished for criminal offences is equal, the severity of the punishment across genders is not equal.

There are areas that could be developed in this study in order to perhaps improve its scope and relevancy to a broader population as well as limitations that can be identified in this study. Although the number of women that were included in these studies was a fairly even split, this may not always be a study that can be compared to real life. This is because although it was a fairly even numbers, compared to a lot of studies, the offending rates of women are way below the offending rates of males. Therefore, even if they did get a better idea how the factors and theories may affect a trial. Therefore, if you did want to investigate these discrepancies women will based on even figures, women will be sampled far more than they generally offend. These results may simply seem to point to one answer when it is simply just a question of numbers in terms of offending rates. There is also an issue with the fact that the study they conducted was built from basic figures taken from the data base they were sourced. Although we can assume from the studies that support the chivalry theory that sentence lengths were affected by the gender of the individual being sentenced, we can be certain due to the lack of specificity within the statistics. Therefore, we cannot rule out the fact that there was a higher percentage of males who had committed a more severe form of the crime that they had committed in comparison to the females who were being sentenced. A final limitation would be that although they would class this as a cautious generalization, they could only possibly say that it is a cautious generalization of the population in the United States, where it was based. The sample population that had been sentenced that they were looking at was entirely from the United States, if they broadened their sample to include statistics from various other countries a much larger cautious observation could be made.

Therefore, if they were going to look at doing a further study with this as the basis, a few steps should be taken to expand on this body of research. A more detailed data set would be needed in order to see more information about crimes committed or perhaps so we can find out more about the defendant being sentenced. We would hopefully be able to see whether Chivalry was in fact taking affect in the sentencing phase, if there were legitimate reasons for sentencing for one group being to harsher degree or if other theories and factors may be able to be more relevant in affecting the process. Finally, a sample of offenders that are from a range of different countries in order to give it the best chance of it being more generalizable to the rest of the world. Different countries have different views, values and offending rates. It would therefore be interesting to see if these theories can be applied across more than just one country.

Study Three

A final study that looks at the Chivalry theory is a study conducted by Spivak et al. (2014), who looked at an area that is unique from the other pieces of literature that were focussed upon. They looked at status offences committed by juvenile offenders. Status offences committed by juveniles include but not limited to, truancy, consumption of alcohol or tobacco or running away. This is an interesting area as previous literature done on this suggests that status offences are the only area in the juvenile system in which female offenders outnumber male offenders (Tracey et al., 2009). It is generally assumed that males do commit more crime than females (Messerschmidt, 2007) and so to find a category in which males do not outnumber females, and in fact females outnumber males, makes it an area for further study. The study was conducted in Oklahoma and the data was collected by a local agency that collects basic data on juvenile cases. They then cut down the cases to only look at the relevant cases in relation to the type of crime they were looking at (Status offences). In relation to this review, two of the hypotheses included looking at the cases as to whether the chivalry theory could apply to these cases. More specifically, they wanted to see if girls’ cases were filed for review, in comparison to boys. They suggest that if proven this may show a want to make sure girls’ cases are scrutinised to make sure they get a correct judgement.

From the results of the study conducted, both hypotheses relating to chivalry seem to be supported. The results show in this that girls were more likely then boys to have their cases further reviewed. An idea of why this could be explained by the chivalry theory is they want to try and protect girls from being guilty through further looking at their cases and the circumstances behind them. This perhaps leading to mitigating circumstances being shown on their behalf which could result in them receiving lesser sentences. Therefore, if chivalrous and paternalistic attitudes can be found even when it comes to looking at cases involving juveniles, it suggests that the want to protect females may start from juvenile court and be seen through most age groups once moved to be judged and sentenced as an adult. However, in this case it must be noted that although it was shown that girls did tend to get lesser sentences than boys, the relationship between gender and lesser sentences was very weak. Meaning that more studies must be done in this area as it is inconclusive when it comes to whether sentencing may differentiate between female and male juveniles, even if there is a slight relationship in favour leniency towards girls.

As is a regular problem when it comes to a lot of research in this area, a lot focus on one state for their research. This means that it cannot be generalisable as there are many differences in population and justice processes across the world and even in the United States. Therefore, more research in this area would be needed specially to help the more inconclusive aspects of this study. The database used is also quite dated for this study as they admit. This means that changes might be viewed if data was collected for juvenile cases now. If policies have been brought more recently, this may lead to a difference in results and lead to different hypotheses being drawn.

3.   Double Deviance / Evil Woman

Double Deviance is theory based on the point of view that certain women are punished under the view of doing two things wrong. They are viewed as having broken societal norms and expectations of how a woman behaves, as well as breaking the law. They are then judged upon the basis that they have done doubly wrong.  Murphy and Brown (2000) suggest that under this theory it creates a situation where women can either be demonised or can be shown more leniency depending on if they broke these societal norms on what is expected of women. Double Deviance theory which is sometimes referred to as the Evil Woman theory. Although this idea may seem like selective chivalry in the way in which some women may be treated more leniently. It differs greatly in the notion that women who break these societal ideas of gender norms are punished even greater than men do when they commit the same crime. Women who fit into this theory and are seen as doubly deviant are seen as more blame worthy in this case which leads to harsher sentences, even if the crime they have committed is the same (Herzog and Oreg, 2008; Tillyer, Hartley and Ward, 2015).

Tillyer et, al (2015) based their study on looking at the perceived unfairness that exists within the court systems. Although they noted various factors may contribute to these differing sentencing outcomes between various groups, they chose to focus this study on gender and the different theories surrounding the gender factor. The crime they chose to investigate was narcotic cases taken from a federal data base. Narcotic cases have been chosen because it is a crime that will be viewed as breaking traditional gender roles. Therefore, if the theory is to be accurate the results will show that women get a harsher punishment then men who commit the same crime. Some factors may effect this as well however such as criminal history as well as evidence showing that race may have an effect as well (Spohn and Holleran, 2006) therefore that also has to be taken into account when looking at the gender and sentencing differences. The dates taken from the database in order to be analysed has been specifically chosen due to it have the highest amount of women to have committed crime within this time frame, with females being most populous being sentenced for this crime than any other in the dates analysed. This should allow for a good comparison to be made between male and female sentencing cases as it gives more cases that will be analysed then other crimes and dates. According to the authors they look to answer two hypotheses. They want to test whether women with limited criminal history will get more lenient sentences and those with extensive criminal history get a harsher sentence then their male counterparts.

The results in this case showed support for both the hypotheses in their assumptions. The first hypothesis which suggested more lenient sentences for women who had limited criminal histories was correct. This can be assumed under the Double Deviance theory that it has led to those who are deemed to have acted in accordance with the image of how an idealised woman should act, which has led to them getting a more lenient sentence overall. The results gained from the database also suggested that the second hypothesis was also correct in it assumption that women who have been deemed to have broken the law and the norms of societal conduct for a women have been punished more severely than men who committed a similar offense. This assumption supports the hypothesis of Bontrager et, al (2013) and Herzog and Oreg (2008) that women are punished further for factors and occurrences that allow them to be viewed as doubly deviant. However, this could also show why there is a gap between the genders within the prison numbers. Although some women are overly punished for breaking a conceived notion of how a woman should act, some are also being given lesser sentences then men for committing similar crimes, which may explain some huge gaps in numbers between the genders in prisons. This study also shows the importance of how criminal history interacts with gender to influence the sentencing phase. That is a unique aspect about this study and has allowed it identify a key factor that has effected women who are being sentenced and may allow law officials to become more aware of factors that may be unconsciously effecting their decision making process. This sort of identification, if it increased awareness, could lead to more equal sentences in regards to these factors.

The limitations that have been noted does increasingly affect the ability to make this study generalizable. A large limitation that seems plague this study is the effect that new policy changes have on the sentencing phase. These policy changes may give judges more or less discretion when it comes to sentencing. For example, if a policy came in after this study was released that limited a judge’s discretion on sentencing for crimes like drug crimes, it may lead to a more equal distribution regardless of gender. This would be because a judge would then have less freedom when they are passing a sentence and may have to stick to more rigid guidelines when sentencing. If a policy such as this did come into law, then another study would have to be held in order to investigate how these changes may have affected sentencing and may lead to theories like the double deviance theory losing its validity. A further limitation is absence of a few different factors that may provide more information on the complexities when it comes to examining gender and sentencing. For example, they only limitedly consider factors such as family status and whether they are depended upon by others, which may include children. This is identified as a factor that could influence a sentencing decision and would be helpful if a more in depth study was done to look at how factors such as this could impact the findings and if they would give them a different look to lead to different findings.

The study conducted by Koons-Witt et, al (2012) had a smaller focus when it comes to population they focused on. They conducted their study on the state of South Carolina, whereas the previous study looked at sentences across the country. However, within this study they looked at various crimes to see how the effect of gender changes across the various crimes that are committed. As well as looking at how gender has an effect when it comes to sentencing, they consider various other factors and how they interact with interact with gender sentencing processes. These include race and gender which will be discussed later in the review. Within the study they expect to find that women are treated more leniently when it comes to sentencing, which is in line with chivalry theory. However, as mentioned within this study the environment they a basing this study in must be taken into account. According to Koons-Witt et, al (2012) the state of South Carolina historically has rather conservative views of women which means that there may be stronger view, than in most places, in the traditional gender role of women. Traditional crimes for women are often non-violent crimes such as fraud according to Rodriguez et, al (2006). Therefore, when women commit crimes that are not viewed as traditionally crimes women commit (violent crimes), the evil woman hypothesis may have a stronger effect then it may do in states or countries with less conservative views.  The data for this study was collected from a now disbanded commission within the state. They focused on the latest data set that was made by the commission which was from 2001. This was due to various reforms that made the data collected before problematic to use. Therefore, they had 12 months of data to study and analyse.

The results for this study again showed support for the double deviance hypothesis. Similarly Tillyer et, al (2015), shows that only women with very limited criminal history are shown leniency in this case. Koons-Witt et, al (2012) mentions how the effect of women having an extensive criminal history background makes the leniency that is shown to women completely vanishes which then leads to them being sentenced on the same level as men. A woman with an extensive criminal history seems to then lose the protection that is often shown when it comes to sentencing. This could be as the law officials view that as the criminal history builds up, they are viewed as increasingly culpable for their actions and lose leniency that they are perceived to have. As a result, they receive greater sentences than females with lesser criminal histories might have. The support for the evil woman theory is further shown by this study because it again shows that women are shown leniency up until a certain point within the criminal justice system. Although this study focuses just on one state, it still supports a pattern that has been shown in many different pieces of literature on this topic (Belknap, 2007; Franklin & Fearn, 2008). This can also explain why there is a clear difference in how women and men are treated in this study. They note that in the sample they looked at the average for women with no criminal history was lower than the average for males with no criminal history. Therefore, this would then lead to a lack of cases where the double deviance hypothesis would take place which would, if women did have a higher average criminal history, theoretically lead to more women being sentenced on a similar or more harsh level then the males.

There some limitations to this study and suggestions on how to further this line of research in the future, if it has not already been conducted. The focus of this study was on, as the author says, a rather conservative state in relation to how women are supposed to behave. Due to this being on juts the single state, it would be interesting to see how this would compare to other states that are viewed as conservative in a similar way to South Carolina. This would give a better idea of how the ideological in the environment may change how different genders are sentenced. If similar results are seen, then it can be ruled out as an outlier and helps with generalizability. It would also be interesting to see how a more liberal state would sentence with a focus on differences between genders. This would give information on whether the conservativeness of the environment may have an effect of sentencing between genders and if it does, how much of an effect does it have. Due to it being secondary data they are working from as well, they cannot control what was recorded and what was not recorded. This has led to various factors being excluded from the recorded data that may have had an influence on the sentencing process. Although this may not have necessarily affected what was learnt from the results, some information that may have allowed a further, more detailed analysis from taking place. This would have looked at how these factors interacted with gender and sentencing outcomes, due to some of these factors often being stereotypically thought of as female roles, such as whether there are dependent children or the role of the individual with a family. These may have an effect due to women often being thought of as the primary care giver when children are involved compared to males. Daly (1987) suggested that women are more likely to receive leniency due to them holding important familial roles. This therefore that accounts for some disparity in sentencing outcomes between genders. However, without data recorded on this factor we cannot tell if this influenced sentencing outcomes in this case.

Tasca et, al (2018) based their study upon analysing how parental status and sentence length interact with each other. The study was based in the state of Arizona with data collected through self-report and official data. The questions for their study were based around whether the parenthood factor, whether they have children that they care for, get affected by gender when sentencing. They also wanted to look at how sentencing lengths vary between offenders without children as well as parents who are involved with their children and those who are not involved. Various studies have already been done in this area on how familial status may affect sentencing outcomes (Daly, 1987; Freiburger, 2011). However, the unique aspect of this study is that it also considers how involved the offender may be with their children. This will give a more complex look into how the parenting dynamic affects sentencing outcome, while considering how gender plays into the dynamic. Within this study, they also look at various smaller factors within the parenting dynamic, such as how women incarcerated often tend to be single mothers with reliant children. Therefore, with factors like these in mind it may be easier to interpret the results as well as make initial predictions. Although within this study they are not necessarily looking into how the evil woman theory plays into this study, through the results it gives you an idea of how the theory’s ideas may have had a role in affecting the sentencing outcomes.

The results, if split between men and women parents, without breaking them down into the further categories could already suggest that women might be viewed as doubly deviant in the crimes they have committed. This is because the results show that female parents received longer sentences than fathers. Under the evil woman hypothesis, you could suggest that this may be because they have broken the traditional gender role of committing a crime when they are supposedly the primary caregiver in comparison males who are traditionally the economic support for the family. These initial results contradict previous research and hypothesis (Daly, 1987; Daly, 1989), however there may be other factors to be revealed that explain this further in the results section. Once the results were then further broken down, although it did show that women who lived with their children prior to arrest received more lenient sentences compared to mothers uninvolved with their children. However, it still shows that males still received less harsh sentences then the women. The fact that the results did show less lenient sentences on women who were uninvolved with their children could suggest that double deviance has an effect. It could be argued that the women who are uninvolved with their children are being more severely punished for not performing their motherly duties and are therefore being punished for more than just the crime they committed. It is also mentioned that the sample was drawn from offenders who are not new to being involved with the criminal justice system. Therefore, the criminal history factor also has to be taken into account as it has been earlier suggested that it can negate any leniency they may have been granted under the chivalry theory (Tillyer et al, 2015; Koons-Witt et al, 2012). Therefore, it could be argued that because this study was mainly populated by a sample of women who have a criminal history, most would lose the leniency effect that would be applied to those without a criminal history. Meaning that if the evil woman theory was applied here it could explain why the women had on average a higher length of sentence then the males did. As under the theory they would be treated more harshly for breaking traditional gender roles and committing the crime.

There are couple of limitations and suggestions for future studies that could be made based from this study. One limitation of this is the need for a larger sample. This is because varying court systems throughout the United States, as well as throughout the world, may show different results. It may have been more generalizable if they at least looked at various court judgements in different states within the country. Due to the different court systems between states giving judges varying degrees of discretion, that may prove to be a key factor in how sentencing patterns show in the results. Therefore, a larger more varied sample should be looked at in further research to see if the results and hypotheses drawn from the results are applicable to more than just Arizona. As the authors mentioned also, the majority of the sample had criminal backgrounds and that may have had a potential effect on the results. Therefore, a more varied sample in terms of whether they have a vast criminal history or not should be explored. This will allow a more balanced view on how much effect certain factors have on the sentencing process as it hard to measure the effect of factors such as criminal history when many of them have criminal histories.

4. Contradicting Literature

Although a lot of literature that has been analysed show a pattern that supports the hypotheses. There is going to be some literature that contradicts the pattern that is seen. This can be for various reasons and these reasons can be discussed later when looking at the literature. One piece of literature that was reviewed did seem to contradict the pattern of support and although there may be more, only one will be the focus here.

A study that contradicts the findings in support of the theory is one that looks at chivalry in relation to nonviolent offending. Specifically, the study undertaken by Koeppel (2012), looks at nonviolent property crime in rural areas in the state of Iowa, in the United States. This study is interesting as Rodriguez et, al (2006) mentioned that there are varying degrees of leniency in the sentencing process depending on the crime, but males do often receive harsher sentences. Rodriguez et, al (2006) and their body of work suggested that males were more likely to receive harsher sentence outcomes when it comes to property crime. This, if backed up by continuous research as well as the study of focus here, would look like it would be following a pattern on which females receive more lenient sentences across multiple types of crime. The author of this study (Koeppel, 2012) suggests that we may see differences because of the setting also, as the they are focussing on a rural setting. Steffensmeier et al. (1998) suggests that because of the volume of cases that an urban judge must get through, they succumb to using stereotypes and generalizations in order to make their decisions. Therefore, if this theory is correct, we should see a decline in the difference in sentences between male and female who commit the same crime. This could then show evidence against chivalry and suggest other factors may yet have a larger influence on sentencing.

The data was collected from five small rural counties that have similar demographics; this could then decrease the effect that different environments may have had on the process. The sample size in this case is a 507 split between 188 females and 319 males. Worth noting also was that 95% of the sample were white which will be a point brought up later when analysing this study. The presumed result for this study was based from previous research and that a clear pattern of leniency in favour of females would be present. However, the results of the study contradicted previous research and found that gender did not have a significant effect on the sentencing outcomes. This is surprising, if the Chivalry theory is to be believed, considering the nature of the crime itself. The crimes looked at were nonviolent crimes that would, in theory, be seen as crimes a woman would get more leniency under the Chivalry theory. Herzog and Oreg (2008) discuss how the judges would think of women as defenceless and weak under chivalry and paternalism. Therefore, because it is a white, female, nonviolent offender they fill that criteria in which chivalry/paternalism is suppose have an effect and lead to leniency in sentencing them. This may also give support to the idea that sentencing may vary between urban and rural with the idea of chivalry also having a different degree of effect. It also goes against Rodriguez et, al (2006) and their idea that chivalry can be seen across the entire spectrum of crime sentences and even if it does have an effect, that effect is negligible enough to not affect the results as much as it would compared to other crimes perhaps because of the non-violent nature of it. Also, due to there being no specific gender effect notable, the double deviance theory cannot be applied either as measures of traditional sex roles did not have any effect on sentencing length. Therefore, in this case the results suggest that no matter if they have broken traditional gender norms, they sentenced on equal basis with women who might not be viewed to have broken traditional gender norms.

There are some developments that could be made to this study that would allow researchers to develop a clearer and more real to life picture of this area of study. One such development would be to look at basing the study in more diverse areas or look at similar data from rural areas in different countries if they want to focus on the rural aspect of the study. This is because it has been suggested (Herzog and Oreg, 2008; Jeffries and Bond, 2013) that chivalry only really effectively applies to certain women who fit the ideal image of “womanhood” which according to Steffensmeier and Demuth (2006) is white and middle class. Therefore, because this study was done in an environment where there is a miniscule amount of individuals, compared to the sample population size, where from an ethnically diverse background, it makes it hard to be generalizable except from maybe to small counties with little ethnic diversity like the ones that have been researched in this study. Also, although it could show some partial evidence against general chivalry’s effects on sentencing for this crime, it would struggle with some other theories. It would struggle to show evidence against selective chivalry as it has very little diversity to show that certain women would get better treatment then others due to its lack of a diverse sample population. As previously referred, it would only be able to show evidence to support counties with similar populations to itself and it would struggle to provide evidence in other states and counties as the diversity varies around the country. Support for the generalization of the conclusions of these studies is very low as it would only apply to a very small group. They state themselves that the results may differ from rural counties in different states. A further limitation was the issue that for some cases they could not access their criminal background. Therefore, they were unable to take the factor of whether some may have a criminal history which would then affect the outcome. The resulting sentence could then be different because of this and would then have to be considered when looking at the results of this study. For example, the reason why in some of the cases women look like their sentence is just as long or harsh as their male counterparts is because they have a criminal history. If this was then it may be that women in this case may have only gotten similar judgements due to having a criminal history. Although, it may be unlikely that enough women had criminal histories that affected their sentence length to then affect the results and conclusion of this study, we cannot rule it out due there being no proof against this idea.

5. Other Factors

So far, the focus has been on how certain theories help explain the differences that may be seen during the sentencing phase of the criminal justice system. Various other factors have been mentioned when looking at the theories and how they may moderate and interact with gender and the theories surrounding differences in sentencing in relation to gender. However, this chapter will look more specifically at studies surrounding factors and how they may have an influence on the sentencing process. Four factors, legal and extra-legal, shall be looked at within this section, however there are many more factors that have been looked at in various research and are can be said to have some sort of effect. Each factor will look at how they interact with gender and sentencing, looking at whether they have a significant impact on how defendants are sentenced.

Racial Factor

Within this section the race of the defendant being sentenced will be looked at in reference to seeing if there are any differences to be seen between gender and race. There are substantial bodies of research that detail how race can affect how individuals may be sentenced, with white defendants often getting the more lenient sentence (Brennan & Spohn, 2008; Spohn, 2000). The focus for this however will be how race and gender can lead to differences in sentencing with a discussion on what those differences are and what they show about the sentencing system.

What is found through looking through various pieces of literature is that black women do not receive the same level of lenience from the court system as white women do (Koons-Witt et al., 2012; Brennan, 2006; Steffensmeier et al., 1993). Therefore, you can assume the leniency that is supposed to apply to women when it comes to sentencing only applies to certain women, which supports the theory of selective chivalry. This shows that there may be some king of racial stereotype in causing this and allows them to fall out of the archetype image of women that need this protection because they believe they are have a low risk to society. However, there are also studies that contend this theory that black females are treated less leniently then white females. Many of these studies still find differences in how people of different races are sentenced, however they find that the difference mostly occurs when it comes to black and white males as earlier mentioned (Steffensmeier & Demuth, 2006; Spohn & Spears, 1997). It can also be suggested that the area that is picked to base the study may affect the outcome and results of these studies. It is suggested by Zatz (2000) that studies based in the United States seem to see a pattern of where race appears to be a larger factor in the sentencing phase and these states where you see it are in the south of the country. Therefore, you may assume that the study may vary from area to area depending on how strong traditional gender roles and views on women are valued. For example, the Koons-Witt et, al (2012) study is based in South Carolina which is in the south of the country. They mention how conservative views on women are held within the state and the results showed that black women and men appear to be on the receiving end of less lenient sentences. A lot of studies also seem to only consider black and white sentencing disparities. There are studies however, that show Hispanic defendants can often face even harsher punishment then both black and white defendants (Brennan & Spohn, 2008). Therefore, although race does seem to have a definite effect in relation to men on a general basis, it is less certain in its effect when women are the defendant. A future study could look at the effects of gender and race and compare them between various states or countries in order to determine whether there is any connection between the factors.

The factor of age has not yet been discussed in this review though it is often included as control within most literature. Most of these studies often focus on the age of male defendants rather than across the gender divide. This maybe perhaps due to it having more of an influence over male sentencing then female leading to a lack of need to test how the factor may influence sentencing of women.

Steffensmeier et al. (1998) hypothesised this when looking at how a range of different factors interact when sentencing. They found that younger males are often sentenced more harshly than older individuals. They also found that as the defendant got older, the race and gender differences diminished. This may be because they are deemed too old to be deemed as much of a threat to the general public. This was then further built upon by Spohn and Holleran (2000) who narrowed down the age in which an individual was most likely to be punished harshly. It also must be borne in mind that they looked at which ethnic groups were mostly likely to receive harsher punishment as well as the age range in which those groups would likely receive this punishment. They identified that black and Hispanic male defendants between the ages of 21 and 29 while being unemployed were most likely to receive the harshest punishments out of the different groups. Therefore, it must be noted that age can show various patterns, when interacting with other factors, that can lead to various other avenues of research and groups that can be focused on that lead to much groups being identified for further examination.

Crime Severity Factor

How serious an offence is may seem like a more obvious indicator into how a defendant may be sentenced with the idea that the more serious the crime, the harsher the sentence. However, this may be a key factor to explain the why there is a lot more males serving out more severe sentences. According to literature women are a lot less likely to commit serious crimes and violent crimes (Belkanp, 2007; Rodriguez et al., 2006). Non-violent crimes, as previously discussed, like fraud and theft are seen as crimes that traditionally women commit. However, this may result in women who commit violent crimes being affected by the views that are brought up in the theories above.

This is possibly the most key feature that can lessen the disparity between sentencing leniency. It is shown in research that a lot of the disparity disappears when it comes to violent crimes (Rodriguez el al., 2006; Boritch, 1992). This is because under the selective chivalry theory, this would fall out of its purview as it is crime that outside of the gender role expectations (Koons-Witt et al., 2012). They, therefore, lose the leniency that they would get under that paradigm as well as being judged as doubly deviant because of that breaking of the gender norms. Warren et al. (2011) suggest that as the seriousness of the offence increases the less discretion the judge then has when giving a sentence. This would then lead to this factor outweighing other key extra-legal factors, such as gender, because the seriousness of the crime has more influence over the sentence than any possible influence these extra-legal factors could possibly have on the outcome of the sentencing. This does mean that when the crime is less severe, the extra-legal factors can seem to have some measure of effect. Rodriguez et al. (2006) can support this view as they saw differences when it came to property crime in the way in which leniency was shown in favour of women when it came to sentence them.

Criminal History Factor

Criminal history is factor that was discussed in one of the studies that was focused on earlier in this review. The results for that study suggested, in the case of narcotics cases at least, that women do tend to get more lenient sentences if they have a lack of criminal history in comparison to males with a lack of criminal history (Tillyer et al., 2015). This can be further backed by Spohn (2000) and Koons-Witt et al. (2012) in relation to chance of being incarcerated.

From the various pieces of literature that have reviewed (Tillyer et al., 2015; Spohn, 2000; Koons-Witt et al., 2012; Daly & Tonry, 1997), there is a large amount that suggest that criminal history is very useful factor to use when predicting how a defendant may be sentenced. Daly and Tonry (1997) suggest that when judges are deciding on how to sentence an individual, when they have some discretion on how sentence a defendant, they look at criminal history as means of predicting future behaviour. They suggest that they use it as a means of testing how much they respect the law. A large criminal history would suggest a lack of respect and give the impression that they have no reservations about breaking the law. If the judges have doubt that that the defendant will not commit another crime, then they will be a lot less likely to give sentence that will be lenient. They may have a lack of belief that the offender would learn from committing their crime and that it would expose the wider public to harm. All these different issues stemming from a larger criminal record would leave a law official less inclined to be lenient to what in their view may be a veteran offender.

6. Conclusion and Discussion

In terms of volume of research in the area of sentencing disparities between genders, there is large amount, as shown above. However, there are a few interesting areas of future research that could be partaken in to further understanding with more specific factors. For example, an underlying factor in the United States may be what region the sentencing is taking place in (Zatz, 2000; Myers and Talarico, 1986). As mentioned earlier certain states seem to have conservative views regarding gender roles as well as Zatz (2000) mentioning that race effects can generally be seen to be stronger. A more recent investigation into the differences that can be seen across a variety of states would be an interesting area. As a detailed comparison between states that have historically been seen either conservative or liberal in their views would be able to give an idea how much of an effect the historical views of the state they are based in, is having. More studies in this area conducted outside of the United States would also be interesting. There is a very large body of work existing in this this area in the United States and not as many in other countries. Therefore, more studies done in other parts of the world may show if cultural differences still allow the ideas of the above theories to have a basis there. As well as how much the other extra-legal and legal factors have in relation to sentencing. This is a large limitation within this review as the most popular studies in this area are mostly conducted in the United States, making it harder to generalize it on continental or worldwide basis due to lack of a large body of research in other countries. Also due to there being so many different extra-legal factors that exist and could influence the sentencing process, more studies at looking into precisely how much each factor may influence a sentencing procedure. The current studies that have been conducted use databases that only give so much information on different factors. Therefore, a future study on the subject should try to address this issue which can be done by a range of measures. One such way could be creating a database that is more qualitive in its approach and includes more extra-legal factors with more information on the offender and their offences. This can allow the analysis of more than on factors, in order to establish their influence, they may have on decision, on the same sentencing cases. More research into race and gender sentencing outcomes may also be an avenue of further research as although there is research in this area a lot of them only investigate the black and white race comparison. This approach risks missing out on how other ethnic groups experience the sentencing process and if there can be witnessed differences in how they are treated in that phase of the justice system

Based from the evidence shown in the studies above and the results given. It can be suggested that Selective Chivalry and the Evil Woman/Double Deviance theories have some relevance in showing how sentencing decisions are made. Gender does appear to be a factor when determining how an offender is sentenced. It can be shown that a large body of work, in which I reviewed, shows a pattern that supports the idea that women are shown more leniency when it comes to sentencing. Yet are judged more harshly sometimes then men when other factors contribute that could break the traditional gender roles and traditional gender stereotypes such as committing violent crime, as opposed to non-violent crime. However, they also show that other factors may need to be taken into consideration in order for it to have a significant effect. Further research into how extra-legal may affect sentencing, due to the sheer number of factors that there is, and how they may be assisting the gender disparity when it comes to sentencing outcomes. Overall, the literature reviewed does generally point to the idea that gender does play a role in sentencing outcomes, but also that other factors are also considered in order to give sentence and not just gender. Chivalry, Selective Chivalry and Evil Woman/Double Deviance theories all cannot be discounted in having elements of the ideas within them being relevant to sentencing within the United States justice system.

Bibliography

Belknap, J. (2007). The invisible woman: Gender, crime, and justice (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth Publishing Company

Bontrager, S., Barrick, K., & Stupi, E. (2013). Gender and sentencing: A meta-analysis of contemporary research. The Journal of Gender, Race, & Justice, [online] 16, 349-635. Available at: https://heinonline.org/HOL/Page?handle=hein.journals/jgrj16&id=371&collection=journals&index= [Accessed 3 April 2020]

Boritch, H., (1992). Gender and Criminal Court Outcomes: An Historical Analysis. Criminology , [online] 30(3), pp.293-326. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1992.tb01106.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Brennan, P. and Spohn, C., (2008). Race/Ethnicity and Sentencing Outcomes Among Drug Offenders in North Carolina. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice , [online] 24(4), pp.371-398. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1043986208322712 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Curry, T., Lee, G. and Rodriguez, S., (2004). Does Victim Gender Increase Sentence Severity? Further Explorations of Gender Dynamics and Sentencing Outcomes. Crime & Delinquency , [online] 50(3), pp.319-343. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0011128703256265 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Daly, K., (1987). Discrimination in the Criminal Courts: Family, Gender, and the Problem of Equal Treatment. Social Forces , [online] 66(1), p.152. Available at: https://academic.oup.com/sf/article-abstract/66/1/152/2231816 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Daly, K., (1989). Rethinking Judicial Paternalism: Gender, Work-Family Relations, and Sentencing. Gender & Society , [online] 3(1), pp.9-36. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/089124389003001002 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Daly, K. and Tonry, M., (1997). Gender, Race, and Sentencing. Crime and Justice , [online] 22, pp.201-252. Available at: https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/abs/10.1086/449263 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Doerner, J. and Demuth, S., (2010). The Independent and Joint Effects of Race/Ethnicity, Gender, and Age on Sentencing Outcomes in U.S. Federal Courts. Justice Quarterly , [online] 27(1), pp.1-27. Available at: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/07418820902926197 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Embry, R. and Lyons, P., (2012). Sex-Based Sentencing: Sentencing Discrepancies Between Male and Female Sex Offenders. Feminist Criminology , [online] 7(2), pp.146-162. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1557085111430214 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Farnworth, M. and Teske, Jr., R., (1995). Gender Differences in Felony Court Processing: Women & Criminal Justice , [online] 6(2), pp.23-44. Available at: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1300/J012v06n02_02 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Fernando Rodriguez, S., Curry, T. and Lee, G., (2006). Gender Differences in Criminal Sentencing: Do Effects Vary Across Violent, Property, and Drug Offenses? Social Science Quarterly 87(2), pp.318-339. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2006.00383.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Franklin, C. and Fearn, N., (2008). Gender, race, and formal court decision-making outcomes: Chivalry/paternalism, conflict theory or gender conflict? Journal of Criminal Justice 36(3), pp.279-290. Available at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0047235208000500 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Freiburger, T., (2011). The Impact of Gender, Offense Type, and Familial Role on the Decision to Incarcerate. Social Justice Research , [online] 24(2), pp.143-167. Available at: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11211-011-0133-8 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Gelsthorpe, L. (2013). ‘Sentencing and gender’ in Sheehan, R., McIvor, G., Trotter, C. (ed.) What Works with Women Offenders . Hoboken: Taylor and Francis.

Herzog, S. and Oreg, S., (2008). Chivalry and the Moderating Effect of Ambivalent Sexism: Individual Differences in Crime Seriousness Judgments. Law & Society Review , [online] 42(1), pp.45-74. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1540-5893.2008.00334.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Holland, M. and Prohaska, A., (2018). Gender Effects Across Place. Race and Justice , [online] p.215336871876749. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/2153368718767495 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Jeffries, S. and Bond, C., (2013). Gender, Indigeneity, and the Criminal Courts: A Narrative Exploration of Women’s Sentencing in Western Australia. Women & Criminal Justice 23(1), pp.19-42. Available at: https://research-repository.griffith.edu.au/bitstream/handle/10072/55598/86997_1.pdf?sequence=1 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

JEFFRIES, S., FLETCHER, G. and NEWBOLD, G., (2003). PATHWAYS TO SEX-BASED DIFFERENTIATION IN CRIMINAL COURT SENTENCING. Criminology , [online] 41(2), pp.329-354. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2003.tb00990.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Koons-Witt, B., Sevigny, E., Burrow, J. and Hester, R., (2012). Gender and Sentencing Outcomes in South Carolina. Criminal Justice Policy Review , [online] 25(3), pp.299-324. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0887403412468884 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Messerschmidt, J., (2007). Masculinities, Crime and. The Blackwell Encyclopaedia of Sociology , Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/9781405165518.wbeosm039 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Murphy, E. and Brown, J., (2000). Exploring gender role identity, value orientation of occupation and sex of respondent in influencing attitudes towards male and female offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology , [online] 5(2), pp.285-290. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1348/135532500168047 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Myers, M. and Talarico, S., (1986). The Social Contexts of Racial Discrimination in Sentencing. Social Problems 33(3), pp.236-251. Available at: https://academic.oup.com/socpro/article-abstract/33/3/236/1733372 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Pollock, O. (1950). The criminality of women . Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press

Spivak, A., Wagner, B., Whitmer, J. and Charish, C., (2014). Gender and Status Offending. Feminist Criminology , [online] 9(3), pp.224-248. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/1557085114531318 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Spohn, C. (2000). Thirty years of sentencing reform: The quest for a racially-neutral sentencing process. In J. Horney (Ed.), Policies, processes, and decisions of the criminal justice system : Vol. 3 (pp. 427-501). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.

SPOHN, C. and HOLLERAN, D., (2000). THE IMPRISONMENT PENALTY PAID BY YOUNG, UNEMPLOYED BLACK AND HISPANIC MALE OFFENDERS. Criminology , [online] 38(1), pp.281-306. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2000.tb00891.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Spohn, C. and Spears, J., (1997). Gender and Case Processing Decisions. Women & Criminal Justice 8(3), pp.29-59. Available at: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1300/J012v08n03_02 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Steffensmeier, D. and Demuth, S., (2006). Does Gender Modify the Effects of Race–ethnicity on Criminal Sanctioning? Sentences for Male and Female White, Black, and Hispanic Defendants. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, [online] 22(3), pp.241-261. Available at: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10940-006-9010-2 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

STEFFENSMEIER, D., KRAMER, J. and STREIFEL, C., (1993). GENDER AND IMPRISONMENT DECISIONS. Criminology , [online] 31(3), pp.411-446. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1993.tb01136.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

STEFFENSMEIER, D., ULMER, J. and KRAMER, J., (1998). THE INTERACTION OF RACE, GENDER, AND AGE IN CRIMINAL SENTENCING: THE PUNISHMENT COST OF BEING YOUNG, BLACK, AND MALE. Criminology , [online] 36(4), pp.763-798. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1998.tb01265.x [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Tasca, M., Cho, A., Spohn, C. and Rodriguez, N., (2018). The Role of Parental Status and Involvement in Sentence Length Decisions: A Comparison of Men and Women Sentenced to Prison. Crime & Delinquency , [online] 65(14), pp.1899-1924. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0011128718811929 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Tillyer, R., Hartley, R. and Ward, J., (2015). Differential Treatment of Female Defendants. Criminal Justice and Behaviour , [online] 42(7), pp.703-721. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0093854814560624 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Tracy, P., Kempf-Leonard, K. and Abramoske-James, S., (2009). Gender Differences in Delinquency and Juvenile Justice Processing. Crime & Delinquency , [online] 55(2), pp.171-215. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0011128708330628 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Warren, P., Chiricos, T. and Bales, W., (2011). The Imprisonment Penalty for Young Black and Hispanic Males. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency , [online] 49(1), pp.56-80. Available at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0022427810397945 [Accessed 3 April 2020].

Zatz, M. S. (2000). The convergence of race, ethnicity, gender, and class on court decision-making: Looking toward the 21st century. In J. Horney (Ed.), Policies, processes, and decisions of the criminal justice system : Vol. 3 (pp. 503-552). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

We will keep fighting for all libraries - stand with us!

Internet Archive Audio

chivalry thesis 1950

  • This Just In
  • Grateful Dead
  • Old Time Radio
  • 78 RPMs and Cylinder Recordings
  • Audio Books & Poetry
  • Computers, Technology and Science
  • Music, Arts & Culture
  • News & Public Affairs
  • Spirituality & Religion
  • Radio News Archive

chivalry thesis 1950

  • Flickr Commons
  • Occupy Wall Street Flickr
  • NASA Images
  • Solar System Collection
  • Ames Research Center

chivalry thesis 1950

  • All Software
  • Old School Emulation
  • MS-DOS Games
  • Historical Software
  • Classic PC Games
  • Software Library
  • Kodi Archive and Support File
  • Vintage Software
  • CD-ROM Software
  • CD-ROM Software Library
  • Software Sites
  • Tucows Software Library
  • Shareware CD-ROMs
  • Software Capsules Compilation
  • CD-ROM Images
  • ZX Spectrum
  • DOOM Level CD

chivalry thesis 1950

  • Smithsonian Libraries
  • FEDLINK (US)
  • Lincoln Collection
  • American Libraries
  • Canadian Libraries
  • Universal Library
  • Project Gutenberg
  • Children's Library
  • Biodiversity Heritage Library
  • Books by Language
  • Additional Collections

chivalry thesis 1950

  • Prelinger Archives
  • Democracy Now!
  • Occupy Wall Street
  • TV NSA Clip Library
  • Animation & Cartoons
  • Arts & Music
  • Computers & Technology
  • Cultural & Academic Films
  • Ephemeral Films
  • Sports Videos
  • Videogame Videos
  • Youth Media

Search the history of over 866 billion web pages on the Internet.

Mobile Apps

  • Wayback Machine (iOS)
  • Wayback Machine (Android)

Browser Extensions

Archive-it subscription.

  • Explore the Collections
  • Build Collections

Save Page Now

Capture a web page as it appears now for use as a trusted citation in the future.

Please enter a valid web address

  • Donate Donate icon An illustration of a heart shape

THe criminality of women

Bookreader item preview, share or embed this item, flag this item for.

  • Graphic Violence
  • Explicit Sexual Content
  • Hate Speech
  • Misinformation/Disinformation
  • Marketing/Phishing/Advertising
  • Misleading/Inaccurate/Missing Metadata

Cut-off text on some pages due too tight binding. This book contains pen markings.

plus-circle Add Review comment Reviews

90 Previews

DOWNLOAD OPTIONS

No suitable files to display here.

PDF access not available for this item.

IN COLLECTIONS

Uploaded by station08.cebu on October 26, 2022

SIMILAR ITEMS (based on metadata)

  • Search Menu
  • Advance articles
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submission Site
  • Open Access
  • Why Submit?
  • About Social Forces
  • Editorial Board
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Journals Career Network
  • Self-Archiving Policy
  • Dispatch Dates
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

Article Contents

  • < Previous

The Criminality of Women . By Otto Pollak. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1950. 180 pp. $3.50

  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data

Ronie Sheffield, The Criminality of Women . By Otto Pollak. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1950. 180 pp. $3.50, Social Forces , Volume 29, Issue 4, May 1951, Pages 461–462, https://doi.org/10.2307/2572738

  • Permissions Icon Permissions

Article PDF first page preview

Email alerts, citing articles via.

  • Recommend to your Library

Affiliations

  • Online ISSN 1534-7605
  • Print ISSN 0037-7732
  • Copyright © 2024 University of North Carolina Chapel Hill
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Review of the Literature and Lineage of Chivalric Ideals

  • First Online: 01 October 2017

Cite this chapter

chivalry thesis 1950

  • Adam I. Attwood 3  

Part of the book series: The Cultural and Social Foundations of Education ((CSFE))

270 Accesses

Attwood examines literature from various points of view on the history of chivalry and its legacy into the twenty-first century. Chivalry is examined as a case study for implications in social studies education, especially as it relates to envisioning history as a dynamic dialogue; part of Attwood’s theory of the archeophisomorph .

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Adams, N., & Bettis, P. (2003). Commanding the room in short skirts: Cheering as the embodiment of ideal girlhood. Gender and Society, 17 (1), 73–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243202238979

Article   Google Scholar  

Agada, J. (1998). Multicultural education and the emerging paradigm: An essay in cultural epistemology. The Urban Review, 30 (1), 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023289429871

Agada, J. (2001, March 18). Deconstructing the at-risk student phenomenon: Can librarian values salvage education for the 21st century? Paper presented at the Association of College and Research Libraries (ACRL) tenth national conference, pp. 81–88, 15–18 March, Denver. Retrieved from http://www.ala.org/acrl/sites/ala.org.acrl/files/content/conferences/pdf/agada.pdf

Apple, M. W. (2004). Ideology and curriculum (3rd ed.). New York: RoutledgeFalmer.

Google Scholar  

Arnold, M. (2008). Desire. In B. Carman (Ed.), The world’s best poetry, volume IV: The higher life (pp. 134–136). Charleston: BiblioBazaar. Original work published 1849.

Arnow, T. (Producer), & Stone, O. (Director). (2012). Savages [Motion picture]. West Hollywood: Relatively Media.

Asbridge, T. (2010). The crusades: The authoritative history of the war for the holy land . New York: HarperCollins.

Atwill, J. M. (1998). Rhetoric reclaimed: Aristotle and the liberal arts tradition . Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

Baker, B. M. (2013). The purposes of history? Curriculum studies, invisible objects and twenty-first century societies. Journal of Curriculum Theorizing, 29 (1), 25–47.

Barber, M. (1994). The new knighthood: A history of the order of the temple . New York: Cambridge University Press.

Barber, R., & Barker, J. (1989). Tournaments: Jousts, chivalry and pageants in the Middle Ages . Woodbridge: The Boydell Press.

Bayles, D., & Orland, T. (2001). Art & fear: Observations on the perils (and rewards) of artmaking . Santa Cruz: The Image Continuum.

Becker, M. J. (2000). Reconstructing the lives of south Etruscan women. In A. E. Rautman (Ed.), Reading the body: Representations and remains in the archaeological record (pp. 55–67). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Benioff, D., Weiss, D. B., Doelger, F., Caulfield, B., Strauss, C., & Martin, G. R. R. (Executive producers). (2013). Game of thrones [Television series]. New York: HBO.

Bennett, J. M. (1999). A medieval life: Cecilia Penifader of Brigstock, c. 1295–1344 . Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Bennett, M. (2003). Why chivalry? Military ‘professionalism’ in the twelfth century: The origins and expressions of a socio-military ethos. In D. J. B. Trim (Ed.), The chivalric ethos and the development of military professionalism (pp. 41–64). Leiden: Brill.

Berk, G., & Galvan, D. (2009). How people experience and change institutions: A field guide to creative syncretism. Theory and Society, 38 (6), 543–580. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11186-009-9095-3

Boyer, C. V. (1923). Self-expression and happiness: A study of Matthew Arnold’s idea of perfection. International Journal of Ethics, 33 (3), 263–290. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2377333

Bruni, L. (1987). The humanism of Leonardo Bruni: Selected texts (trans: Griffiths, G., Hankins, J., & Thompson, D.). Binghamton: Medieval & Renaissance Texts & Studies in conjunction with the Renaissance Society of America. (Original work published approximately 1442).

Bruni, L. (2001). History of the Florentine people, Volume 1: Books I-IV (trans: & Ed. Hankins, J.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1442).

Burn, K. (2007). Professional knowledge and identity in a contested discipline: Challenges for student teachers and teacher educators. Oxford Review of Education, 33 (4), 445–467. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054980701450886

Burnett, C. (1987). Justice: Myth and symbol. Legal Studies Forum, 11 (1), 79–80.

Castiglione, B. (2002). The book of the courtier (trans: C.S. Singleton, & Ed. Javitch, D.). New York: W. W. Norton & Company. (Original work published 1528).

Caughlan, S. (2005). Considering pastoral power: A commentary on Aaron Schutz’s “rethinking domination and resistance: Challenging postmodernism.”. Educational Researcher, 34 (2), 14–16. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X034002014

Cheng, M. M. H., Chan, K.-W., Tang, S. Y. F., & Cheng, A. Y. N. (2009). Pre-service teacher education students’ epistemological beliefs and their conceptions of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 25 (2), 319–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2008.09.018

Chibnall, M. (1993). The empress Matilda: Queen consort, queen mother, and lady of the English . Malden: Blackwell Publishers.

Cogman, B. (Writer), & MacLaren, M. (Director). (2014). Oathkeeper [Television series episode]. In D. Benioff, D. B. Weiss, F. Doelger, B. Caulfield, C. Strauss, & G. R. R. Martin (Executive producers), Game of thrones . United States: HBO.

Coldron, J., & Smith, R. (1999). Active location in teachers’ construction of their professional identities. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31 (6), 711–726. https://doi.org/10.1080/002202799182954

Connor, H. P. (Ed.). (2006). Off the pedestal: New women in the art of homer, chase, and sargeant . Newark: Rutgers University Press.

Coss, P. R. (1991). Lordship, knighthood and locality: A study in English society c.1180–c.1280 . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Cotts, J. D. (2013). Europe’s long twelfth century: Order, anxiety and adaptation, 1095–1229 . New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Crichton, M. (1999). Timeline . New York: Knopf.

Crouch, D. (2002). William marshal: Knighthood, war and chivalry, 1147–1219 (2nd ed.). Uppers Saddle River: Pearson.

Cumming, I. P. (1955). Vestige of chivalry: Ring tournaments in the South. The Georgia Review, 9 (4), 406–421.

de Charny, G. (2005). A knight’s own book of chivalry (trans: Kennedy, E.). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. (Original work published 1356).

de Pizan, C. (1999). The book of the city of ladies (trans: Brown-Grant, R.). New York: Penguin Books. (Original work published 1405).

Demurger, A. (2009). The last Templar: The tragedy of Jacques de Molay (trans: Nevill, A.). London: Profile Books.

Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). Introduction: The discipline and practice of qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 1–32). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education . New York: Macmillan.

Dewey, J. (2005). Art as experience . New York: Perigee Books. (Original work published 1934).

Dietler, M. (1994). “Our ancestors the Gauls”: Archaeology, ethnic nationalism, and the manipulation of Celtic identity in modern Europe. American Anthropologist, 96 (3), 584–605. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1994.96.3.02a00090

Dove, M. (1990). Mourning Dove: A Salishan autobiography (Ed. Miller, J.). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Duby, G. (1978). Medieval marriage: Two models from twelfth-century France (trans: Forster, E.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Duby, G. (1980). The three orders: Feudal society imagined (trans: Goldhammer, A.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Duby, G. (1985). William Marshal: The flower of chivalry (trans: Howard, R.). New York: Pantheon Books.

Eco, U. (2002). Art and beauty in the Middle Ages . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Ek, A., & Latta, M. A. M. (2013). Preparing to teach: Redeeming the potentialities of the present through “conversations of practice”. Education and Culture, 29 (1). https://doi.org/10.1353/eac.2013.0000

Elton, G. R. (1967). The Tudor revolution in government: Administrative changes in the reign of Henry VIII . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Fallon, L. M., O’Keeffe, B. V., & Sugai, G. (2012). Consideration of culture and context in school-wide positive behavior support: A review of current literature. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 14 (4), 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300712442242

Fallows, N. (2013). Introduction. In R. Llull (Ed.), The book of the order of chivalry (trans: Fallows, N., pp. 1–23). Rochester: Boydell Press.

Faught, C. B. (2008). Gordon: Victorian hero . Washington, DC: Potomac Books.

Fine, M. (2011). Sexuality, schooling, and adolescent females: The missing discourse of desire. Harvard Educational Review, 58 (1), 29–54. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.58.1.u0468k1v2n2n8242

Flinders, D. J., Noddings, N., & Thornton, S. J. (1986). The null curriculum: Its theoretical basis and practical implications. Curriculum Inquiry, 16 (1), 33–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/03626784.1986.11075989

Foucault, M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison (trans: Sheridan, A.). New York: Vintage Books.

Freire, P. (2000). Pedagogy of the oppressed (30th anniversary ed.) (trans: Ramos, M.B.). New York: Continuum.

Gadamer, H.-G. (2013). Truth and method (trans: Weinsheimer, J. & Marshall, D.G.). New York: Bloomsbury. (Original work published 1975).

Garrison, J. W. (1995). Style and the art of teaching. In J. W. Garrison & A. G. Rud (Eds.), The educational conversation: Closing the gap (pp. 41–60). Albany: State University of New York Press.

Garrison, J., & Rud, A. G. (2009). Reverence in classroom teaching. Teachers College Record, 111 (11), 2626–2646.

Geijsel, F., & Meijers, F. (2005). Identity learning: The core process of educational change. Educational Studies, 31 (4), 419–430. https://doi.org/10.1080/03055690500237488

Georgoudi, S. (1994). Creating a myth of matriarchy. In P. S. Pantel (Ed.), & A. Goldhammer (Trans.), A history of women: From ancient goddesses to Christian saints (pp. 449–463). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gest, K. L. (2010). Chivalry: The origins and history of the orders of knighthood . Birmingham: Ian Allen Publishing.

Gies, F. (2011). The knight in history . New York: Harper Perennial. (Originally published 1984).

Gies, J., & Gies, F. (1981). Life in a medieval city . New York: Harper Colophon.

Goldsworthy, A. K. (2006). Caesar: Life of a colossus . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Gravett, C. (1992). Knights at tournament . Oxford: Osprey Publishing.

Gravett, C. (2007). Knight , DK eyewitness books . London: DK Publishing.

Green, R. L. (2008). King Arthur and his knights of the Round Table . New York: Puffin Books. (Original work published 1953).

Greene, C., del Toro, G., Jashni, J., Parent, M., Tull, T. (Producers), & del Toro, G. (Director). (2013). Pacific rim [Motion picture]. United States of America: Legendary Pictures.

Gregor, M. (Writer & Director). (2014). Carthage’s lost warriors [Television series episode]. In S. Burns (Executive producer), Secrets of the dead . Arlington: PBS.

Gutterman, D. S. (1994). Postmodernism and the interrogation of masculinity. In H. Brod & M. Kaufman (Eds.), Theorizing masculinities (pp. 219–238). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Hall, J. (2010). Making art, teaching art, learning art: Exploring the concept of the artist teacher. International Journal of Art & Design Education, 29 (2), 103–110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1476-8070.2010.01636.x

Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450 , 557–560. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature0288

Hanel, R. (2010). Life as a knight: An interactive history adventure . Mankato: Capstone Press.

Hankins, J. (2011). Civic knighthood in the Early Renaissance: Leonardo Bruni’s De militia (ca. 1420) (Working paper). Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Harvard University. Retrieved from http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:5473602

Haraway, D. J. (1991). Simians, cyborgs, and women: The reinvention of nature . New York: Routledge.

Hardt, M., & Negri, A. (2001). Empire . Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Hart, P. (1991). Irving L. Janis’ victims of groupthink. Political Psychology, 12 (2), 247–278. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3791464

Hatfield, C., Montana, V., & Deffenbaugh, C. (2006). Artist/art educator: Making sense of identity issues. Art Education, 59 (3), 42–47.

Henri, R. (2007). The art spirit . New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1923).

Heshusius, L. (1994). Freeing ourselves from objectivity: Managing subjectivity or turning toward a participatory mode of consciousness? Educational Researcher, 23 (3), 15–22. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X023003015

Hirst, K. K. (2014). Secrets of the dead: Carthage’s lost warriors—A wild and woolly thought experiment [Review of the DVD Secrets of the dead: Carthage’s lost warriors , produced by PBS, 2014]. About.com. Retrieved from http://archaeology.about.com/od/alternative/fl/Secrets-of-the-Dead-Carthages-Lost-Warriors-review.htm

Hirst, M., Chibnall, C., Fuchs, F., Weber, J., Flynn, J., Thomopoulos, A., et al. (Producers). (2011). Camelot [Television series]. Ireland: Starz Originals.

Hodges, K. (2005). Forging chivalric communities in Malory’s le Morte D’Arthur . New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781403979322

Hong, J. Y. (2010). Pre-service and beginning teachers’ professional identity and its relation to dropping out of the profession. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26 (8), 1530–1543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.06.003

Hutnyk, J. (2005). The dialectics of European hip-hop: Fun^da^mental and the deathening silence. South Asian Popular Culture, 3 (1), 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/14746680500118335

Ianziti, G. (2012). Writing history in renaissance Italy: Leonardo Bruni and the uses of the past . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Jacques, D. J. (2010). Chivalry – Now: The code of male ethics . Winchester: John Hunt Publishing.

Johnson, T. W., & Reed, R. F. (Eds.). (2012). Philosophical documents in education (4th ed.). New York: Pearson.

Jones, R. (2011). Knight: The warrior and world of chivalry . Long Island City: Osprey.

Joseph, P. B. (2011). Conceptualizing curriculum. In P. B. Joseph (Ed.), Cultures of curriculum (2nd ed., pp. 3–22). New York: Routledge.

Joseph, D., & Heading, M. (2010). Putting theory into practice: Moving from student identity to teacher identity. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 35 (3), 75–87. https://doi.org/10.14221/ajte.2010v35n3.6

Joseph, P. B., Mikel, E. R., & Windschitl, M. A. (2011). Reculturing curriculum. In P. B. Joseph (Ed.), Cultures of curriculum (2nd ed., pp. 55–77). New York: Routledge.

Kaeuper, R. W. (1999). Chivalry and violence in medieval Europe . New York: Oxford University Press.

Kaeuper, R. W. (2005). Historical introduction to the text. In G. de Charny, A knight’s own book of chivalry (trans: Kennedy, E., pp. 1–46). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Keen, M. (2005). Chivalry . New Haven: Yale University Press. (Original work published 1984).

Kitsis, E., Horowitz, A., Chambliss, A., & Goldberg, I. (Writers), & Cheylov, M., & Barrett, D. (Directors). (2012). Lady of the lake [Television series episode]. In C. Boylan & A. Chambliss (Producers), Once upon a time . Burbank: American Broadcasting Company.

Lassieur, A. (2010). The Middle Ages: An interactive history adventure . Mankato: Capstone Press.

Lather, P. (1993). Fertile obsession: Validity after poststructuralism. The Sociological Quarterly, 34 (4), 673–693. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1533-8525.1993.tb00112.x

Lehr, S. (Ed.). (2001). Beauty, brains, and brawn: The construction of gender in children’s literature . Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Lesko, N. (2001). Act your age! A cultural construction of adolescence . New York: Routledge Falmer.

Lessard, J. (2008). Hospitaller: A tale of the unknown knight in the third crusade . Bradenton: Booklocker.

Lewis, R. (2011). Raising a modern-day knight: A father’s role in guiding his son to authentic manhood . Carol Stream: Tyndale House Publishers.

Llull, R. (2013). The book of the order of chivalry (trans: Fallows, N.). Rochester: Boydell Press. (Original work published approximately 1276).

Macedo, D., & Gounari, P. (Eds.). (2006). The globalization of racism . Boulder: Paradigm Publishers.

MacMahon, L. (2003). Chivalry, military professionalism and the early Tudor army in renaissance Europe: A reassessment. In D. J. B. Trim (Ed.), The chivalric ethos and the development of military professionalism (pp. 183–212). Leiden: Brill.

Macaulay, C. (1790/2014). Letters on education: With observations on religious and metaphysical subjects. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (Original work published 1790).

Martin, G. R. R. (2000). A clash of kings . New York: Bantam.

Marwick, A. (2006). Youth culture and the cultural revolution of the long sixties. In A. Schildt & D. Siegfried (Eds.), Between Marx and Coca-Cola: Youth cultures in changing European societies, 1960–1980 (pp. 39–58). Oxford: Berghahn Books.

Marx, K., & Engels, F. (1970). The German ideology (Ed. Arthur, C.J.). New York: International Publishers Company, Inc. (Original work written 1845; original work published 1926).

Marzano, R. J. (2007). The art and science of teaching: A comprehensive framework for effective instruction . Alexandria: ASCD.

McCarthy, L., SenGupta, S., Silberling, B., Weber, J., & Siracusa, F. (Executive producers). (2014). Reign [Television series]. New York: CBS.

McCoy, R. C. (1989). The rites of knighthood: The literature and politics of Elizabethan chivalry . Berkeley: University of California Press.

McIntyre, A. (2008). Participatory action research , Qualitative research methods series (Vol. 52). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Mersey, D. (2013). King Arthur. Myths and legends . Oxford: Osprey Publishing.

Milligan, E., & Ragland, R. G. (2011). Educate, equip, engage, empower: A framework for preparing preservice teachers in civic education. Action in Teacher Education, 33 , 94–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/01626620.2011.559446

Milner, H. R., & Tenore, F. B. (2010). Classroom management in diverse classrooms. Urban Education, 45 (5), 560–603. https://doi.org/10.1177/0042085910377290

Mondale, S., & Patton, S. B. (Eds.). (2001). School, the story of American public education . Boston: Beacon Press.

Munro, D. C. (1914). The children’s crusade. American Historical Review, 19 (3), 516–524.

Munsch, R. (2002). The paper bag princess (Illus.: Martchenko, M.). Buffalo: Firefly Books. (Original work published 1980).

Nash, M. A. (2005). Women’s Education in the United States, 1780–1840 . New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-137-05035-9

Netter, G., Rattray, E., Lowry, H., & Zucker, J. (Producers), & Zucker, J. (Director). (1995). First knight [Motion picture]. United Kingdom: Sony Pictures.

Nicholson, H. (2006). The knights Hospitaller . Rochester: Boydell & Brewer.

Nicolle, D. (2008). Knights of Jerusalem: The crusading order of Hospitallers 1100–1565 . Oxford: Osprey Publishing.

Neiman, F. (1957). The zeitgeist of Matthew Arnold. PMLA, 72 (5), 977–996.

Novick, P. (1988). That noble dream: The “objectivity question” and the American historical profession . New York: Cambridge University Press.

Oreck, B. (2004). The artistic and professional development of teachers: A study of teachers’ attitudes toward the use of the arts in teaching. Journal of Teacher Education, 55 (1), 55–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022487103260072

Orme, N. (2001). Medieval children . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Orme, N. (2006). Medieval schools: From Roman Britain to Renaissance England . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Ovitz, M., Levinsohn, G., Granger, D., Donner, R., et al. (Producers), & Donner, R. (Director). (2003). Timeline [Motion picture]. Hollywood: Paramount Pictures.

Ozment, S. (1981). The age of reform, 1250–1550: An intellectual and religious history of late medieval and reformation Europe . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Paglia, C. (1990). Sexual personae: Art and decadence from Nefertiti to Emily Dickinson . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Perry, M. (Ed.). (2012). Sources of the western tradition – Volume 1: From ancient times to the enlightenment (9th ed.). Boston: Wadsworth.

Perry, M., Peden, J. R., & Von Laue, T. H. (Eds.). (2008). Sources of the western tradition – Volume I: From ancient times to the enlightenment (7th ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Pinar, W. F., Reynolds, W. M., Slattery, P., & Taubman, P. M. (1995). Understanding curriculum: An introduction to the study of historical and contemporary curriculum discourses . New York: Peter Lang.

Price, B. R. (2001). Introduction. In W. Caxton & B. R. Price (Trans.), Ramon Lull’s book of knighthood & chivalry and the anonymous Ordene de Chevalerie (pp. i–iii). The Chivalry Bookshelf.

Raimi, S., Tapert, R., Donen, J., Nalle, N., & Biller, K. (Producers). (2008). Legend of the seeker [Television series]. United States of America: ABC Studios.

Rapple, B. (1989). Matthew Arnold and comparative education. British Journal of Educational Studies, 37 (1), 54–71.

Roddenberry, G., & Zambrano, J., (Writers), & Shaw, L. (Director). (1989). Loud as a whisper [Television series episode]. In G. Roddenberry et al. (Producers), Star Trek: The Next Generation . United States of America: Paramount.

Rothbauer, P. (2008). Triangulation. In L. Given (Ed.), The SAGE encyclopedia of qualitative research methods (pp. 893–895). Thousand Oaks: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412963909.n468

Rud, A. G. (2007). Caring for others as a path to teaching and learning: Albert Schweitzer’s reverence for life. In D. T. Hansen (Ed.), Ethical visions of education: Philosophies in practice (pp. 157–171). New York: Teachers College Press.

Rud, A. G. (2011). Albert Schweitzer’s legacy for education: Reverence for life . New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230116238

Rud, A. G., & Garrison, J. (2009, May). The reverent educator: The call to supporting students’ spiritually on our campuses. Spirituality in Higher Education Newsletter, 5 (2), 1–8. Retrieved from http://spirituality.ucla.edu/docs/newsletters/5/Rud_Garrison_Final.pdf

Rud, A. G., & Garrison, J. (2010). Reverence and listening in teaching and leading. Teachers College Record, 112 (11), 2777–2792.

Sagan, E. (2002). The splitting of the psyche; the splitting of the world: The projection of uncontaminated virtue and absolute evil. Journal of Psychohistory, 29 (3), 298–311.

Said, E. W. (1979). Orientalism . New York: Vintage Books.

Sameshima, P. (2007). Seeing red—A pedagogy of parallax: An epistolary bildungsroman on artful scholarly inquiry . Youngstown: Cambria Press.

Schulberg, L., et al. (1968). Historic India. Great ages of man: A history of the World’s cultures . New York: Time-Life Books.

Schutz, A. (2004). Rethinking domination and resistance: Challenging postmodernism. Educational Researcher, 33 (1), 15–23. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X033001015

Shockley, K. G., Bond, H., & Rollins, J. (2008). Singing in my own voice: Teachers’ journey toward self-knowledge. Journal of Transformative Education, 6 (3), 182–200. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541344608324017 .

Society for Creative Anachronism, Inc. (2009, June 11). A brief introduction to the Society for Creative Anachronism. In Organizational handbook (2001 ed., 2009 updated ed.). Retrieved from http://www.sca.org/docs/pdf/govdocs.pdf

Spielvogel, J. J. (2012). Western civilization (8th ed.). Boston: Cengage.

Spretnak, C. (1992). Lost goddesses of early Greece: A collection of pre-Hellenic myths . Boston: Beacon Press.

Sullivan, D. (2012). Rambling through the fields of justice in search of well-being for all. Contemporary Justice Review, 15 (2), 139–161. https://doi.org/10.1080/10282580.2012.681157

Symons, D. (1981). The evolution of human sexuality . New York: Oxford University Press.

Taylor, L. J. (2010). The virgin warrior: The life and death of Joan of Arc . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Thomas, L., & Beauchamp, C. (2011). Understanding new teachers’ professional identities through metaphor. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27 (4), 762–769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2010.12.007

Trim, D. J. B. (2003). Introduction. In D. J. B. Trim (Ed.), The chivalric ethos and the development of military professionalism (pp. 1–38). Leiden: Brill.

Warren, W. L. (1977). Henry II . English Monarchs. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Webb, H. (2010). The medieval heart . New Haven: Yale University Press.

Weinstein, C. S., Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55 (1), 25–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022487103259812

Weir, A. (2001). Eleanor of Aquitaine: A life . New York: Ballantine Books.

Weir, A. (2011). The wars of the roses . New York: Ballantine Books.

Wertheimer, L. (2006). Children of disorder: Clerical parentage, illegitimacy, and reform in the Middle Ages. Journal of the History of Sexuality, 15 (3), 382–407. https://doi.org/10.1353/sex.2007.0023

Wheeler, B., & Parsons, J. C. (Eds.). (2002). Eleanor of Aquitaine: Lord and lady . New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-137-05262-9

White, L. T., Jr. (1962). Medieval technology and social change . New York: Oxford University Press.

Whitehead, A. N. (1967). The aims of education and other essays . New York: The Free Press. (Original work published 1929).

Wiesner, M. E., Ruff, J. R., & Wheeler, W. B. (2008). Discovering the western past: A look at the evidence – Volume 1: To 1789 (6th ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Willis, P. (1977). Learning to labor: How working class kids get working class jobs . New York: Columbia University Press.

Woodruff, P. (2001). Reverence: Renewing a forgotten virtue . New York: Oxford University Press.

Zeydel, E. H. (Trans.). (1966). Vegabond verse: Secular Latin poems of the Middle Ages . Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Washington State University, Pullman, Washington, USA

Adam I. Attwood

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Attwood, A.I. (2018). Review of the Literature and Lineage of Chivalric Ideals. In: Social Aesthetics and the School Environment. The Cultural and Social Foundations of Education. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-60345-2_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-60345-2_2

Published : 01 October 2017

Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-60344-5

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-60345-2

eBook Packages : Education Education (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Psychiatr Psychol Law
  • v.25(5); 2018

Logo of pplaw

Saving damsels, sentencing deviants and selective chivalry decisions: juror decision-making in an ambiguous assault case

Lauren t. meaux.

Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, United States

Jennifer Cox

Megan r. kopkin.

In sexually motivated crimes, female defendants are treated more leniently and female jurors are more punitive, relative to their male counterparts. However, few studies have examined the impact and interactions of juror, defendant and victim sex in non-sexually motivated crimes. In this study, mock jurors responded to an assault case in which the sex of both the defendant and the victim was manipulated, creating four conditions. The female jurors reported higher confidence in a guilty verdict, regardless of the defendant's and victim's sex. Additionally, the mock jurors – particularly the females – were more confident in a guilty verdict when the victim was female, regardless of the defendant's sex. Finally, the mock jurors recommended a harsher sentence for the female defendant – but only when the victim was male. These results are discussed in the context of understanding sex and gender within the criminal justice system and potential implications for juror decision-making.

Introduction

In the adversarial United States (US) justice system, the jury plays a vital role in both civil and criminal trials. Jurors weigh and consider the evidence presented by both the prosecution (in civil cases, the plaintiff) and the defence, before ultimately reaching a verdict concerning the defendant's culpability. In felony criminal trials specifically, some jurisdictions allow for jurors to participate in the punishment phase of the trial as well (Hoffman, 2003 ). In these cases, jurors consider various factors related to the crime and the defendant in order to determine the most appropriate punishment to impose. Considering the critical role of juries within the US justice system, research regarding juror decision-making has been a dominant focus of psycholegal research for the last 65 years (Devine, 2012 ; Diamond, 1997 ).

The legal system is predicated on the assumption that jurors are unbiased decision-makers. The court attempts to safeguard this supposition by allowing both the prosecution and the defence to excuse jurors that they believe to be biased, as well as instructing jurors on their role to objectively apply the law and securing an oath from every juror to perform this duty accordingly. However, as evidenced by the wealth of literature examining jury decision-making, jurors cannot be expected to maintain complete impartiality (Bornstein, 1999 ; Bornstein & Greene, 2017 ). Decades of research suggests that juror decision-making may be influenced by a variety of extralegal factors, including cognitive biases, the quality of evidence communication and sociodemographic factors (Devine, 2012 ; Schutte & Hosch, 1997 ; Skeem & Golding, 2001 ; Stalans & Diamond, 1990 ). However, the specific circumstances under which extralegal factors play a role, as well as the robustness of these effects, remains a debate within the psycholegal literature.

Effects of defendant sex on verdicts

One extralegal factor of interest is the sex of criminal defendants. A large body of literature exists concerning the differential treatment of men and women in the criminal justice system (Ahola, Christianson, & Hellström, 2009 ; Auerhahn, 2007 ; Henning & Feder, 2005 ; Hodell, Wasarhaley, Lynch, & Golding, 2014 ; Pozzulo, Dempsey, Maeder, & Allen, 2010 ; Spohn & Beichner, 2000 ; Steffensmeier & Demuth, 2006 ). When considering the verdict phase of criminal trials, the vast majority of the research on this differential treatment focuses on intimate partner violence (IPV) and sexually motivated crimes. Within this context, female defendants are consistently treated more leniently than their male counterparts (Henning & Feder, 2005 ; Pozzulo et al., 2010 ). For example, Hodell et al. ( 2014 ) asked 189 mock jurors to read a scenario involving the killing of an allegedly abusive intimate partner and determine a verdict for the defendant. The researchers manipulated the sex of the defendant and the victim so that the scenario either involved a male killing an abusive female partner or a female killing an abusive male partner. It was found that the female defendant was more likely to be acquitted and that both the male and female jurors sympathized more with the female compared to her male counterpart, regardless of whether she was the defendant or the victim. Similar findings were reported by Pozzulo et al. ( 2010 ), with mock jurors more likely to find a male defendant guilty of sexual assault compared to a female defendant.

Despite the body of literature on gender effects in IPV and sexually motivated crimes, limited research exists that examines gender discrepancies and juror verdicts in non-IPV cases, such as assault and homicide (Devine, 2012 ). However, in one study, Blais and Forth ( 2014 ) found no main effect of defendant gender on juror verdict in a case of aggravated assault. In fact, of the studies that have found differences due to gender, confounding factors such as defendant mental illness (Breheney, Groscup, & Galitta, 2007 ) have limited the extent to which conclusions on the specific impact of gender can be drawn. Given the ‘file-drawer effect’ and the lower rate of publication for studies revealing null effects, one can conclude that the defendant's sex does not reliably influence juror verdicts in non-IPV cases.

Effects of defendant sex on sentencing

In addition to the effects of defendant sex on verdicts, researchers have also explored potential disparities in sentencing. Concerning IPV cases, Henning and Feder ( 2005 ) examined over 4000 domestic violence arrests over a one-year period and found that prosecutors were less likely to pursue criminal charges when the case involved a female defendant. Furthermore, female defendants were more likely than their male counterparts to be released on their own recognisance and less likely to serve time in jail.

However, when the crime does not include a sexual act or IPV, data are inconsistent (Ahola et al., 2009 ; Ahola, Hellström, & Christianson, 2010 ; Blais & Forth, 2014 ; Spohn, 1999 ). Some research concerning non-IPV crimes supports the notion of leniency towards female defendants. Drawing from a large sample of urban courts, Steffensmeir and Demuth ( 2006 ) found that female defendants, across a wide range of crimes, received more lenient sentences. Additionally, Spohn ( 1999 ) examined 1544 drug-related convictions over a two-year period in Chicago and determined that although females were less likely to be sentenced to prison than males, the females who were sentenced received sentence lengths comparable to their male counterparts. Ahola et al. ( 2009 ) found across a variety of crimes that, on average, mock jurors gave significantly shorter sentences to female defendants (2.78 years) compared to male defendants (4.02 years). However, not all research has concluded that there is a main effect of leniency on female defendants. Ahola et al. ( 2010 ) found a ‘same-sex penalty effect’, wherein judges and jurors provided harsher sentences for a defendant accused of a violent crime when the defendant's sex was the same as the sex of the judge or juror. On the other hand, when Blais and Forth ( 2014 ) examined the effect of manipulating defendant sex on juror decision-making in the context of a violent crime, they found no differences in incarceration rates between males and females.

Some opine that leniency is the product of chivalry and paternalism on the part of legal decision-makers. Chivalry theory postulates that, within the realm of the legal system, women are perceived as less accountable for their actions, and thus in need of protection from harsh punishment (Edwards, 1989 ). Some research suggests the existence of a selective chivalry effect, wherein only females whose crimes are consistent with stereotypical gender roles benefit from paternalistic leniency (Bickle & Peterson, 1991 ; O'Neil, 1999 ; Steury & Frank, 1990 ). Given that violent crimes are seen as masculine offenses (Gilbert, 2002 ; Hendree & Nicks, 2000 ), selective chivalry may offer one explanation as to the inconsistencies and discrepancies in verdict (Blais & Forth, 2014 ) and sentencing (Ahola et al., 2010 ; Blais & Forth, 2014 ) outcomes for males and females in IPV and non-IPV cases.

Effects of victim sex

Compared to the literature base exploring defendant sex, the research examining the impact of victim sex is less well developed, although fairly consistent in that offenders convicted of victimizing a female are more likely to receive harsher penalties (Curry, Lee, & Rodriguez, 2004 ; Hindson, Potter, & Radelet, 2006 ; Williams & Holcomb, 2001 ). For example, Curry et al. ( 2004 ) examined the data from 1242 cases across several large Texan cities, in which the defendants were convicted of assault, robbery or homicide. The researchers found that both male and female defendants who victimized a female received longer sentences than those who victimized a male. Additionally, of the defendants who victimized females, the males received the longest sentences. Other research suggests that prosecutors are more likely to pursue the death penalty when the defendant is male (Hindson et al., 2006 ), and that male defendants are more likely to receive the death penalty when the victim is female (Williams & Holcomb, 2001 ).

Similarly, Franklin and Fearn ( 2008 ) examined over 2500 homicide offences from across the US and determined that male defendants who targeted female victims received the most punitive sentences compared to any other defendant–victim sex dyad. The authors argue that these data support an adapted version of the chivalry theory, in that the criminal justice system and its legal decision-makers were acting to protect females and punish those males who have deviated from the protector role.

Effects of juror sex

In addition to defendant and victim sex, researchers have opined as to how, if at all, juror sex may influence verdict and sentencing decisions. A meta-analysis examined 36 juror decision-making studies and reported that female jurors were more likely than male jurors to convict in cases involving sex crimes (Schutte & Hosch, 1997 ). This effect has held true in other studies for a variety of sex crimes, including rape (McNamara, Vattano, & Viney, 1993 ), child sexual abuse (Gabora, Spanos, & Joab, 1993 ), repressed childhood sexual abuse (Golding, Sego, Sanchez, & Hasemann, 1995 ; Key, Warren, & Ross, 1996 ; Schutte, 1994 ) and teacher molestation of a student (Bottoms & Goodman, 1994 ). Additionally, female jurors are more likely than male jurors to give harsher punishments to defendants convicted of sex crimes (Scroggs, 1976 ). Of note, not a single reviewed published report found that male mock jurors were more punitive, suggesting a fairly robust relationship between juror sex and punitiveness in the context of sexually motivated and IPV cases.

However, the harshness of female jurors may not generalize outside of the context of IPV/sexually motived crimes. Devine ( 2012 ) notes that the theoretical relationship between juror sex and decision-making is based on the juror's ability to empathize with the victim, a trait which is presumably more salient for women, who are more likely to be victims of sexually motivated crimes (Catalano, Curto, & Young, 2013 ). Thus, researchers in this domain may have no theoretical basis for examining the relationship between juror and legal decision-making in crimes that are not sexual in nature. It is also possible that research has failed to find a consistent and robust impact of juror sex on decision-making (outside of the specific contexts mentioned above) because, indeed, no relationship exists. In fact, Devine notes that a lack of empirical support could be the result of the ‘file-drawer effect’, with non-significant relationships less likely to be published (Franco, Malhotra, & Simonovits, 2014 ; Rosenthal, 1979 ).

The current study

The purpose of the current study is to examine how defendant, victim and juror sex influence mock juror decision-making in an ambiguous case of violent assault. The aim is to develop a better understanding of what role, if any, these demographic factors play in legal cases that do not involve a sexually motivated crime. Regarding verdict, a main effect of defendant sex was anticipated, with the male defendant more likely to receive a guilty verdict than his female counterpart. A main effect of defendant sex on sentencing recommendations was also anticipated, with the female defendant receiving a more lenient sentence than the male defendant. Additionally, a main effect of victim sex on juror punitiveness was hypothesized, with jurors more likely to find the defendant guilty and sentence the defendant more harshly when the victim is female. Finally, given the dearth of published research exploring juror sex and decision-making in non-IPV and sexually motived crimes, it is of interest to understand the relationship between juror sex and verdict and sentencing decisions. No specific hypotheses were made regarding these factors.

In addition to the main effects listed above, it was expected that several interactions would emerge. First, an interaction between juror and victim sex was predicted such that female jurors would be more punitive in terms of conviction and sentencing, particularly when the victim is female. Second, it was anticipated that the male defendant would be more likely to receive a guilty verdict and a more punitive sentence, particularly when the victim was female. In addition, exploratory analyses were conducted to investigate a possible three-way interaction between juror, defendant and victim sex with no a-priori hypotheses.

Participants

A total of 394 undergraduate students at a large state university were recruited from introductory psychology courses and received class credit for participation, of which 48 were removed for failing one of three questions designed to ensure that participants appropriately attended to the stimulus material, resulting in a final sample of 346 participants. Just over half of the 346 participants (54.3%) identified as male, and an average age of 18.74 years ( SD = 1.10) was reported. The majority of the sample was Caucasian (85.0%), followed by relatively smaller proportions of Black/African Americans (8.7%), Asian/Asian Americans (1.4%), Native Americans (1.2%), and ‘other’ (3.8%).

Materials and procedures

The mock jurors first completed a standard demographic questionnaire followed by a series of questionnaires irrelevant to the current study. Next, they read a short summary of an ambiguous assault case in which the defendant claimed self-defence. In order to simulate a criminal trial, the summary included the judge's initial statements, the prosecution's and defence's opening statements, witness testimony from both the victim and the defendant and cross-examination transcripts.

The case describes an altercation between two acquaintances in which the victim accused the defendant of stealing money (US$500) from the victim while attending a party at the victim's apartment. According to the trial testimony, the victim confronted the defendant and a physical altercation ensued. The transcript states that the victim was stabbed in the chest with a piece of broken glass before being rushed to hospital, where doctors were able to treat his/her injuries. The testimony from the defence also states that the defendant sustained bruising to his/her neck. The stimulus materials conclude with instructions from the judge to the jurors regarding applicable law (based on Alabama's official criminal code) and their civic duty (e.g. to reach a verdict, burden of proof).

The sexes of the defendant and the victim, implied by gender-specific names (Jessica and Stephen vs James and Sarah), are manipulated to create four conditions: male defendant and female victim; male defendant and male victim; female defendant and female victim; and female defendant and male victim. With the exception of the name manipulations, all other information is identical in all the conditions.

After reading through the case materials, the mock jurors rendered a verdict (guilty or not guilty) and, in the case of a guilty verdict, a sentence recommendation in accordance with the guidelines provided by the judge. Although it is not particularly common for juries to dole out specific sentences in non-capital trials, a handful of jurisdictions do allow for felony juries to act in this capacity (Hoffman, 2003 ). In the current study, this variable serves as an indicator of mock juror perceptions of, and punitiveness towards, the defendant. The mock jurors were provided with guidelines based on Alabama law that outline a minimum of 2 years and a maximum of 20 years in a state prison.

The mock jurors also completed additional questionnaires pertaining to their perceptions of the defendant's conformity to traditional gender roles, treatment amenability and likelihood of recidivism, as well as the extent to which the defendant is a ‘psychopath’. Finally, the mock jurors answered three questions to ensure their comprehension of pertinent facts related to the case.

Preliminary analyses

Portions of the preliminary data have been reported elsewhere (Cox & Kopkin, 2016 ). The preliminary analyses indicated no significant demographic differences between the experimental groups. Regarding verdict, overall, 59% of mock jurors supported a guilty verdict. However, previous research suggests that mock jurors’ confidence in their verdicts may impact decision-making (Krauss, McCabe, & Lieberman, 2012 ). Therefore, consistent with earlier studies (Cox & Kopkin, 2016 ), verdict confidence was controlled for by multiplying subjective confidence ratings with categorical guilty (−1) and not guilty (+1) verdicts. This ‘Verdict Index’ calculation resulted in a continuous variable reflecting mock juror confidence in a guilty verdict (as indicated by scores closer to −1) and mock juror confidence in a not guilty verdict (as indicated by scores closer to +1). For all mock jurors, Verdict Index scores ranged from −1 to +1 ( M = −19.32, SD = 68.04).

In terms of sentencing decisions, more than half of the mock jurors (67%) endorsed the minimum punishment of 2 years in state prison. A small number (9.5%) selected the maximum punishment of 20 years, and the rest chose a sentence between these two extremes. Consistent with previous research (Cox & Kopkin, 2016 ; Cox, Edens, Rulseh, & Clark, 2016 ), these write-in responses were coded and combined with the minimum and maximum sentence options to create a single continuous ‘Sentencing’ variable. To account for variability, the coded variable considered the recommended number of years in prison. For example, if a mock juror recommended 10 years, this was coded as a 10. However, if a mock juror recommended probation or retribution, this was coded as a 0, considering that the defendant would spend no time incarcerated. Overall, the mock jurors recommended an average sentence of 4.59 years ( SD = 5.55).

Main effects

The hypotheses predicting a significant impact of defendant sex were not supported. Specifically, the mock jurors were not significantly more or less confident in a guilty verdict for the male defendant ( M = −15.15, SD = 71.21) compared to the female defendant ( M = −23.18, SD = 64.92), F (1, 341) = 1.00, p = .317, d = .11. Defendant sex also had no impact on recommended sentence length, with a similar sentence being delivered for the male defendant ( M = 5.20, SD = 6.25) and the female defendant ( M = 4.07, SD = 4.84), F (1, 196) = 1.40, p = .237, d = .20.

Regarding victim sex, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) indicates a significant effect, F (1, 341) = 4.02, p = .046, with mock jurors showing more confidence in a guilty verdict when the victim was female ( M = −25.81, SD = 67.54) than when the victim was male ( M = −12.39, SD = 68.04; d = .20). Considering only the mock jurors who found the defendant guilty, the mean recommended sentences do not differ between victim sex groups, F (1, 196) = 0.36, p = .552, with the mock jurors sentencing the defendant who assaulted a male victim to an average of 4.34 years ( SD = 5.57) compared to 4.81 years ( SD = 5.55; d = .08) for the defendant who assaulted a female victim.

Finally, a main effect of mock juror sex emerged, F (1, 341) = 9.36, p = .002, with more confidence shown in a guilty verdict by female mock jurors ( M = −31.66, SD = 64.71) than male mock jurors ( M = −9.03, SD = 69.19, d = .34). However, no significant differences emerged concerning sentencing recommendations, F (1, 196) = 0.77, p = .383, with male mock jurors recommending an average of 4.95 years ( SD = 5.68) and female mock jurors recommending an average of 4.25 years ( SD = 5.42; d = .13).

Interactions

In addition to the main effects, it was hypothesized that a number of interactions would emerge. A factorial MANOVA indicates no significant interactions between defendant sex, victim sex and mock juror sex on the Verdict Index ( Table 1 ). However, concerning the dependent variable of sentence recommendation, three interactions emerged ( Table 1 ). Simple effects analyses indicate that the female mock jurors recommended more lenient sentences when the defendant was female, F (1, 188) = 6.96, p < .01, d = .39, while male mock jurors recommended harsher sentences when the defendant was female, F (1, 188) = 7.76, p < .01, d = .41. Finally, all mock jurors, regardless of sex, sentenced the female defendant more harshly when the victim was male ( M = 4.87, SD = 6.01) compared to when the victim was female ( M = 3.33, SD = 3.32, d = .38).

Interaction effects of the defendant, victim and mock juror sex on the Verdict Index and the sentence.

Note. * p < .05; ** p < .01. Defendant = defendant sex; juror = mock juror sex; victim = victim sex.

Perceptions of the defendant, the victim and the case

In addition to the mock jurors’ ultimate legal decisions of verdict, their perceptions of the defendant were also assessed, as these perceptions may act as a proxy to better understanding the cognitive judgements being made. 1. Regarding the mock jurors’ perception of the defendant's likelihood to recidivate violently, a factorial MANOVA indicates no main effects, although a significant interaction emerged ( Table 2 ) wherein the female mock jurors rated the defendant as being more likely to violently recidivate, but only when the victim was female ( M = 60.25, SD = 26.91) as opposed to when the victim was male ( M = 54.64, SD = 24.92, d = .22). The mock jurors were asked how typical the defendant is in regard to their same-sex peers. When the victim was male, the mock jurors rated the defendant (either male or female) as more typical of their respective sex ( M = 36.80, SD = 21.71) compared to when the victim was female ( M = 30.71, SD = 23.29, d = .26). The mock jurors were also asked how much the defendant may benefit from mental health treatment. No significant main effects emerged; however, a factorial MANOVA yielded a significant three-way interaction, with the female mock jurors perceiving the male defendant as being less likely to benefit from treatment, but only when the victim was male ( Table 3 ). Finally, the mock jurors rated how ‘psychopathic’ they believed the defendant to be; the female defendant was rated as significantly more psychopathic ( M = 51.97, SD = 25.98) compared to the male defendant ( M = 36.64, SD = 26.23, d = .59). No other significant main effects or interactions occurred.

Influence of the defendant, victim and mock juror sex on how the defendant is perceived.

Influence of the defendant, victim and mock juror sex on perceptions of the likelihood that the defendant would benefit from treatment.

Note. * p < .05.

The results of the research on the impact of gender on legal decision-making are mixed (as demonstrated in the review above), with most studies indicating a significant impact of juror and/or defendant sex when the crime is sexually motivated. The purpose of the current study is to investigate how defendant, victim and juror sex interact to influence juror decision-making in the context of a non-sexually motivated violent crime. This is the first study to explore the main and interaction effects of each of these independent variables.

Defendant sex

While previous studies report increased punitiveness towards male defendants in IPV (Henning & Feder, 2005 ; Spohn & Beichner, 2000 ; Steffensmeier & Demuth, 2006 ) and sexual assault (Pozzulo et al., 2010 ) cases, the data from the current study indicate no effect of defendant sex on the juror verdicts and sentencing recommendations in an assault case. Although seemingly contrary to the chivalry hypothesis, null findings are consistent with Blais and Forth ( 2014 ), who report that the defendant's gender did not impact the jurors’ verdict in a case of aggravated assault. Considering publication bias (Franco et al., 2014 ), the ‘file-drawer problem’ (Rosenthal, 1979 ) in scientific research and the tendency for journals to focus on publishing significant results, one may opine that these null findings are more reliable and generalizable than is currently reported in the literature. Yet, replication (and dissemination) is necessary before any definitive conclusions can be drawn.

The null findings in the current study may be explained by selective chivalry theory. Building on Edwards’ ( 1989 ) chivalry theory, which proposes that protective attitudes towards women drive the leniency that they receive in conviction rates and sentencing, selective chivalry theory is postulated to account for non-traditional females in the legal system (Bickle & Peterson, 1991 ; O'Neil, 1999 ; Visher, 1983 ). Selective chivalry holds that only women who adhere to the normative female role regarding gender-typical behaviours benefit from the legal system's leniency because they are perceived as more respectable than their non-traditional counterparts. Furthermore, women who engage in behaviour that is not compatible with their traditional gender role (e.g. violence, physical aggression, promiscuity) are doubly punished – both for their crimes and for their failure to conform to gender norms (Bickle & Peterson, 1991 ; Eaton, 1987 ).

Previous studies have found some support for selective chivalry theory (Gilbert, 2002 ; Hendree & Nicks, 2000 ). For example, Fosterlee, Fox, Fosterlee, and Ho ( 2004 ) found that a victim impact statement in a mock heinous homicide case resulted in the participants reporting more anger towards a female defendant than a male defendant. Given that homicide is a highly aggressive act which is typically viewed as male-perpetrated behaviour (Gilbert, 2002 ), a female defendant accused of violence may not receive the same leniency afforded to a female defendant accused of a non-violent or drug-related crime.

In the current study, the treatment of the male and female defendants is relatively equal in terms of verdict and sentencing decisions, suggesting that selective chivalry does not afford leniency in violent assault cases. The mock jurors’ ratings on attitudinal variables potentially related to ultimate decision-making provide additional support for this notion; for example, the female defendant was rated as significantly more ‘psychopathic’ compared to the male defendant, suggesting that a difference in maleficent personality traits may have been perceived between the two sexes.

When examining gender differences, Eagly ( 1995 ) and Archer ( 1996 ) argue that interactions, as opposed to main effects, are most appropriate. Although defendant sex did not affect punitiveness overall, a significant interaction emerged regarding sentencing recommendations wherein the male mock jurors provided harsher sentences for the female defendant than for the male defendant. The mock jurors, regardless of sex, also sentenced the female defendant more harshly when the victim was a male compared to when the victim was a female. While female physical aggression is incongruent with traditional gender roles, female aggression directed at a male may be viewed as being even more so, and may explain these results. Further, these data are somewhat consistent with Breheney et al.'s ( 2007 ) findings of increased punitiveness towards females committing violent offenses, and lend further credence to the selective chivalry hypothesis.

A main effect of victim sex emerged in that the mock jurors were more confident in a guilty verdict when the victim was female compared to when the victim was male. Although little research has examined attitudinal perceptions and their effect on legal decision-making in regard to victim sex, this finding may suggest an adaptation of true chivalry. In patriarchal cultures, women are viewed as defenceless and thus requiring protection from danger (Edwards, 1989 ). Perhaps the same protective attitudes that some argue are afforded to female defendants (although notably not found in this study) translate to female victims – specifically, jurors may punish the defendant for causing harm to someone that society generally characterizes as defenceless, penalizing the defendant not only for committing a violent crime but for committing a violent crime against a woman.

A main effect of juror sex emerged wherein the female mock jurors were more confident in a guilty verdict regardless of the defendant and victim sex. This pattern, while present in the IPV literature, has not previously been reported for non-IPV, violent crimes. In IPV and sexually motivated cases, theory assumes that female jurors empathize with the victim because they are more likely to be victims of such crimes themselves compared to their male counterparts. Previously, the extent to which this theory translates to non-IPV and non-sexually motivated crimes was unknown, as the female stereotype does not typically include barroom brawls and fist fights; however, the data from the current study support the notion that juror sex may demonstrate a relationship with legal decision-making in contexts that are broader than IPV and sexually motivated crimes.

The results also indicate that the female mock jurors were less punitive when the defendant was female, regardless of the victim's sex. This leniency afforded by women to women may be related to gender-based homophily, the tendency of members of the same gender to affiliate or relate with one another (Lazarsfeld & Merton, 1954 ; McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2003 ). Lazarsfeld and Merton ( 1954 ) categorized most major sociodemographic variables that stratify society, such as sex, under status homophily. Status homophily based on sex has been observed in a variety of social situations, such as conflict resolution (Eder & Hallinan, 1978 ), employment settings (Kalleberg, Knoke, Marsden, & Spaeth, 1996 ) and voluntary organizations (Popielarz, 1999 ), and may be one explanation for this result.

Limitations

As is often a limitation in jury research, this study did not use actual jurors and deliberating processes but rather employed artificial stimulus materials, which may affect the outcome and also limits the ecological validity of the findings (Wiener, Krauss, & Lieberman, 2011 ). Further, it is common in mock jury research to recruit undergraduates, as research suggests that data developed from this group may be comparable to community samples (Bornstein, 1999 ; Bornstein et al., 2017 ). However, considering that community samples are the population from which juries are constructed, it may be beneficial to theory development to replicate this study with a more diverse and generalizable community sample.

The participants provided ratings on a number of continuous dependent variables. Although the statistical analyses indicate main and interaction effects of the independent variables on many of the dependent variables, a number of the standard deviations are large and overlap between groups, calling into question the utility of the outcome measures. Further, although significant results emerged, with some exceptions, the effect sizes are small, suggesting that the differences between groups, although present, may not be particularly meaningful.

Another notable limitation concerns the lack of focus on the intersectionality of race and gender. The stimulus material does not specify the race or ethnicity of the defendant and the victim; however, names have been shown to prime stereotypes (Carpusor & Loges, 2006 ) and the names used in the study (Jessica, Sarah, James and Stephen) may be more likely to be associated with White individuals. The disparate treatment of African Americans in the criminal justice system is well documented (Mitchell, Haw, Pfeifer, & Meissner, 2005 ), and it is unclear if the data from the current study would generalize to non-White defendants, victims and jurors. The study of the intersectionality of gender and race is important, and these data do not consider how gender and race may interact to influence juror decision-making. Further research in this area is needed before more definitive conclusions can be drawn.

The data from the current study suggest novel sex-dependent patterns regarding juror decision-making in a non-sexually motivated crime. Although sex-dependent patterns have been established in sexually motivated crimes (Gabora et al., 1993 ; Hodell et al., 2014 ; McNamara et al., 1993 ; Schutte, 1994 ; Schutte & Hosch, 1997 ), such patterns do not typically emerge for non-sexually motivated crimes. The results of the current study may have practical implications in that they may inform legal decision-makers as to the extralegal, demographic variables that can impact decision-making in a criminal context. Although one may interpret these data as suggesting certain patterns in female juror decision-making, the paucity of literature on the topic makes it impossible to draw definitive conclusions. Further research is therefore needed to investigate whether or not the effects that have emerged from these data are replicable and generalize to other non-IPV and non-sexually motivated crimes.

1. To facilitate clarity and conserve space, only significant interaction statistics are reported.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Marissa Stanziani. The content of this paper was previously presented at the 2017 American Psychology-Law Society Conference in Seattle, WA.

Ethical standards

Declaration of conflicts of interest.

Lauren T. Meaux has declared no conflicts of interest.

Jennifer Cox has declared no conflicts of interest.

Megan R. Kopkin has declared no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study

  • Ahola A., Christianson S. Å., & Hellström Å. (2009). Justice needs a blindfold: Effects of gender and attractiveness on prison sentences and attributions of personal characteristics in a judicial process . Psychiatry Psychology and Law , 16 , S90–S100. doi: 10.1080/13218710802242011 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ahola A. S., Hellström Å., & Christianson S. Å. (2010). Is justice really blind? Effects of crime descriptions, defendant gender and appearance, and legal practitioner gender on sentences and defendant evaluations in a mock trial . Psychiatry Psychology and Law , 17 ( 2 ), 304–324. doi: 10.1080/13218710903566896 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Archer J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: Are the social role and evolutionary explanations compatible ? American Psychologist , 51 ( 9 ), 909–917. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.909 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Auerhahn K. (2007). Just another crime? Examining disparity in homicide sentencing . The Sociological Quarterly , 48 , 277–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2007.00079.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bickle G. S., & Peterson R. D. (1991). The impact of gender-based family roles on criminal sentencing . Social Problems , 38 , 372–394. doi: 10.2307/800605 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blais J., & Forth A. E. (2014). Potential labeling effects: Influence of psychopathy diagnosis, defendant age, and defendant gender on mock jurors’ decisions . Psychology Crime and Law , 20 , 116–134. doi: 10.1177/0093854813519629 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bornstein B. H. (1999). The ecological validity of jury simulations: Is the jury still out ? Law and Human Behavior , 23 , 75–91. doi: 10.1023/A:1022326807441 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bornstein B. H., Golding J. M., Neuschatz J., Kimbrough C., Reed K., Magyarics C., & Luecht K. (2017). Mock juror sampling issues in jury simulation research: A meta-analysis . Law and Human Behavior , 41 ( 1 ), 13–28. doi: 10.1037/lhb0000223 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bornstein B. H., & Greene E. (2017). The jury under fire: Myth, controversy, and reform . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bottoms B. L., & Goodman G. S. (1994). Perceptions of children's credibility in sexual assault cases . Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 24 , 702–732. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb00608.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breheney C., Groscup J., & Galitta M. (2007). Gender matters in the insanity defence . Law & Psychology Review , 31 , 93–123. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carpusor A. G., & Loges W. E. (2006). Rental discrimination and ethnicity in names . Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 36 ( 4 ) , 934–952. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00050.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Catalano S. M., Curto V., & Young M. (2013). Intimate partner violence: Attributes of victimization, 1993–2011: Special report/Bureau of Justice Statistics: Special report (United States Bureau of Justice Statistics) . Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cox J., Edens J. F., Rulseh A., & Clark J. W. (2016). Juror perceptions of the interpersonal-affective traits of psychopathy predict sentence severity in a white-collar criminal case . Psychology, Crime & Law , 22 ( 8 ), 721–740. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2016.1174864 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cox J., & Kopkin M. R. (2016). Defendant and victim sex, sexism, and decision making in an ambiguous assault case . Women & Criminal Justice , 26 ( 5 ), 381–393. doi: 10.1080/08974454.2016.1167153 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Curry T. R., Lee G., & Rodriguez S. F. (2004). Does victim gender increase sentence severity? Further explorations of gender dynamics and sentencing outcomes . Crime and Delinquency , 50 , 319–343. doi: 10.1177/0011128703256265 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Devine D. J. (2012). Jury decision making: The state of the science . New York, NY: New York University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diamond S. S. (1997). Illuminations and shadows from jury simulations . Law and Human Behaviour , 21 , 561–571. doi: 10.1023/A:1024831908377 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eagly A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men . American Psychologist , 50 ( 3 ), 145–158. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.3.145 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eaton M. (1987). Question of bail: Magistrates’ responses to applications for bail on behalf of men & women defendants . In Carlen P. & Worral A. (Eds.), Gender, Crime & Justice (95–107). Philadelphia: Open University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eder D., & Hallinan M. T. (1978). Sex differences in children's friendships . American Sociological Review , 43 , 237–250. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Edwards A. R. (1989). Sex/gender, sexism and criminal justice: Some theoretical considerations . International Journal of the Sociology of Law , 17 , 165–184. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fosterlee L., Fox G. B., Fosterlee R., & Ho R. (2004). The effects of a victim impact statement and gender on juror information processing in a criminal trial: Does the punishment fit the crime ? Australian Psychologist , 39 ( 1 ), 57–67. doi: 10.1080/00050060410001660353 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Franco A., Malhotra N., & Simonovits G. (2014). Publication bias in social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer . Science , 345 ( 6203 ), 1502–1505. doi: 10.1126/science.1255484 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Franklin C. A., & Fearn N. E. (2008). Gender, race, and formal court decision-making outcomes: Chivalry/paternalism, conflict theory, or gender conflict? . Journal of Criminal Justice , 36 ( 3 ), 279–290. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2008.04.009 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gabora N. J., Spanos N. P., & Joab A. (1993). The effects of complainant age and expert psychological testimony in a simulated child sexual abuse trial . Law and Human Behaviour , 17 , 103–119. doi: 10.1007/BF01044540 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gilbert P. R. (2002). Discourses of female violence and societal gender stereotypes . Violence Against Women , 8 , 1271–1300. doi: 10.1177/107780102762478019 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Golding J. M., Sego S. A., Sanchez R. P., & Hasemann D. (1995). The believability of repressed memories . Law and Human Behavior , 19 , 569–592. doi: 10.1007/BF01499375 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hendree P. K., & Nicks S. D. (2000). Perceptions of violent crimes in relation to sex of perpetrator . Perceptual and Motor Skills , 90 , 485–488. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.90.2.485 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Henning K., & Feder L. (2005). Criminal prosecution of domestic violence offenses: An investigation of factors predictive of court outcomes . Criminal Justice and Behavior , 32 , 612–642. doi: 10.1177/0093854805279945 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hindson S., Potter H., & Radelet M. L. (2006). Race, gender, region and death sentencing in Colorado, 1980–1999 . University of Colorado Law Review , 77 ( 3 ), 549–594. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hodell E. C., Wasarhaley N. E., Lynch K. R., & Golding J. M. (2014). Mock juror gender biases and perceptions of self-defence claims in intimate partner homicide . Journal of Family Violence , 29 , 495–506. doi: 10.1007/s10896-014-9609-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoffman M. B. (2003). The Case for Jury Sentencing . Duke Law Journal , 52 ( 5 ), 951–1010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kalleberg A. L., Knoke D., Marsden P. V., & Spaeth J. L. (1996). Organizations in America: Analyzing their structures and human resource practices . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Key H. G., Warren A. R., & Ross D. F. (1996). Perceptions of repressed memories; A reappraisal . Law and Human Behavior , 20 , 355–563. doi: 10.1007/BF01499041 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krauss D. A., McCabe J. G., & Lieberman J. D. (2012). Dangerously misunderstood: Representative jurors’ reactions to expert testimony on future dangerousness in a sexually violent predator trial . Psychology, Public Policy, and Law , 18 , 18–49. doi: 10.1037/a0024550 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lazarsfeld P. F., & Merton R. K. (1954). Friendship as a social process: A substantive and methodological analysis . In Berger M. (Ed.), Freedom and Control in Modern Society (18–66). New York: Van Nostrand. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNamara K., Vattano F., & Viney W. (1993). Verdict, sentencing, and certainty as a function of sex of juror and amount of evidence in a simulated rape trial . Psychological Reports , 72 , 575–583. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1993.72.2.575 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McPherson M., Smith-Lovin L., & Cook J. M. (2003). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks . Annual Review of Sociology , 27 ( 1 ), 415–444. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.415 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mitchell T. L., Haw R. M., Pfeifer J. E., & Meissner C. A. (2005). Racial bias in mock juror decision-making: A meta-analytic review of defendant treatment . Law and Human Behavior , 29 ( 6 ), 621–637. doi: 10.1007/s10979-005-8122-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O'Neil M. E. (1999). The gender gap argument: Exploring the disparity of sentencing women to death . New England Journal on Criminal and Civil Confinement , 25 , 213–244. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Popielarz P. A. (1999). (In)voluntary association: A multilevel analysis of gender segregation in voluntary organizations . Gender & Society , 13 , 234–250. doi: 10.1177/089124399013002005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pozzulo J. D., Dempsey J., Maeder E., & Allen L. (2010). The effects of victim gender, defendant gender, and defendant age on juror decision making . Criminal Justice and Behavior , 37 ( 1 ), 47–63. doi: 10.1177/0093854809344173 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenthal R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results . Psychological Bulletin , 86 ( 3 ), 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schutte J. W. (1994). Repressed memory lawsuits: Potential verdict predictors . Behavioral Sciences & the Law , 12 ( 4 ), 409–416. doi: 10.1002/bsl.2370120409 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schutte J. W., & Hosch H. M. (1997). Gender differences in sexual assault verdicts: A meta-analysis . Journal of Social Behavior & Personality , 12 ( 3 ), 759–772. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scroggs J. R. (1976). Penalties for rape as a function of victim provocativeness, damage, and resistance . Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 6 , 360–368. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1976.tb02411.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skeem J. L., & Golding S. L. (2001). Describing jurors’ personal conceptions of insanity and their relationship to case judgments . Psychology, Public Policy, and Law , 7 , 561–621. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.7.3.561 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spohn C. (1999). Gender and sentencing of drug offenders: Is chivalry dead ? Criminal Justice Policy Review , 9 , 365–399. doi: 10.1177/088740349900900305 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spohn C., & Beichner D. (2000). Is preferential treatment of female offenders a thing of the past? A multisite study of gender, race, and imprisonment . Criminal Justice Policy Review , 11 , 149–184. doi: 10.1177/0887403400011002004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stalans L. J., & Diamond S. S. (1990). Formation and change in lay evaluations of criminal sentencing: Misperception and discontent . Law and Human Behavior , 14 , 199–214. doi: 10.1007/BF01352749 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steffensmeier D., & Demuth S. (2006). Does gender modify the effects of race-ethnicity on criminal sanctioning? Sentences for male and female white, black, and Hispanic defendants . Journal of Quantitative Criminology , 22 ( 3 ), 241–261. doi: 10.1007/s10940-006-9010-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steury E. H., & Frank N. (1990). Gender bias and pretrial release: More pieces of the puzzle . Journal of Criminal Justice , 18 , 417–432. doi: 10.1016/0047-2352(90)90057-I [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Visher C. A. (1983). Gender, Police Arrest Decisions, and Notions of Chivalry . Criminology , 21 ( 1 ) , 5–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.1983.tb00248.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wiener R. L., Krauss D. A., & Lieberman J. D. (2011). Mock Jury Research: Where Do We Go from Here ? Behavioral Sciences & The Law , 29 ( 3 ) , 467–479. doi: 10.1002/bsl.989 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams M., & Holcomb J. (2001). Racial disparity and death sentences in Ohio . Journal of Criminal Justice , 29 ( 3 ) , 207–202. doi: 10.1016/S0047-2352(01)00085-X [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

chivalry thesis 1950

Final dates! Join the tutor2u subject teams in London for a day of exam technique and revision at the cinema. Learn more →

Reference Library

Collections

  • See what's new
  • All Resources
  • Student Resources
  • Assessment Resources
  • Teaching Resources
  • CPD Courses
  • Livestreams

Study notes, videos, interactive activities and more!

Sociology news, insights and enrichment

Currated collections of free resources

Browse resources by topic

  • All Sociology Resources

Resource Selections

Currated lists of resources

Topic Videos

Chivalry Thesis - Gender and Crime

Last updated 30 Nov 2022

  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on Twitter
  • Share by Email

The concept of chivalry thesis as an explanation of why crime statistics show men commit many more criminal acts than women is explained in this video.

  • Gender (Crime)
  • Crime and Deviance
  • Chivalry Thesis

You might also like

Crime and deviance: a level webinar video 2017, ​example answer for question 1 paper 3: a level sociology, june 2017 (aqa).

Exam Support

​Example Answer for Question 2 Paper 3: A Level Sociology, June 2017 (AQA)

Example answer for question 4 paper 3: a level sociology, june 2017 (aqa), second year a level sociology students: listen up (3).

3rd October 2017

Making Demography Come to Life

18th October 2017

Modern Crime: Drug Dealing and the Dark Web

19th October 2017

Teaching Sociological Skills: Analysis

Our subjects.

  • › Criminology
  • › Economics
  • › Geography
  • › Health & Social Care
  • › Psychology
  • › Sociology
  • › Teaching & learning resources
  • › Student revision workshops
  • › Online student courses
  • › CPD for teachers
  • › Livestreams
  • › Teaching jobs

Boston House, 214 High Street, Boston Spa, West Yorkshire, LS23 6AD Tel: 01937 848885

  • › Contact us
  • › Terms of use
  • › Privacy & cookies

© 2002-2024 Tutor2u Limited. Company Reg no: 04489574. VAT reg no 816865400.

Marked by Teachers

  • TOP CATEGORIES
  • AS and A Level
  • University Degree
  • International Baccalaureate
  • Uncategorised
  • 5 Star Essays
  • Study Tools
  • Study Guides
  • Meet the Team
  • Crime & Deviance

The chivalry thesis claims that women will be treated more leniently for committing certain crimes, generally shoplifting is often associated more with females than males, but the statistics suggest that males commit many more acts of theft than women, an

Authors Avatar

Using material from Item A and elsewhere, assess the value of ‘chivalry thesis ‘in understanding differences in crime.  gfg

Pollak (1950) was of the opinion that police and magistrates tended to be more ‘chivalrous’ and ‘lenient’ towards female offenders, resulting in sentence disparities, and as a result, criminal statistics underestimate the amount of female offending. (Item A).

Pollak (1961) argued that men – namely in this case police officers, magistrates and judges, are socialised to be protective towards women and thus are less likely to chare or prosecute them, and are also treated more leniently in court. Pollak goes further to argue that women are accustomed to deceiving men, for example in faking orgasms during sex, or lying in a relationship to gain material wealth. This skill in deceit mean that their crimes, such as poisoning and infanticide, are less easily uncovered (item A) leading to women being underrepresented in criminal statistics.

The chivalry thesis claims that women will be treated more leniently for committing certain crimes, generally shoplifting is often associated more with females than males, but the statistics suggest that males commit many more acts of theft than women, and this may be because females are let off with a warning rather than a conviction. This could be because the statistics of crime are so male dominated, a police officer may not think convicting a woman of petty theft is worth it, when there may be, in his opinion, a man selling drugs elsewhere, it may not be worth it in his view. Similarly, men are more likely to be convicted of theft because the criminal justice system seemingly victimises males over females, while it is probably more likely that males are more likely to commit crime than females, the gap between the crime rates between gender may not be as large as first assumed. It is also possible that female occurrences of shoplifting, as Pollak argues, never even come to the attention of the authorities, again because more often than not they are not reported.

Join now!

Farrington and Morris (1983) which involved a study of ‘sentencing’ in Magistrates courts. They discovered in 1979, 6.6% of men, but only 2% of women found guilty of indictable offences were imprisoned. Although men received more severe sentences than women, the research found the differences disappeared when the severity of offences was taken into account. Although personal opinions of the judges, witness reports or specific details of the crimes in question may have played a role, this does show that to an extent women are more likely to be let off for an offence.

This is a preview of the whole essay

Hilary Allen (1989) based upon an examination criminal statistics of 1987, showed apparent leniency towards female offenders. For example, of people found guilty of indictable motoring offences, 73% of women, but only 54% of men were given fines, this difference resulting in more men being given prison sentences. Although these findings may not show the severity of the specific offences. Campbell through self report studies in 1976, of urban schoolgirls, compared to 16 year old males, these sources showed that 1.33 offences were committed by males for every 1.0 for females, this hugely contrasted to 1976 official figures on convictions, which showed 8.95 convictions for every female 1.0. This shows how official statistics can be greatly misleading, that although males may convict more crime, the gulf between the amount of offences shown by statistics, and popular opinion, is not as wide as it seems.

Although these studies do give evidence towards the chivalry thesis, the bulk of research does not give this indication. Steven box 1981, reviewed data from self report studies in UK and USA, and concluded that ‘the weight of evidence on women committing serious offences does not give clear support to the view that they receive differential and more favourable treatment from members of the public, police or judges’. Although it has to always be remembered that in a self report study, the validity and reliability relies upon the respondent’s ability and willingness to tell the truth, which is not always apparent.

To counter this, Buckle and Farrington carried out an observational study on shoplifting in a British department store in England 1981, found that only 1.4 % of the 361 females observed shoplifted, whereas 2.8% of the 361 males did. Although far too small of a sample to conclude anything categorically, it does provide evidence against the chivalry thesis.

A rather different, feminist view is that women are treated far more harshly by the criminal justice system than men, which often is the case in rape trials. Carol Smart argues that such trials ‘celebrate notions of male sexual need and female sexual capriciousness. ‘ The feminist view is often of the opinion that women are labelled as being dressed provocatively, being drunk when the rape occurred, or often the male in the case argues she did not object at the time of the incident, and by these happenings the male is let off. A similar feminist opinion is found in the case of domestic violence, in a study on domestic violence by Dobash and Dobash, it was found that police officers were ‘very unlikely to make an arrest when the offender has used violence against his wife. ‘(1979). this has spurred feminist campaigners to urge police officers to take cases of domestic violence more seriously.

Pollak’s full chivalry thesis is in short, slightly sexist as it states that ‘women are particularly adept at hiding their crimes, this is attributed to female biology ‘. Because of his inadequate analysis the theory has been subject to a heavy and fierce amount of criticism. Frances Hiedensohn 1985 slated the theory greatly, stating that is was based on analysis that had very little evidence and many unsupported assumptions, she regarded it as being based upon an unsubstantiated stereotypical image of women. In conclusion, although some parts of Pollak’s chivalry thesis, namely the parts saying women are naturally more likely to conceal crime are quite unsupported and base, studies associated with the theory have hinted that women are often in some cases treated more leniently by the criminal justice system. Although the extent of this cannot really be measured, statistics would have us believe that the gap between male and female criminality is huge, but upon closer investigation this is not the case. Reasons for this may be that criminology and sociology is largely dominated by males that the research is done on males by males, leading to an underrepresentation on female crime, or that perhaps female crimes are comparatively seen as trivial and are not deemed worthy of research. Whichever way it is looked it, there is a case for the argument that females are often not treated as harshly as males upon committing crime, but of course the opposite argument could be valid also.

Teacher Reviews

Here's what a teacher thought of this essay.

Avatar

Matthew Wilkin

Overall this is a really strong essay. The content never drifts from the focus of the question, there is a good even debate on both sides of the discussion and the use of the item has been applied well. The studies applied are also very effective and support the evidence throughout. At times there is opportunity to apply key terms such as iceberg theory or the hidden figure of crime.

The chivalry thesis claims that women will be treated more leniently for committing certain crimes, generally shoplifting is often associated more with females than males, but the statistics suggest that males commit many more acts of theft than women, an

Document Details

  • Word Count 1144
  • Page Count 2
  • Level AS and A Level
  • Subject Sociology

Related Essays

Examine some of the reasons why females may be less likely than males to commit crimes

Examine some of the reasons why females may be less likely than males to co...

crimes committed by males and females

crimes committed by males and females

Are women underrepresented in crime statistics because of sexism within the criminal justice system?

Are women underrepresented in crime statistics because of sexism within the...

Asses the arguments and evidence which suggest women commit much less crime than men

Asses the arguments and evidence which suggest women commit much less crime...

IMAGES

  1. "The Chivalry Thesis"- Otto Pollak (1950) by Amy Biggs on Prezi

    chivalry thesis 1950

  2. What is the chivalry thesis?

    chivalry thesis 1950

  3. PPT

    chivalry thesis 1950

  4. Chivalry thesis

    chivalry thesis 1950

  5. The chivalry thesis

    chivalry thesis 1950

  6. Chivalry thesis

    chivalry thesis 1950

VIDEO

  1. Is Chivalry 1 Worth Playing in 2024?

  2. @MaxGoodrich its training… for chivalry

  3. "A Cruel Angel's Thesis"

  4. Chivalry: Medieval Warfare (EN VIVO)

  5. CHIVALRY IS DEAD?: Rules of Modern Dating & Understanding Women "It's Complicated"

  6. Chivalry: Medieval Warfare 2D Lore Trailer

COMMENTS

  1. system was first noted by Thomas (1907) in his book, Sex and ...

    THE "CHIVALRY" PROPOSITION The thesis that women are subject to "chivalrous" treatment by the justice system was first noted by Thomas (1907) in his book, Sex and Society. ... (1950) expands this myth, viewing the woman criminal as sly and insidious and playing an instigatory role in criminal behavior. Konopka (1966), in her study of the ...

  2. Gender Differences in Crime

    The Chivalry Thesis: Examining Gender Differences in Crime. In 1950, sociologist Edmund Pollak coined the concept of the chivalry thesis —state that as most criminal justice agents like magistrates and judges and police officers are mainly men, they are also socialised to act in a chivalrous way towards women. While there is some evidence to support this thesis, a closer look at the research ...

  3. Gender Differences and Sentencing: A Critical Literature Review

    Chivalry, Selective Chivalry, and Paternalism Theory. This is an idea that was put forward by Otto Pollak (1950) to suggest that women within the criminal justice system are treated much more leniently than men due to the idea of chivalry towards women. Later Paternalism would be identified as something that frequently follows the chivalry aspect.

  4. The criminality of women.

    Man's self-deception about women seems to have been excessive with respect to crime. Criminal statistics which support the view of the lack of criminality in women are the least reliable of all statistics. For crimes to be reliably reported they must have three criteria, which woman's crimes lack: (1) be highly injurious to society, (2) be of public nature, (3) have the co-operation of the ...

  5. Is Chivalry Colour-Blind? Race-Class-Gender Articulation in the

    The recent multivariate analysis of sentencing reports and probation reports by Roger Hood makes the controversial claim that the 'chivalry thesis' that women are generally treated more favourably than men in the courts is applicable to both white women and black women.

  6. THe criminality of women : Otto Pollak : Free Download, Borrow, and

    1950 Collection inlibrary; printdisabled; internetarchivebooks Contributor Internet Archive Language English. Notes. Cut-off text on some pages due too tight binding. This book contains pen markings. Access-restricted-item true Addeddate 2022-10-29 10:01:16 Autocrop_version ..14_books-20220331-.2 ...

  7. The Criminality of Women. Otto Pollak

    menuDrawerCloseText menuDrawerOpenText Home. Subscribe/renew. Institutions; Individual subscriptions; Individual renewals; Recommend to your library; Purchase back issues; Browse issues

  8. PDF Gender in Crime News: A Case Study Test of the Chivalry Hypothesis

    Of the 2,281 crime stories identified in this census, a majority (73.5%) appeared in the main section of the paper. Around 26.5% of the crime stories were drawn from the region or local section ...

  9. Review of The criminality of women.

    Reviews the book, The criminality of women by Otto Pollak (1950). A study of some of the characteristics of female crime. Among the aspects discussed are the methods of crime commission, the extent and specificity of female crime, the personal characteristics of the offender, an analysis of female crimes against persons and property, and the influential biological and social factors. The ...

  10. Criminality of Women. By Otto Pollak. Philadelphia: University of

    Ronie Sheffield; The Criminality of Women. By Otto Pollak. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1950. 180 pp. $3.50, Social Forces, Volume 29, I

  11. Gender, race, and formal court decision-making outcomes: Chivalry

    The chivalry/paternalism hypotheses suggest that women are awarded leniency in sentencing as a result of their inherent biological weaknesses and consequently, their need to be protected and coddled both as offenders and as victims (Bishop and Frazier, 1984, Curry et al., 2004, Farnworth and Teske, 1995, Zatz, 2000). Gender conflict is derived ...

  12. PDF Dr Samantha Jeffries Dr Christine E.W. Bond

    system has a long history. Over 60 years ago, Pollak (1950) claimed that female offenders were the recipients of male notions of chivalry from a male-dominated criminal justice (Tjaden system and Tjaden 1981). However, the universality of chivalry was later questioned by the paternalism thesis, which suggested that

  13. (PDF) Chivalry and the Moderating Effect of Ambivalent Sexism

    Chivalry theories argue that such leniency is the result of paternalistic, benevolent attitudes toward women, in particular toward those who fulfill stereotypical female roles.

  14. Review of the Literature and Lineage of Chivalric Ideals

    One of the ideals in the longue durée of European societies was that of the chivalric code . Knights were to uphold a code of chivalry that championed honor, loyalty, and service to others regardless of rank or social position. Knighthood was earned, not given; and the high respect accorded to knights was likewise earned at regular intervals ...

  15. Statistics on Gender and Crime

    An outdated theory from the 1950s, related to chivalry thesis, comes from Otto Pollack who argued that women commit as many crimes as men but are better liars and therefore get away with their crimes. He argues that women have to learn deception in order to hide menstruation and fake orgasms. I imagine you can come up with your own evaluation ...

  16. "The Chivalry Thesis"- Otto Pollak (1950) by Amy Biggs on Prezi

    Otto Pollak (1950) Karen Matthews was jailed for 8 years in January 2009 for the kidnap of her 9 year old daughter Shannon, with her boyfirend's uncle Michael Donovan. Matthews drugged her daughter and hid her away for 24 days tied to a roof beam, in an attempt to make money by claiming rewards offered for the little girl's safe return.

  17. PDF The Chivalry Hypothesis & Filicide: Are There Categorical ...

    is known as the typicality thesis of the chivalry hypothesis. The female not only gets punished for committing a violent offense, but for failing to behave as a woman is expected (Farnworth & Teske, 1995). Differential discretion predicts that chivalry is used disproportionately during the beginning stages of criminal proceedings.

  18. Saving damsels, sentencing deviants and selective chivalry decisions

    Building on Edwards' chivalry theory, which proposes that protective attitudes towards women drive the leniency that they receive in conviction rates and sentencing, selective chivalry theory is postulated to account for non-traditional females in the legal system (Bickle & Peterson, 1991; O'Neil, 1999; Visher, 1983). Selective chivalry holds ...

  19. Chivalry Thesis

    Chivalry Thesis. When trying to explain crime statistics showing that men commit many more criminal acts than women, some sociologists suggest that these statistics do not reflect reality; rather that mostly-male law enforcement officers tend to attempt to protect women from the criminal justice system out of gentlemanliness. Show more.

  20. Chivalry Thesis

    Chivalry Thesis - Gender and Crime. Level: AS, A-Level, IB. Board: AQA, Edexcel, OCR, IB, Eduqas, WJEC. Last updated 30 Nov 2022. Share : The concept of chivalry thesis as an explanation of why crime statistics show men commit many more criminal acts than women is explained in this video.

  21. The chivalry thesis claims that women will be treated more leniently

    Using material from Item A and elsewhere, assess the value of 'chivalry thesis 'in understanding differences in crime. gfg. Pollak (1950) was of the opinion that police and magistrates tended to be more 'chivalrous' and 'lenient' towards female offenders, resulting in sentence disparities, and as a result, criminal statistics underestimate the amount of female offending.

  22. Gender and crime sociology Flashcards

    Gender and crime sociology. Chivalry thesis. Click the card to flip 👆. Pollak 1950 argues men have a protective attitude towards women thus the criminal justice system is more lenient - therefore this gives an invalid picture in statistics which exaggerates the extent of gender differences. Click the card to flip 👆. 1 / 23.