• Search Menu
  • Advance articles
  • Featured Content
  • Author Guidelines
  • Open Access
  • About The British Journal of Criminology
  • About the Centre for Crime and Justice Studies
  • Editorial Board
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Journals Career Network
  • Self-Archiving Policy
  • Dispatch Dates
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

Issue Cover

Article Contents

Introduction, literature review, methodology.

  • < Previous

Crime prevention, swarm intelligence and stigmergy: Understanding the mechanisms of social media-facilitated community crime prevention

  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data

Mark A Wood, Chrissy Thompson, Crime prevention, swarm intelligence and stigmergy: Understanding the mechanisms of social media-facilitated community crime prevention, The British Journal of Criminology , Volume 61, Issue 2, March 2021, Pages 414–433, https://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/azaa065

  • Permissions Icon Permissions

Social media are now utilized extensively by Neighbourhood Watch-style initiatives; however, the impact social media have on the practices and mechanisms of community crime prevention remains under-theorized. Drawing on our observations of an Australian-based community crime prevention group over two-and-a-half years, this article develops a grounded theory of the mechanisms underpinning the group’s social media-facilitated practices of responding to local crime. We find that social media-facilitated Neighbourhood Watch is shaped by two phenomena that have yet to receive sustained attention in crime prevention research. These are swarm intelligence—a form of self-organization wherein collectives process information to solve problems that members cannot solve individually—and stigmergy: work that stimulates further work. In explaining how swarm intelligence and stigmergy interact with several of the long-acknowledged mechanisms and issues associated with Neighbourhood Watch, we emphasize the importance of examining how the media context of community crime prevention groups shapes their practices, behaviour and (in)efficacy.

Hailing from a municipality in Victoria, Australia, Springsclare Crime Prevention is a community crime prevention group that has accumulated over 13,000 members since being established in 2016. Unlike traditional Neighbourhood Watch schemes, Springsclare Crime Prevention is run entirely through social media, using a semi-public Facebook group to quickly spread information about crimes in the area, crime prevention tips and other local concerns. As groups like Springsclare Crime Prevention demonstrate, mobile and social media represent potentially disruptive technologies for community crime prevention. Despite this, the potentially transformative impact of mobile and social media on community crime prevention has received relatively limited attention within criminology ( Akkermans and Vollaard 2015 ; Lub 2017 ; 2018 ; Mols and Pridmore 2019 ; Pridmore et al. 2019 ). In particular, few studies have conceptualized the mechanisms underlying social media-facilitated community crime prevention and how they interface with well-established mechanisms, such as natural surveillance and collective efficacy ( Cozens 2008 ; Sampson 2012 ). Better understanding this interface between old and new mechanisms provides much-needed knowledge on whether, when and how social media may be more productively integrated into community crime prevention initiatives.

To better understand this interface, we draw on observations of Springsclare Crime Prevention to develop a grounded theory of the mechanisms underpinning the group’s crime prevention practices. Through doing so, we argue that social media-facilitated Neighbourhood Watch initiatives can be better understood using two mechanisms that have yet to be applied to understanding community crime prevention: swarm intelligence and stigmergy. These mechanisms help us understand the distinctly self-organized nature of crime prevention behaviour that can be fostered through social media-facilitated Neighbourhood Watch initiatives, as well as their use of collective intelligence to respond to local crime (see Estellés-Arolas 2020 ). They also help us understand how social media can amplify some of the long-standing issues associated with Neighbourhood Watch programs, including their potential to increase fear of crime and exclusionary sentiments ( Rosenbaum 1987 ; Laycock and Tilley 1995 ).

First used to describe the collective behaviour of social insects ( Bonabeau and Meyer 2001 ), the notion of swarm intelligence represents a form of self-organization wherein ‘two or more individuals independently collect information that is processed through social interaction and provides a solution to a cognitive problem that is not available to single individuals’ ( Beni and Wang 1993 ; Krause et al. 2010 : 28). In this sense, swarm intelligence is intentional: it is put to use in solving problems faced by a group. In the animal world, one key problem addressed by swam intelligence is protecting groups against threats, primarily through what Lima (1995) has termed the ‘many eyes effect’ or ‘many eyes theory’. Put simply, the many eyes effect posits that larger groups of animals will detect predators and other threats sooner through information diffusion, with swarm intelligence facilitating escape by both the individual who initially detects the threat and the other members of the swarm. Such swarm intelligence can be underpinned by stigmergy: a mechanism of indirect coordination where ‘work … stimulates more work’ ( Lewis and Marsh 2016 : 1). Introduced by biologist Pierre-Paul Grassé (1959) to explain termite behaviour, the concept of stigmergy has since been examined as a ‘universal organizing principle’ ( Heylighen 2016a; , 2016b ) relevant to understanding issues as diverse as building design, Wikipedia editing and graffiti writing ( MacDowall 2014 ; see also Nieto-Gomez 2016 ; Caskey et al. 2018 ).

To demonstrate the stigmergic and swarm intelligence characteristics of community crime prevention facilitated through social media, our article unfolds as follows. In the following literature review section, we review research on the mechanisms underpinning, and issues associated with, Neighbourhood Watch-style community crime prevention groups. After explaining our critical realist grounded theory methodology, the three findings sections of our article then examine, in turn, how the concepts of swarm intelligence and stigmergy can be utilized to better understand the mechanisms underpinning social media-facilitated crime prevention on groups such as Springsclare Crime Prevention. Bringing the ‘many eyes’ theory of swarm intelligence into conversation with Jacobs’ (1961) notion of ‘eyes on the street’, we examine the impact social and mobile media may have on community crime prevention initiatives, given their influence on the nature, reach and speed of information diffusion among members of community groups. While, on the one hand, social and mobile media enable many ‘eyes on the street’ to meet on the screen, this can generate hyperconsciousness of crime that is conducive to ‘suspicion creep’—a widening of the parameters of suspicion among members of a group. As we detail in the final findings section on stigmergy, this suspicion creep is amplified by a reliance on first-order crime prevention measures: activities that primarily aim to reduce crime. When self-organized through stigmergic systems, direct crime prevention activities, such as neighbourhood patrols, have a tendency toward positive feedback, for they lack feasible negative feedback mechanisms. This issue is less likely to affect second-order crime prevention measures: activities that prevent crime as an indirect by-product of pursuing other social aims. Such crime prevention initiatives have greater scope to be stigmergic but not stigmatic—i.e. to enjoy the benefits of stigmergic self-organization without increasing exclusionary and hypervigilant sentiments.

Neighbourhood Watch-style community crime prevention groups have been the subject of extensive research, with criminologists examining their aims, preventative efficacy, mechanisms and pitfalls (see Rosenbaum 1987 ; Bennett 1990 ). In this research, scholars have proposed numerous mechanisms to explain why Neighbourhood Watch programs might lower crime rates. These include that Neighbourhood Watch programs may represent a visible deterrent ( Laycock and Tilley 1995 ; Newburn 2007 ; see Jacobs 1961 ); that they may limit opportunities for crime to occur through informing residents about security ( Cirel et al. 1977 ; Bennett 1990 ) and that they may indirectly affect crime rates through providing the police with information about suspicious activities ( Clarke and Hough 1984 ; Garofalo and McLeod 1989 ). Furthermore, scholars have argued that, Neighbourhood Watch groups may lower crime rates through enforcing standards of desired behaviour and engaging in other direct interventions ( Rosenbaum 1987 ; Lub 2017 ). Neighbourhood Watch groups may, in other words, encourage greater informal social control among residents ( Rosenbaum 1987 ; Sampson 2012 ), often through increasing the collective efficacy of communities, i.e. ‘social cohesion among neighbors combined with their willingness to intervene on behalf of the common good’ ( Sampson et al. 1997 : 918; Sampson 2012 ). Collective efficacy has been employed extensively in the fields of crime prevention and social ecology, with empirical studies finding strong empirical support for collective efficacy as a mechanism of informal social control ( Pratt and Cullen 2005 ; Sampson 2012 ).

These theoretical foundations of Neighbourhood Watch have, however, been problematized by studies examining the limitations and potential pitfalls of such programs. Much of this literature notes that Neighbourhood Watch programs can only be successful when a very specific set of conditions are met ( Palumbo et al. 1997 ). Thus, whilst evidence suggests that Neighbourhood Watch programs can have a modest effect in reducing crime ( Garofalo and McLeod 1989 ; Bennett et al. 2008 ), such programs are not viable for all neighbourhoods. Research has, e.g., consistently indicated that Neighbourhood Watch programs are considerably more common in relatively affluent and comparatively low-crime areas ( Husain 1988 ; Hope 1995 ; Laycock and Tilley 1995 ; Brunton-Smith and Bullock 2019 ) and face considerable challenges in disadvantaged and high-crime neighbourhoods. For these reasons, Rosenbaum (1987 : 127) also questions several of the core foundations of the Neighbourhood Watch ‘Implant Hypothesis’, i.e. that ‘citizen participation, and eventually informal social control mechanisms, can be “implanted in neighbourhoods where they do not currently exist”’. Similarly, important questions have also been raised about whether Neighbourhood Watch-associated practices, such as Block Watches, are self-sustaining (see Rosenbaum 1987 ; Hope 1995 ). As Skogan (1988 : 49) notes, ‘concern about crime simply does not provide a basis for sustained individual participation’, with many evaluations of community crime prevention initiatives indicating that participants’ interest in the initiative waned over time ( Lindsay and McGillis 1986 ; Garofalo and McLeod 1989 ).

Finally, scholars have noted that, contrary to Neighbourhood Watch’s aims of increasing social cohesion and lowering residents’ fear of crime, such schemes can serve to amplify exclusionary sentiments ( Laycock and Tilley 1995 ; Crawford 1997 ; 1999 ) and increase residents’ fear of crime ( Rosenbaum 1987 ; Rosenbaum and Heath 1990 ). Research has indicated that fear of crime is lower in neighbourhoods where residents feel that they have a greater degree of informal control over behaviour in the area ( Skogan and Maxfield 1981 ; Greenberg et al. 1982 ). However, several studies have indicated that Neighbourhood Watch programs have the potential to increase residents’ fear of crime through increasing their awareness of local crime ( Rosenbaum and Heath 1990 ). Indeed, as Rosenbaum (1987 : 128) notes, mechanisms intended to decrease fear of crime, such as residential meetings, may actually serve as forums that ‘heighten residents’ fears and prejudices’.

As noted earlier, few studies have examined community crime prevention groups on Facebook, and only a number of studies have examined similar groups established on WhatsApp: an instant messaging app now owned by Facebook ( Dixon 2017 ; Lub 2018 ; Pridmore et al. 2019 ). Much of this literature examines the implications social media have had in making it easier to report crimes or ‘suspicious’ behaviour to members of a community; as Lub (2017 : 136) notes, ‘today, reporting to the police or fellow residents about suspicious behaviour or persons is just a phone swipe or a mouse click away, lowering the threshold for reporting “suspicious” acts or persons’. This lowering of the threshold for reporting crime and ‘suspicious’ behaviour can lead to both positive and negative outcomes ( Lub 2017 ). Positively, it may increase group members’ ability to engage in situational control. Though efficacy-focussed studies into this issue remain scant, Akkermans and Vollaard’s (2015) research indicates that WhatsApp neighbourhood crime prevention groups in the Dutch city of Tilburg contributed to a decline in burglaries within some neighbourhoods. Negatively, it may calcify ingroup/outgroup divisions and intensify the ethnic profiling and stigmatization of marginalized populations. Moreover, as Mols and Pridmore (2019 : 280) indicate, the use of WhatsApp for the purpose of crime prevention can lead to the normalization of surveillance and vigilance among members of the community—‘a neighbourhood where lateral surveillance becomes the status quo’.

Background: Springsclare Crime Prevention

Located in a suburb of Melbourne and home to over 100,000 people, the pseudonymous Springsclare is one of several local government areas in Victoria. Springsclare is a relatively affluent municipality with a low crime rate and bears many of the demographic hallmarks of areas where community crime prevention groups are more likely to emerge (see Skogan 1988 ; Hope 1995 : 44). Australian Bureau of Statistics census data indicates that Springsclare has higher than Australian average rates of residents who are married, own their own homes, have children in a nuclear family and have a university level of educational attainment. Following an increase of burglaries in the area and intense media coverage of crime rates within Victoria, the municipality came to host one of the largest Facebook crime prevention groups within the state. As noted above, Springsclare Crime Prevention was established and operates primarily through social media. With the exception of regular in-person meetings between the small number of page administrators who moderate posts and comments on the page, group members do not meet regularly face-to-face. Rather, the overwhelming majority of Springsclare Crime Prevention-related activity is self-organized through the Facebook page.

We purposively selected Springsclare Crime Prevention as our case study as it represented a relatively large semi-public Facebook group. Our choice of case study was guided by considerations pertaining to the ethics of ‘online’ observation. The ethics of ‘online’ observation are hotly debated, in particular the practice of covertly observing or ‘lurking’ on Internet groups and pages ( Reilly and Trevisan 2016 ). As Willis (2019) argues, in spaces where users would reasonably expect their posts to be viewed by a large number of individuals, we can consider such spaces semi-public and view research examining such spaces as comparable to observational research in a public space. For these reasons, we, like other scholars concerned with digital communities ( Willis 2019 ), eschewed private groups and selected a semi-public case study group, i.e. a group that allows anyone to join without a vetting process. Furthermore, we sought, and were granted, ethical approval by our university’s research ethics committee to conduct our study of Springsclare Crime Prevention.

To protect the identity of group members, we have, first, anonymized all posts we quote directly, second, assigned the group itself a pseudonym and, third, made slight alterations to the syntax of direct quotes from members to prevent the identification of their authors through Internet searches. Following the long tradition of providing pseudonyms for research locations ( Crow and Maclean 2000 ), we have elected not to identify the name of the municipality to protect the identity of its members and to avoid subjecting the crime prevention group to undue external attention. Using a random name generator, we allocated the municipality the pseudonym Springsclare and the Facebook group the pseudonym Springsclare Crime Prevention.

Data collection and analysis

To investigate the mechanisms underpinning Springsclare Crime Prevention, we adopted a critical realist grounded theory methodology ( Oliver 2011 ) that drew on naturalistic observations of the group between August 2016 and February 2019. In practice, our methodology combined a critical realist ontology and its attendant use of retroduction ( Bhaskar 1986 ) with the well-established grounded theory conventions of coding, memo writing and theoretical sampling ( Glaser and Strauss 1967 ).

In August 2016, we both joined Springsclare Crime Prevention. Upon joining, we regularly began receiving updates from the group, which we collected and coded using the procedure detailed below. To avoid our presence affecting the behaviour of group members (see Webb et al. 1966 ), we took an unobtrusive approach to our ‘digital fieldwork’ (see Hine 2015 ) and did not interact with the members of Springsclare Crime Prevention. However, rather than retroactively ‘scraping’ archived data from Springsclare Crime Prevention, we analysed all material posted to the page as it was posted. Collecting and analysing our data in this fashion enabled us as to experience Springsclare Crime Prevention as members of the group would; during some weeks, we received and analysed few comments and updates from group members, whilst, in others, we analysed a considerable quantity of user-generated content. In this manner, we observed and analysed Springsclare Crime Prevention daily between August 2016 and February 2019. Textual and visual media content posted to Springsclare Crime Prevention—including the ‘like’ and ‘share’ metrics of posts and comments—were downloaded and analysed employing a grounded theory procedure, in which codes were assigned to data excepts through the ‘constant comparison’ of new to existing findings ( Glaser and Strauss 1967 : 114). In line with this grounded theory methodology ( Glaser and Strauss 1967 ), incidents were our unit of analysis. In our study, such incidents were comprised of (1) an individual post in the group and (2) any comments, shares and ‘reaction’ responses to the post by other group members or the original posting member. In examining these incidents to understand the mechanisms of Facebook-facilitated community crime prevention, we analysed three of their key characteristics:

(1) Group members’ practices on Springsclare Crime Prevention: how, if at all, were individuals using Springslare Crime Prevention to engage in crime prevention?

(2) Group members’ interactions through Springsclare Crime Prevention: how did group members coordinate crime prevention activities and interact with other members?

(3)  The architecture of Facebook and Springslare Crime Prevention: how did the architecture of the Springslare Crime Prevention Facebook group shape members’ practices and interactions?

In line with a critical realist grounded theory approach, our analysis was primarily retroductive, asking ‘what must be true for this to be the case?’ before abstracting potential causal mechanisms underpinning Springsclare Crime Prevention and seeking empirical evidence for these abstractions ( Bhaskar 1986 ; Oliver 2011 ). As Oliver (2011 : 379–80) details, critical realism’s emphasis on retroduction is highly compatible with grounded theory’s increasing emphasis on abduction over induction ( Charmaz 2006 ; Reichertz 2007 ). Indeed, as Oliver (2011 : 380) puts it, ‘retroduction is simply abduction with a specific question in mind’. In line with an abductive/retroductive approach, we recognised that coding is always informed by the researchers’ pre-existing knowledge, but that concepts have to ‘earn their way’ into the analysis through being evidenced in the data ( Oliver 2011 : 381). To increase the reliability of our findings, we analysed data independently before discussing and resolving any discrepancies in our analysis by consensus ( Syed and Nelson 2015 ). We concluded our observations of the group in February 2019 after deciding that we had reached theoretical saturation: the point in a critical realist grounded theory project where the theory generated by the study ‘has, for the time being, greater explanatory power than its rivals’ ( Oliver 2011 : 379).

The swarm intelligence of social media-facilitated crime prevention

In this section, we detail, first, the applicability of swarm intelligence as a mechanism for understanding collective behaviour on Facebook community crime prevention pages and, second, several of the implications swarm intelligence holds for crime prevention practice. As detailed earlier, when applied to human collectives, swarm intelligence has been defined by Krause et al. (2010 : 28) as the phenomenon whereby ‘two or more individuals independently collect information that is processed through social interaction and provides a solution to a cognitive problem that is not available to single individuals’. Underlying this definition of swarm intelligence are several necessary conditions that must be met for collective behaviour to be considered an example of the phenomenon. These conditions are: 1) collective intelligence; 2) self-organised behaviour and 3) a medium that allows members to coordinate in real-time . Moreover, it is a necessary condition that the collective intelligence emerges out of self - organized behaviour, which is fostered by this medium for real-time coordination and collaboration. Each of these conditions, we argue, are met by the behaviour we observed on Springsclare Crime Prevention.

First, to be considered swarm intelligence, collective behaviour must be driven by the collective intelligence of a group: i.e. intelligence that emerges out of the collaboration of individuals in the collective but which is irreducible to the individual collective members’ intelligence ( Lévy 1997 ). Springsclare Crime Prevention meets this condition for swarm intelligence as the collaborative and self-coordinated behaviour of members hinges on intelligence that is irreducible to that of individual group members. Indeed, Springsclare Crime Prevention’s potential as a crime prevention platform, we argue, lies primarily in its fostering of collective intelligence, wherein intelligence between actors enables novel forms of coordination, cooperation, deliberation and collaboration in responding to local crime. Here, Springsclare Crime Prevention’s potential as a crime prevention platform arises not simply through the aggregation of information about crime but through facilitating real-time collective behaviour in responding to this information.

Second, to be considered swarm intelligence, this collective intelligence must arise out of behaviour that is self-organized —‘organization without an organizer’ ( Garnier et al. 2007 : 4). As we detail when we turn to the mechanism of stigmergy on Springsclare Crime Prevention, members’ behaviour in the group was overwhelmingly self-organized. Members of Springsclare Crime Prevention do not issue commands to other members when they report a crime or a suspicious occurrence. Nonetheless, by reporting such behaviour, they may set off a chain of self-organized activity among other users, whose own additional behaviour may lead to real-time interventions that would not have occurred otherwise.

Third, for swarm intelligence to emerge between humans, a medium is required that allows members of a collective to coordinate in real-time ( Lévy 1997 ). Human swarm intelligence is, therefore, an emergent property of three combined elements: 1) the individuals who make up the swarm; 2) the technological infrastructure that allows members to self-coordinate in real-time and 3) the information shared between actors. Any change in one of these three elements will, therefore, shape the behaviour and outcomes of the swarm. As we detail further below, this medium for real-time coordination between members has the potential to fundamentally change the temporality of community-based crime prevention. On Springsclare Crime Prevention, it is the speed at which information about a crime can be transmitted between geographically dispersed local actors that sets the group apart from traditional Neighbourhood Watch groups, where such information is shared to members primarily at scheduled meetings and via newsletters ( Hope 1995 ; Wickes 2010 ). If a property crime, such as the theft of a vehicle, is reported by one member of the group, a large number of individuals in the area can quickly be alerted to the offence and be on the lookout for individuals matching the description of ‘suspects’. In September 2016, a user posted the following:

I know most of you are asleep right now, but I just had a car pull up in the middle of the street next to my work car. A guy got out and was looking through my canopy with a small torch/phone light. I just so happened to be on my way to bed when I heard the car. I quickly flicked on the outside light, unlocked the front door and made my way out (in just my jocks mind you) and the guy quickly jumped back in and took off...If anyone has cameras in the area that can shed some light on the car’s details, I’m sure the police will be grateful.

This post elicited over 100 reactions from members and 28 comments, including immediate responses from members asking what time the attempted burglary occurred so that neighbours on the street could check their own CCTV footage. Here, we see a clear example of members combining and processing information about potential criminal activity in their area in real-time. In the discussion between members on this post, information is exchanged:

If you end up identifying the vehicle let me know. Did it look like a station wagon by any chance? About 6 months ago I was driving up a side street … around 4am. As I was driving through the street I had to come to a complete stop because there was a SILVER HOLDEN STATION WAGON parked in the middle of the road I live on [street name] and the house next door got his work car broken into a few nights ago and stole all his tools. It may be the same person

In the following sections, we examine, in turn, two of the key implications of this swarm intelligence: the creation of a ‘many eyes’ effect in detecting and responding to crime; and the fostering of ‘suspicion creep’—a widening of what group members perceived to be suspicious behaviour.

Many eyes on the street

In studies of threat detection and avoidance among animal swarms, ethnologists have demonstrated that the speed and accuracy of group decisions increase with group size ( Ward et al. 2011 ). Ward et al. (2011) put such increases down to the ‘self-organized division of vigilance’ within groups, drawing on a variant of the ‘many eyes’ theory. In explaining threat detection and avoidance in animal swarms, the ‘many eyes’ theory considers two key factors: the size of a group and the speed at which information is transmitted within the group. As Ward et al. (2011) explain, ‘the “many eyes” theory predicts that predators will be detected sooner by larger groups, increasing the probability of escape for the individual that first detects them, as well as for other group members as the information is transmitted across the group’.

With the rise of social media-facilitated Neighbourhood Watch groups, the ‘many eyes’ theory can be readily translated into the domain of crime prevention—perhaps, unsurprising given the frequency with which the figure of citizen ‘eyes’ is referred to in crime prevention literature. As Smith et al. (2002 : 83) note, Neighbourhood Watch is often described as extra ‘eyes and ears’ for the police. More substantially, the notion of ‘eyes on the street’ figures centrally within Jacobs’ (1961) explanation of natural surveillance—a core technique employed in Neighbourhood Watch schemes. Insofar as it has been taken up within Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design literature, Jacob’s notion of promoting ‘eyes on the street’ has focussed on developing physical spaces that better facilitate natural surveillance ( Cozens 2008 ; Cozens and Davies 2013 ). Jacob’s (1961) notion of ‘eyes on the street’, therefore, frames ‘eyes’ as potential guardians of spaces that may, when a critical mass is achieved, serve to discourage certain forms of offending within well-surveilled areas ( Cozens 2008 ).

Building on Jacob’s emphasis on ‘eyes on the street’, we suggest that there is an additional need to consider how these ‘eyes’ acquire, combine and process the information that they see (and hear). Moreover, we suggest that changes in information communications technology create a renewed need to consider the morphology of information diffusion in crime prevention groups. Together, social and mobile media introduce three key changes to the ‘media context’ ( Hampton 2010 : 1129) of community crime prevention groups that can fundamentally disrupt the nature, reach and speed of information diffusion within them.

First, social media facilitates a pattern of many-to-many information diffusion that facilitates swarm intelligence. Swarms, as Thacker (2004) explains, are characterized by leaderless decentralized coordination and, for this reason, swarm intelligence is occasionally said to generate a hive mind: forms of collective intelligence or consciousness, where intelligence is located between actors ( Rosenberg and Baltaxe 2016 ). By changing the pattern of information sharing from a one-to-many format to a many-to-many real-time format, such crime prevention pages may produce human swarms that are collectively aware of changes in information regarding a crime and are able to (self-)coordinate in responding to it. Second, social media can greatly increase the potential reach of information provided by individual group members. Members of crime prevention groups are able to self-broadcast information about crime and suspicious behaviour to a large quantity of individuals who, informed about an incident, may furnish the group with additional information. In line with the ‘many eyes’ theory, this increase in the size of the crime prevention group increases the probability that one member of the group will detect an incident and, subsequently, relay information regarding it to the remaining members of the group. Finally, social and mobile media increase the speed of information diffusion within citizen crime prevention groups. In line with the ‘many eyes’ theory, this increase in the speed of information transmission enables users to respond to incidents ‘in real-time’, providing additional clarifying information that not only enables members of the ‘swarm’ to avoid unfolding risks but also to obtain additional information that may assist criminal investigations. Bringing Jacob’s notion of ‘eyes on the street’ into dialogue with the ethnological ‘many eyes’ theory, we might understand social media crime prevention groups as producing many eyes on the street that meet on the screen.

Suspicion creep

As a result of this swarm intelligence, members of Facebook crime prevention groups may receive a more informed picture of crime within their area than they would by consuming news media. However, this more accurate picture of crime can come at a cost. As members became more conscious of local crime, receiving sometimes multiple reports of crimes within their area, many also became hypervigilant, distrustful and fearful—an issue long associated with Neighbourhood Watch programs (see Rosenbaum 1987 ; Rosenbaum and Heath 1990 ) but amplified by the real-time updates and lowered threshold for reporting ‘“suspicious” acts or persons’ afforded by social media. Early in 2018, one member of Springsclare Crime Prevention articulated this sentiment explicitly, before ‘signing out of’ (leaving) the group:

The Age reported today that Victoria is rated one of the safest places yet Victorians are the most scared. This is not a Crime Prevention page it’s a crime reporting page and it’s making me paranoid. This type of page just encourages people to worry about what could happen but may not happen ... not healthy. Signing out of living in fear.

This post, which was deleted within a day by administrators, highlights several shortcomings of Facebook-facilitated crime prevention groups: their propensity to increase members’ fear of crime, their tendency to only report rather than actively prevent crime and, subsequently, their failure to systematically engage in actual crime prevention behaviours.

In addition to these shortcomings, swarm intelligence contributed to what we term suspicion creep . In suspicion creep, the forms of behaviour perceived to be suspicious by group members expands as individual members report witnessing new behaviours that they perceived to be suspicious. These behaviours are then recoded by sections of the group as suspicious, leading to an increased repertoire of behaviours deemed suspicious and, by corollary, reportable. Though some of the behaviours reported by members are undoubtedly crime-related, many are not. Many of the posts we analysed, e.g., concerned groups of young people doing nothing more than occupying public space. Others concerned individuals wearing hoodies at night, Pokémon GO players or individuals taking pictures of flowers in an individual’s garden. Suspicion creep can, therefore, reinforce an ‘us vs them’ mentality, an assertion of community and national borders as in need of protection from outsiders—a key issue long identified with Neighbourhood Watch ( Laycock and Tilley 1995 ; Crawford 1997 ; 1999 ) but, in this case, amplified by the many-to-many and real-time communication afforded by social media.

Social media-facilitated crime prevention as stigmergy

One of the key differences between groups such as Springsclare Crime Prevention and traditional Neighbourhood Watch schemes lies in the organizational mechanisms they employ. Whereas Neighbourhood Watch schemes tend to plan organized patrols and other timetabled activities centrally, Springsclare Crime Prevention and similar social media groups promote a form of community crime prevention that is overwhelmingly self-organized . Preventative actions, in other words, emerge ‘from local interactions, without any centralised control directing the activity’ ( Heylighen 2016a : 14). As one moderator wrote, in a post celebrating the group reaching 5000 members:

This page was designed for the best interest of our beloved shire … but more importantly the community who make it tick. It’s fantastic to see the interaction, contribution, education and awareness displayed by all.… keep up the great work everyone and let’s try and look after one another. Springsclare needs this, no wait Springsclare deserves this symbiotic relationship between its people and the shire for a healthy and prosperous future.

In Springsclare Crime Prevention, we see a shift from scheduled preventative activity to stigmergic preventative activity: crime prevention practices that are enacted in response to traces left in the medium and, in being enacted, themselves leave a trace that stimulates further preventative action from others. Heylighen (2016a : 5) defines stigmergy as ‘an indirect, mediated mechanism of coordination between actions, in which the trace of an action left on a medium stimulates the performance of a subsequent action’. As Heylighen (2016a) notes, stigmergy describes a feedback loop in which marks produced in an action incite further action of the same kind, which, in turn, produces a further action-inciting mark (see Figure 1 ). Importantly, stigmergy does not describe a deterministic process. For the most part, marks that incite a particular action represent neither a necessary nor sufficient condition for that action to occur ( Heylighen 2016a ).

Heylighen’s (2016a) stigmergic feedback loop model.

Heylighen’s (2016a) stigmergic feedback loop model.

To use the terms of Heylighen’s (2016a) model of stigmergy, social media introduces a medium for action-inciting traces to be registered. Though there exist a variety of examples of human stigmergy that are not facilitated by digital technologies, the digital traces that characterize many computational media can offer a fertile environment for stigmergy. On Springsclare Crime Prevention, stigmergic crime prevention is a direct product of the visible digital footprint left by members’ actions on the page, which serves to stimulate further crime preventative actions by members. In one example from August 2016, a moderator posted the following with a screenshot of Google Maps showing their current location:

Just patrolled surrounding areas, nothing suspicious. Feel free to patrol your surrounding area and upload a screenshot so everyone know when and where you patrolled.

Another member responded in the comments that they had ‘also patrolled around’, while another raised alarm over a car driving around the streets:

Not sure if one of us, but a beige Honda CR-V was driving in and out of streets inside Primrose drive just after 6am this morning. Didn’t get plate as it was pretty dark.

Here, we can see how the digital footprint of the original post triggered further crime prevention actions by other members. For this reason, social media-facilitated crime prevention represents a form of marker-based stigmergy in that the marker (social media posts) is produced indirectly by the action (engaging in lateral surveillance). In other words, where engaging with ‘analog’ crime prevention media, such as Neighbourhood Watch pamphlets (see Wickes 2010 ), does not generate a trace that stimulates further trace-generating action, engaging with the digital equivalents of such material does. This has several implications that have a bearing on members’ participation in group practices—a longstanding issue faced by community crime prevention initiatives ( Rosenbaum 1987 ; Skogan 1988 ). Most notably, owing to their stigmergic character, social media-facilitated crime prevention techniques do not require planning or anticipation from different members, nor do they require centralized control or supervision. This lends the crime prevention practices of such groups a high degree of flexibility as members’ cooperative actions do not need to be coordinated or task allocated, nor do members even need to be mutually aware of one another’s actions ( Zamfirescu and Filip 2010 ; Heylighen 2016b ).

However, on account of its propensity for positive feedback loops ( Heylighen 2016b ), stigmergy is a risky tool for crime prevention practice. As Dipple (2015 : 3) explains, feedback mechanisms are crucial to stigmergy:

Stigmergy creates equilibrium between positive and negative feedback within the system. Positive feedback is provided by agents contributing to a signal and results in the signal strength of a trail increasing as more agents contribute. This promotes rapid and successful task completion. In opposition to this, the environment provides a negative feedback that will diminish the contributions making up the signal as they become unused and irrelevant. Signal decay ensures old information dissipates as it becomes redundant.

In stigmergy, goal attainment provides atrophic negative feedback that moderates amplification generated by positive feedback loops. This introduces a key problem for Springsclare Crime Prevention and similar groups. Namely, individual tasks on Springsclare Crime Prevention may be completed by identifying individuals or establishing that ‘suspicious’ behaviour represents a false alarm. The completion of such individual tasks, however, does not necessarily bring into play negative feedback, for their very completion leaves a residuum of suspicion and vigilance directed at the overarching goal of creating a ‘crime free’ neighbourhood. In August 2016, a moderator acknowledged this positive feedback loop, noting that ‘some members are afraid. Scared and anxious about incidences happening a little too frequently, that they have told me they left the page’. As we witnessed through following Springsclare Crime Prevention, such stigmergic feedback loops were readily apparent as a driving force behind the amplification of surveillance, vigilance and fear on the platform. Members of the group came to ‘see’ crime everywhere, as they received constant reminders that crime was occurring near them, and exhortations to engage in lateral surveillance. As one member who joined the group soon after its establishment in July 2016 stated:

Reading through these posts has now scared me half to death and I have locked my doors! Up to now I have been very slack with locking etc. but I might now be a bit more careful. I don’t want to become paranoid though.

Another user lamented how the popular augmented reality smartphone game Pokémon GO had resulted in more people walking and driving around the neighbourhood:

How can we know if people/cars are suspicious or Pokemon GO hunters? Maybe we all need to download ‘Pokemon GO’ and check for Pokemon in the street when we see something suss

This last example is particularly noteworthy in demonstrating how new mobile technologies—in this case, augmented reality smartphone applications—can disrupt expected uses of public space. The temporary flood of people in public space eagerly hunting Pokémon on their phone application, utilizing public space and coming together to participate in a collective activity is ostensibly one of the key aims of Springsclare Crime Prevention, i.e. to bring the community together. However, due to the positive feedback loop produced on this page, the presence of Pokémon GO players sparked disquiet and suspicion such that locals were encouraged to download the Pokémon GO app to better distinguish ‘suspicious’ behaviour from the Pokémon hunting behaviour associated with the augmented reality game.

This propensity for amplifying vigilance indicates that stigmergy may be a productive force for crime prevention when the preventative efficacy of an action is an indirect by-product of the stigmergic behaviour rather than the direct aim of that behaviour. Here, we can productively distinguish between first-order and second-order forms of crime prevention. First-order crime prevention represents practices and initiatives aimed at reducing crime. Examples of first-order crime prevention include neighbourhood watch patrols and sharing crime prevention tips. Second-order crime prevention, by contrast, represents practices and initiatives that prevent crime as a byproduct of pursuing other aims. Here, we can include initiatives that foster community cohesion, which consequently increases informal social control and collective efficacy.

In being narrowly conceived as a crime prevention page, Springsclare Crime Prevention did little to foster such forms of second-order crime prevention; its remit was practices aimed directly at preventing crime. As demonstrated by the group, this narrow remit can be circumvented through branding not as a crime prevention page specifically but as a broader local community group concerned with a range of issues affecting an area. Indeed, the utility of this approach has long been recognized by researchers examining crime prevention. As Bursik and Grasmick (1999 : 154) note, ‘crime prevention programs have the greatest likelihood of success if they are integrated into the activities of more general, multi-issue neighbourhood organizations’. Such an approach is precisely what occurred with Springsclare Crime Prevention within the group’s second year, when it rebranded as Springsclare Community Hub. Upon rebranding, the group flourished as a platform where residents of Springsclare assisted one another in a variety of tasks, from offering advice on service providers in the area to locating lost animals. Such actions still benefit from the stigmergic, marker-based mechanism of self-organization provided by Facebook groups; members’ trace-making actions are stimulated by traces left by other members, which, in turn, stimulate further trace-making actions. But unlike first-order crime prevention, they do not simultaneously amplify hypervigilance and fear of crime among group members. The second-order crime prevention they allow for stokes a very different affective register among members, for the positive feedback of members’ contributions is put in check by the attainment of small-scale goals that do not overtly pertain to crime prevention. The running newsfeed is now more balanced with just as many (if not more) news stories about good deeds, found pets, successful charity drives and good Samaritans handing in lost belongings:

Tommy has come home, thank you for everyone’s help!! – Feb 2018 I would like to thank the lovely lady and her son who paid for my parent’s groceries at the supermarket at The Bentley Square. What a totally inspiring and amazing thing you did today. My Parents (although embarrassed) were so chuffed with your act of kindness. As my Grandmother used to say ‘Your blood’s worth bottling’. Good things will come your way, thank you xxx – June 2018

Coupling traditional Neighbourhood Watch crime prevention techniques with technologies that facilitate human swarming and stigmergic self-organization can allow groups to more effectively harness the four key functions of swarm organization: coordination, cooperation, collaboration and deliberation ( Garnier et al. 2007 ). Furthermore, through generating marker-creating action that stimulates the performance of subsequent marker-generating action, technologies that promote stigmergic self-organization have the potential to address one of the long-standing issues identified in Neighbourhood Watch groups: their struggle to sustain participation among members over time ( Lindsay and McGillis 1986 ; Rosenbaum 1987 ; Skogan 1988 ; Garofalo and McLeod 1989 ). As Hope (1995 : 49) notes, ‘the longevity of neighborhood watch schemes may have most to do with their not requiring much of participants in practice’. Stigmergic crime prevention can serve to meet this crucial condition for longevity through shifting from programmed interventions—such as scheduled block watches requiring ongoing commitment from members—to self-organized interventions that respond to markers left by members.

Though human stigmergy is far from a deterministic process, stigmergic markers can serve to ‘nudge’ members to engage in preventative practices ( Thaler and Sunstein 2009 ). These practices, in turn, may generate positive feedback loops of self-organized first- and second-order preventative behaviour that responds to members’ concerns as they arise. In doing so, groups built around swarm intelligence and stigmergic second-order crime prevention have the potential to increase the collective efficacy of communities. Indeed, as the findings of other studies indicate, networked digital technologies have the potential to enhance local community by increasing the social capital of neighbourhoods and the community attachment of their members ( Stern and Adams 2010 ). For these reasons, Hampton (2010 : 1129) concludes that ‘media play an integral role in collective efficacy’ and that current theories of neighbourhood effects need to be revised to better account for the role of ‘media context’ ( Hampton 2010 : 1129). Harnessed in the service of second-order crime prevention, then, stigmergy may have the potential to increase the collective efficacy of communities. Harnessed in the service of first-order crime prevention initiatives, stigmergy has the potential to increase fear among residents and further stigmatize those at risk of offending. The difficulty, therefore, lies in generating crime prevention programs that are stigmergic but not stigmatic.

As our analysis demonstrates, the swarm intelligence of Facebook crime prevention groups can generate hyperconsciousness of crime among members. Viewed purely from an instrumental perspective, this hyperconsciousness may increase the efficacy of local crime prevention techniques that thrive on information sharing. However, taking a broader view of what constitutes effective crime prevention, this hyperconsciousness may amplify members’ fear of crime, foster punitive sentiments and lead to ‘suspicion creep’—a widening of the parameters of suspicion, wherein increasingly innocuous acts are viewed as suspect. This widening of the parameters of suspicion is, furthermore, a product of the stigmergic self-coordination that operates on community crime prevention pages such as Springsclare Crime Prevention. In explaining the stigmergic dimensions of such pages, we have advanced six arguments:

(1)  Crime prevention on Facebook community crime prevention groups is stigmergic as it leaves markers in a medium that, in turn, stimulates further marker-generating activity.

(2) Stigmergic organization has different implications for first-order community crime prevention activities and second-order community crime prevention activities.

(3)  When self-organized through stigmergic systems, first-order crime prevention activities, such as neighbourhood patrols, have a tendency towards positive feedback. This is because such forms of direct crime prevention often lack a feasible negative feedback mechanism indicating that such activities have achieved their goals.

(4) A key consequence of this tendency towards positive feedback is that stigmergic first-order crime prevention can lead to (a) an amplification of members’ fear of crime and vigilance and (b) ‘suspicion creep’.

(5) This consequence does not similarly affect second-order community crime prevention activities. When self-organized through stigmergic systems, second-order crime prevention activities can have a tendency towards equilibrium, with the positive feedback of members’ contributions checked by the attainment of small-scale goals.

(6)  Whilst retaining the benefits of stigmergic self-coordination, second-order crime prevention techniques do not carry the previously detailed negative consequences stemming from the application of stigmergic self-coordination to first-order crime prevention activities. Such crime prevention initiatives are more likely to be stigmergic but not stigmatic and, consequently, have the potential to reduce crime through facilitating certain forms of community cohesion and collective efficacy.

In examining the stigmergic properties of social media-facilitated crime prevention, it is worth briefly examining how the concept of stigmergy connects with and departs from other key concepts that criminologists have drawn upon to understand human interaction effects, such as interaction ritual chains (see Collins 2004 ; Rossner 2011 ), emotional and social contagion ( Fagan et al. 2007 ) and cascades ( Braithwaite 2020 ). First, stigmergy as a coordination mechanism can be distinguished from Collins’ (2004) influential concept of interaction ritual chains in that, whereas interaction rituals operate through physically co-present face-to-face encounters ( Collins 2004 : 19; Henry 2020 ), stigmergic cooperation operates without direct encounters; communication between members is asynchronous and is the product of traces one actor leaves in a medium. However, like interaction rituals, stigmergic behaviour can produce ‘emotional energy’ among actors—indeed, such emotional energy can be key to stigmergic mechanisms ‘stimulating’ indirect cooperation between individuals.

For this reason, certain forms of stigmergy also bear an ostensible similarity to emotional and social contagion: the transmission of behaviours—or, in the case of emotional contagion, emotions—through ‘contact, communication, and imitation’ ( Fagan et al. 2007 ). However, there are again key distinctions between these concepts, as well as Braithwaite’s 2020 : 138) similar notion of cascades, which emphasizes that behaviour diffuses through emulation and that ‘All contagions are cascades and contagions are the most important kinds of cascades’. The most notable of these is that, unlike cascades and social and emotional contagion, stigmergy does not occur through a process of one actor imitating another. Indeed, stigmergic coordination does not occur through individuals witnessing the behaviour of marker-leaving individuals—rather it occurs through engaging with the markers they leave behind. Furthermore, as Drury et al. (2019) highlight, one of the key issues associated with contagion-related metaphors is that they can imply an automatic transmission of affect and behaviour between parties. Contrary to contagion’s imitative automaticity, stigmergy does not necessarily require an individual to undertake the same kind of action involved in creating the marker. Thus, whilst emotional contagion can arise out of what Theraulaz and Bonabeau (1999) terms quantitative stigmergy—wherein traces stimulate the same kind of action but at different intensities and frequencies—it can never represent a form of what they term qualitative stigmergy: where a trace stimulates a different kind of action ( Heylighen 2016b : 54). Here, it is important to emphasize that unlike cascades and contagion, the concept of stigmergy is concerned with behaviour coordination rather than behaviour diffusion. Thus, whilst stigmergic crime prevention practices can cascade through contagion (see Braithwaite 2020 ), stigmergy is not reducible to behavioural contagion, nor is it reducible to Braithwaite’s related notion of crime prevention as a cascade phenomenon.

In examining the stigmergic properties of Springsclare Crime Prevention, our research highlights the need to further examine the mediatic conditions and media context of community crime prevention: the role different media and communication technologies play in shaping community crime prevention by altering the speed, morphology and reach of information diffusion and coordination between community members. As philosophers of technology and perspectives such as actor-network theory have long emphasized (see Brown 2006 ), technologies play a key role in shaping human sociality and interactions—interactions that are central to community crime prevention initiatives. As a small qualitative study into a specific spatially bounded community, our findings are not generalizable; however, our insights into the underlying mechanisms of swarm intelligence and stigmergy are transferable to similar crime prevention groups employing Facebook and similar social media platforms. In examining the potential impact of social media in shaping community crime prevention initiatives, however, we want to emphasize that care needs to be taken in avoiding an approach characterized by technological determinism, i.e. an approach that treats social media, such as Facebook, as having a uniform effect in shaping community crime prevention initiatives. As social events are multiply determined (see Elder-Vass 2010 ), the mechanisms we have examined here do not operate in isolation from a variety of other mechanisms pertaining to the characteristics of spatially bounded communities. Such mechanisms will shape the impact that stigmergic mechanisms have on events arising from social media-facilitated community crime prevention initiatives. Future studies examining the mediatic dimensions of community crime prevention might, therefore, add measures of Internet-facilitated collective intelligence to the list of variables they examine (see Malone 2018 ). Moreover, by measuring the reach and speed of information-diffusion among crime prevention groups on social media, they might test whether ‘many eyes on the street’ meeting on the screen does increase the efficacy of such groups—and whether it leads to a form of ‘suspicion creep’ in doing so. For as our research indicates, the mechanisms of swarm intelligence and stigmergy may represent a double-edged sword for community crime prevention. Though powerful in increasing the ability for community crime prevention groups to collectively respond in real-time to crime, swarm intelligence and stigmergy can contribute to a hive mind buzzing with fear, hypervigilance and exclusionary sentiments.

Akkermans , M and Vollaard , B . ( 2015 ). Effect van het WhatsApp-project in Tilburg op het Aantal Woninginbraken—Een Evaluatie . Tilburg University .

Google Scholar

Google Preview

Beni , G and Wang , J . ( 1993 ), ‘Swarm intelligence in cellular robotic systems’, in P Dario , G Sandini and P Aebischer , eds, Robots and Biological Systems: Towards a New Bionics? , 703 – 12 . Springer .

Bennett , T H . ( 1990 ), Evaluating Neighborhood Watch . Cambridge University Press .

Bennett , T , Holloway , K and Farrington , D . ( 2008 ), ‘ The Effectiveness of Neighborhood Watch’ , Campbell Systematic Reviews , 4 : 1 – 46 .

Bhaskar , R . ( 1986 ), Scientific Realism and Human Emancipation. The Thetford Press .

Bonabeau , E and Meyer , C . ( 2001 ), ‘Swarm Intelligence: A Whole New Way to Think About Business’ , Harvard Business Review , 79 : 106 –15.

Braithwaite , J . ( 2020 ), ‘Crime as a Cascade Phenomenon’ , International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice , 44: 137–69.

Brown , S . ( 2006 ), ‘The Criminology of Hybrids: Rethinking Crime and law in Technosocial Networks’ , Theoretical Criminology , 10 : 223 – 44 .

Brunton-Smith , I and Bullock , K . ( 2019 ), ‘Patterns and Drivers of Co-Production in Neighbourhood Watch in England and Wales: From Neo-Liberalism to New Localism’ , The British Journal of Criminology , 59 : 85 – 106 .

Bursik , R J and Grasmick , H G . ( 1999 ), Neighborhoods & Crime . Lexington Books .

Caskey , T R , Wasek , J S and Franz , A Y . ( 2018 ), ‘Deter and Protect: Crime Modeling With Multi-Agent Learning’ , Complex & Intelligent Systems , 4 : 155 – 69 .

Charmaz , K . ( 2006 ), Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide Through Qualitative Analysis . Sage Publications .

Cirel , P , Evans , P , McGillis , D and Whitcomb , D . ( 1977 ), Community Crime Prevention Program, Seattle: An Exemplary Project . Government Printing Office .

Clarke , R and Hough , M . ( 1984 ), Crime and Police Effectiveness, Home Office Research Study No. 79. Home Office .

Collins , R . ( 2004 ), Interaction Ritual Chains . Princeton University Press .

Cozens , P . ( 2008 ), ‘Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design’, in R Wortley and L Mazerolle , eds, Environmental Criminology and Crime Analysis , 153 – 77 . Willan.

Cozens , P and Davies , T . ( 2013 ), ‘Investigating “Eyes on the Street,” Perceptions of Crime and the use of Security Shutters—Insights from a Residential Suburb in Perth (WA)’ , Crime Prevention and Community Safety , 15 : 175 – 91 .

Crawford , A . ( 1997 ), The Local Governance of Crime: Appeals to Community and Partnerships . Clarendon Press .

—— ( 1999 ), ‘Questioning Appeals to Community Within Crime Prevention and Control’ , European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research , 7 : 509 – 30 .

Crow , G and Maclean , C . ( 2000 ), ‘Community’, in G Payne , ed., Social Divisions , 223 – 41 . Macmillan .

Dipple , A C . ( 2015 ), ‘Collaboration in Web N.0: Stigmergy and Virtual Pheromones’ , doctoral dissertation, Queensland University of Technology .

Dixon , N . ( 2017 ), ‘ Stranger-ness and Belonging in a Neighbourhood WhatsApp Group’ , Open Cultural Studies , 1 : 493 – 503 .

Drury , J , Ball , R , Neville , F , Reicher , S and Stott , C . ( 2019 ), Re-Reading the 2011 Riots: ESRC Beyond Contagion Interim Report. University of Sussex . Available online at https://www.sussex.ac.uk/webteam/gateway/file.php?name=beyond-contagion-report-forthe-guardian-january-2019.pdf&site=557 [accessed: 20 July 2020 ].

Elder-Vass , D . ( 2010 ), The Causal Power of Social Structures: Emergence, Structure and Agency . Cambridge University Press .

Estellés-Arolas , E . ( 2020 ), ‘ Using Crowdsourcing for a Safer Society: When the Crowd Rules’, European Journal of Criminology , 1 – 20 . doi: 10.1177/1477370820916439 .

Fagan , J , Wilkinson , D L and Davies , G . ( 2007 ), ‘ Social Contagion of Violence’, in D J Flannery , A T Vazsonyi and I D Waldman , eds, The Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression . Cambridge University Press . p. 688 – 723 .

Garnier , S , Gautrais , J and Theraulaz , G . ( 2007 ), ‘ The Biological Principles of Swarm Intelligence’ , Swarm Intelligence , 1 : 3 – 31 .

Garofalo , J and McLeod , M . ( 1989 ), ‘ The Structure and Operations of Neighborhood Watch Programs in the United States’ , Crime & Delinquency , 35 : 326 – 44 .

Glaser , B and Strauss , A . ( 1967 ), ‘ Grounded Theory: The Discovery of Grounded Theory’ , Sociology: the Journal of the British Sociological Association , 12 : 27 – 49 .

Grassé , P P . ( 1959 ), ‘ La Theorie de la Stigmergie: Essai d’interpretation du Comportement des Termites Constructeurs’ , Insectes Sociaux , 6 : 41 – 81 .

Greenberg   S W , Rohe W M and Williams J R . ( 1982 ), Safe and Secure Neighbourhoods: Physical Characteristics and Informal Territorial Control in High and Low Crime Neighbourhoods . Department of Justice .

Hampton , K N . ( 2010 ), ‘ Internet Use and the Concentration of Disadvantage: Glocalization and the Urban Underclass’ , American Behavioral Scientist , 53 : 1111 – 32 .

Henry , A . ( 2020 ), ‘ Interaction Rituals and “Police” Encounters: New Challenges for Interactionist Police Sociology’ , Policing and Society . doi: 10.1080/10439463.2020.1791861 .

Heylighen , F ( 2016a ), ‘ Stigmergy as a Universal Coordination Mechanism I: Definition and Components’ , Cognitive Systems Research , 38 : 4 – 13 .

—— ( 2016b ), ‘ Stigmergy as a Universal Coordination Mechanism II: Varieties and Evolution’ , Cognitive Systems Research , 38 : 50 – 9 .

Hine , C . ( 2015 ), Ethnography for the Internet: Embedded, Embodied and Everyday . Bloomsbury Publishing .

Hope , T . ( 1995 ), ‘ Community Crime Prevention’ , Crime and Justice , 19 : 21 – 89 .

Husain , S . ( 1988 ), Neighbourhood Watch in England and Wales: A Locational Analysis . Home Office .

Jacobs , J . ( 1961 ), The Death and Life of Great American Cities . Jonathon Cope .

Krause , J , Ruxton , G D and Krause , S . ( 2010 ), ‘ Swarm Intelligence in Animals and Humans’ , Trends in Ecology & Evolution , 25 : 28 – 34 .

Laycock , G and Tilley , N . ( 1995 ), Policing and Neighbourhood Watch: Strategic Issues . Home Office Police Research Group .

Lévy , P . ( 1997 ), Collective Intelligence: Mankind’s Emerging World in Cyberspace . Perseus Books .

Lewis , T G and Marsh , L . ( 2016 ), ‘ Human Stigmergy: Theoretical Developments and New Applications’ , Cognitive Systems Research , 38 : 1 – 60 .

Lima , S L . ( 1995 ), ‘ Back to the Basics of Anti-Predatory Vigilance: The Group-Size Effect’ , Animal Behaviour , 49 : 11 – 20 .

Lindsay , B and McGillis , D . ( 1986 ), ‘ Citywide Community Crime Prevention: An Assessment of the Seattle Program’, in DP Rosenbaum , ed., Community Crime Prevention: Does it Work? , 46 – 67 . SAGE .

Lub , V . ( 2017 ), Neighbourhood Watch in a Digital Age: Between Crime Control and Culture of Control. Palgrave Macmillan .

—— ( 2018 ), ‘ Neighbourhood Watch: Mechanisms and Moral Implications’ , British Journal of Criminology , 58 , 906 – 24 .

MacDowall , L . ( 2014 ), ‘ Graffiti, Street Art and Theories of Stigmergy’, in J. Lossau and W. Stevens , eds, The Uses of Art in Public Space , 45 – 60 . Routledge .

Malone , T W . ( 2018 ), Superminds: The Surprising Power of People and Computers Thinking Together . Little, Brown and Company

Mols , A and Pridmore , J . ( 2019 ), ‘ When Citizens Are “Actually Doing Police Work”: The Blurring of Boundaries in WhatsApp Neighbourhood Crime Prevention Groups in The Netherlands’ , Surveillance & Society , 17 : 272 – 87 .

Newburn , T . ( 2007 ), Criminology. Willian Publishing .

Nieto-Gomez , R . ( 2016 ), ‘ Stigmergy at the Edge: Adversarial Stigmergy in the War on Drugs’ , Cognitive Systems Research , 38 : 31 – 40 .

Oliver , C . ( 2011 ), ‘ Critical Realist Grounded Theory: A New Approach for Social Work Research’ , British Journal of Social Work , 42 : 371 – 87 .

Palumbo , D , Ferguson , J L and Stein , J . ( 1997 ), ‘The Conditions Needed for Successful Community Crime Prevention’, in S P La , ed., Crime Prevention at a Crossroads. Anderson . pp. 79 – 98 .

Pratt , T C and Cullen , F T . ( 2005 ), ‘ Assessing Macro-Level Predictors and Theories of Crime: A Meta-Analysis’ , Crime and Justice , 32 : 373 – 450 .

Pridmore , J , Mols , A , Wang , Y and Holleman , F . ( 2019 ), ‘ Keeping an Eye on the Neighbours: Police, Citizens, and Communication Within Mobile Neighbourhood Crime Prevention Groups’ , The Police Journal , 92 : 97 – 120 .

Reichertz , J . ( 2007 ), ‘ Abduction: The Logic of Discovery of Grounded Theory’ , A Bryant and K Charmaz , eds, The Sage Handbook of Grounded Theory , 214 – 28 . SAGE .

Reilly , P and Trevisan , F . ( 2016 ), ‘ Researching Protest on Facebook: Developing an Ethical Stance for the Study of Northern Irish Flag Protest Pages’ , Information, Communication & Society , 19 : 419 – 35 .

Rosenbaum , D P . ( 1987 ), ‘ The Theory and Research Behind Neighborhood Watch: Is it a Sound Fear and Crime Reduction Strategy? ’, Crime & Delinquency , 33 : 103 – 34 .

Rosenbaum , D P and Heath , L . ( 1990 ), ‘ The “Psycho-Logic” of Fear-Reduction and Crime-Prevention Programs’, in Social Influence Processes and Prevention , 221 – 47 . Springer .

Rosenberg , L B and Baltaxe , D . ( 2016 ), ‘ Swarm Intelligence and Morality of the Hive Mind’, paper presented at Collective Intelligence , 1 – 9 . New York University , 1–3 June.

Rossner , M . ( 2011 ), ‘ Emotions and Interaction Ritual: A Micro Analysis of Restorative Justice’ , British Journal of Criminology , 51 : 95 – 119 .

Sampson , R J . ( 2012 ), Great American City: Chicago and the Enduring Neighborhood Effect. University of Chicago Press .

Sampson , R J , Raudenbush , S W and Earls , F . ( 1997 ), ‘ Neighborhoods and Violent Crime: A Multilevel Study of Collective Efficacy’ , Science , 277 : 918 – 24 .

Skogan , W G . ( 1988 ), ‘ Community Organizations and Crime’ , Crime and Justice , 10 : 39 – 78 .

Skogan   W G and Maxfield M G . ( 1981 ), Coping With Crime. SAGE .

Smith , M J , Clarke , R V and Pease , K . ( 2002 ), ‘ Anticipatory Benefits in Crime Prevention’ , Crime Prevention Studies , 13 : 71 – 88 .

Stern , M J and Adams , A E . ( 2010 ), ‘ Do Rural Residents Really Use the Internet to Build Social Capital? An Empirical Investigation’ , American Behavioral Scientist , 53 : 1389 – 422 .

Syed , M and Nelson , S C . ( 2015 ), ‘ Guidelines for Establishing Reliability When Coding Narrative Data’ , Emerging Adulthood , 3 : 375 – 87 .

Thacker , E . ( 2004 ), Networks, Swarms, Multitudes: Part Two . CTHEORY .

Thaler , R H and Sunstein , C R . ( 2009 ), Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. Penguin .

Theraulaz , G and Bonabeau , E . ( 1999 ), ‘ A Brief History of Stigmergy’ , Artificial Life , 5 : 97 – 116 .

Ward , A J , Herbert-Read , J E , Sumpter , D J and Krause , J . ( 2011 ), ‘ Fast and Accurate Decisions Through Collective Vigilance in Fish Shoals’ , Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 108 : 2312 – 5 .

Webb , E J , Campbell , D T , Schwartz , R D and Sechrest , L . ( 1966 ), Nonreactive Measures in the Social Sciences . Rand McNally .

Wickes , R L . ( 2010 ), ‘ Generating Action and Responding to Local Issues: Collective Efficacy in Context’ , Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology , 43 : 423 – 43 .

Willis , R . ( 2019 ), ‘ Observations Online: Finding the Ethical Boundaries of Facebook Research’ , Research Ethics , 15 : 1 – 17 .

Zamfirescu , C B and Filip , F G . ( 2010 ), ‘ Swarming Models for Facilitating Collaborative Decisions’ , International Journal of Computers Communications & Control , 5 : 125 – 37 .

Email alerts

Citing articles via.

  • Recommend to your Library

Affiliations

  • Online ISSN 1464-3529
  • Print ISSN 0007-0955
  • Copyright © 2024 Centre for Crime and Justice Studies (formerly ISTD)
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

  • Systematic Review
  • Open access
  • Published: 06 January 2021

Systematic review of situational prevention methods for crime against species

  • Dorothea Delpech   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4555-6531 1 ,
  • Herve Borrion 1 &
  • Shane Johnson 1  

Crime Science volume  10 , Article number:  1 ( 2021 ) Cite this article

7927 Accesses

11 Citations

14 Altmetric

Metrics details

Illegal activities concerning terrestrial species (TS) are responsible for a variety of health, environmental, economic and security issues. The majority of academic research associated with species relates to conservation, with few publications specifically investigating the scale of crimes impacting species or how they can be prevented. This article systematically reviews the available evidence about what works to prevent crime against terrestrial species. Of over 29,000 documents that were returned in the first stage of the review, these were filtered to just over 100. The remaining documents were partially or fully read to identify the most relevant documents to include in the final qualitative synthesis. The review results show there is a significant lack of primary research in this area, as only five articles were found that met the study inclusion criteria. The identified articles focus on the effects of two types of situational crime prevention interventions: community outreach and ranger patrol frequency. Community outreach was shown to have a significant impact on local poaching levels, while for patrolling the evidence suggests a positive impact on the discovery of poachers, animal carcasses and poaching paraphernalia, however, the quality of these studies varied greatly. To prevent the further decline of species numbers internationally, more effort should be invested in publicising existing research into the effectiveness of prevention strategies that have not reached the wider scientific audience, as well as the funding and promotion of research into alternate methods of crime prevention.

The illicit exploitation of flora and fauna has a variety of negative impacts internationally including threats to health security (e.g. disease spreading, improper preparation of meat), national security (e.g. terrorism financing through illicit trade in species), environmental security (e.g. animal population decline and possible extinctions) and the economy (e.g. costs associated to the damage and removal of natural capital). In addition to the anthropocentric impacts described above, there is an increasing acknowledgement in the scientific literature and law of the direct impact of crimes on the species as sentient beings that can feel pain and suffering (Nurse 2016 ; Sollund 2016 ; Blattner 2019 ).

In 2014, the illicit trade in wild flora and fauna was estimated to be worth US$7–23 billion internationally, in combination with other forms of environmental crime (Nellemann et al. 2014 ). However, the ‘dark figure’ of wildlife crime (i.e. unreported/undetected offenses), and difficulty in attributing a ‘value’ to natural capital, makes accurately estimating the total global costs of such crimes challenging. A recent publication by the World Bank aimed to account for the financial and economic value of these natural assets. Montero et al. ( 2019 ) estimated that when combining the economic and financial values of these resources, the actual cost of the illicit trade in flora and fauna has a global economic value of between US$1 and US$2 trillion per year.

Contrary to popular belief, the targets of crime are not limited to exotic and iconic species, such as elephants and tigers, but also include farmed produce including livestock and crops (e.g. livestock theft/rustling, sheep worrying and coursing). Crimes involving farmed produce in particular is an increasing problem for developing and developed countries alike, where agriculture forms one of the main contributors to both local and national economy through natural assets and exports (Donnermeyer and Barclay 2005 ; Swanson 1981 ). The fundamental role of agriculture globally means the impacts of crime involving farmed produce are widespread and affect stakeholders from ‘field to fork’. The National Farmers Union (NFU) Mutual, one of the leading insurers of farms in the UK, estimates the cost of rural crime in the UK at GBP£49.9 M in 2018, with Livestock crime (LC) alone costing GBP£2.5 M (Sidebottom 2013 ; NFU 2019 ; 24th PANIU 2015a , b ).

Various stakeholders, from individuals to governments, are involved in tackling the issue of wildlife crime and spend significant sums of money on programs aiming to protect species.

A review of donor funding of the Global Wildlife Program, a partnership of organisations led by the World Bank, identified that since 2010, funds of around US$1.3 Billion were pledged to tackling wildlife crimes internationally. Beyond international conglomerate donations, collaborations such as the Wildlife Crime Initiative (WCI) between WWF and TRAFFIC, aim to tackle wildlife crime by engaging with local, national governments, charities and NGOs to deter the continued exploitation and extinction of species (UNODC 2017 ; WWF 2017 ; UN News 2016 ).

For all stakeholders there exist pragmatic questions about what problems to focus on, and what approaches and interventions to invest in. To ensure that the programs implemented are cost effective and produce no or limited negative consequences, decision-makers must also be aware of the likely impacts of different crime prevention techniques. However, the range of crime prevention techniques is large, varying from the use of a padlock on a barn door, to international legislation regulating trade in specific products. The variety of techniques employed, and the fidelity of implementation achieved, hinder the ability to estimate the effectiveness of programs on a macro-scale. This is illustrated by the example of the use of policy in order to prevent the trafficking of illegitimate goods. Establishing the impact of legislation and policy on an international scale, whilst accounting for the influence of local projects and schemes, would be major task.

An article by Kurland et al ( 2017 ) provided the first, and to the authors’ knowledge, only overview of prevention methods used in conservation and wildlife crime prevention. The Kurland et al ( 2017 ) article recognised the importance of providing stakeholders with guidance on what prevention methods exist in the hope this would lead to more informed and effective decision making. The article has, however, two important limitations that are addressed in the present study:

The first limitation is that Kurland et al ( 2017 ) combined literature from the fields of conservation and crime prevention. Whilst both research areas relate to species protection, and the conservation field is likely to provide the majority of information of interest to this study, conservation techniques are not used solely to address illicit activities against species. The Kurland ( 2017 ) study tried to identify articles where the methods used mirrored those of SCP. A combined review of prevention techniques used to alter legal and illegal activities, requires a clear distinction between the different methods and/or mechanisms by which the techniques work (e.g. increased penalties for illegal activity vs. education of the impact of legal but destructive activity). Assessing the effectiveness of the techniques used, becomes even more complex, as the driving forces behind situational crime prevention techniques may not be directly applicable when addressing non-criminal activity (e.g. removal of excuses using signs/rules may not be suitable if the activity is legal, despite its negative impacts on species). It is not possible to determine the significance of this limitation without more details about the searching and filtering methods used in Kurland’s review.

The second limitation was identified by Kurland et al ( 2017 ) themselves. Whilst the method for the selection and filtering of articles was described, there was limited information relating to inclusion and exclusion criteria or an extraction framework. In concluding their review, Kurland et al ( 2017 ) commented on the benefits of completing a more systematic review that could provide a comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms, contexts and outcomes of assessed prevention methods (Petticrew 2001 ).

The purpose of this article is to assess the effectiveness of existing Situational Crime Prevention (SCP) techniques for the prevention of crime against species. It aims to complement Kurland et al.’s work and address the lack of research into what works in the prevention of species crime. Our work focuses on the measures implemented for the situational prevention of crimes against ‘Terrestrial Species’ (TS). Species is the term used as a principal taxonomic unit that denotes a ‘group of organisms of similar individuals which are able to interbreed’ (Larkcom and Delpech 2013 ). Species fall into one of five Kingdoms: Plantae, Animalia, Fungi, Bacteria, and Protoctists. During the scoping phase of this review, the authors acknowledged that movement on and around areas of water and shorelines introduced additional variables (e.g., theoretical offshore boundaries vs. physical on land boundaries; freedom of movement on and around these areas, modes of transport), and due to the reviews already broad search strategy, it was decided that the aquatic environment, including marine and other aquatic species, and their associated crime prevention methods would not be included in this review. The authors recognise that this area is deserving of its own independent systematic review, and could be combined with the review of terrestrial species crime prevention methods in future (Larkcom and Delpech 2013 ). Microscopic species (e.g. protozoa, algae) were also excluded. The terrestrial species in the remaining Kingdoms of Animalia (other than Humans), Plantae and Fungi were included, and hereafter will be collectively referred to as ‘Terrestrial Species’.

TS can be divided into two main groups (Driscoll et al. 2009 ):

Wild species: native fauna and flora of a region e.g. elephants, tigers, bluebells, orchids.

Farmed (domesticated) species: kept & bred/raised and used as assets e.g. cows, chickens, wheat, ginseng.

The decision to combine information on prevention methods relating to wild and farmed species was made because many TS are categorized as both wild and farmed, depending on the given habitat—e.g. ginseng can be found in the wild but is also farmed in many countries (Daerr 2001 ). In addition to a categorical overlap, there also exists a geographic overlap, where the environments wild species inhabit are increasingly being used for agricultural purposes. The review assessed the effectiveness of prevention techniques used in rural areas. Rural areas were selected again due to the overlap between the agricultural landscape and areas where wildlife inhabit. The authors also acknowledged the differences in the physical structure, level of surveillance, and opportunities of rural and urban areas making the comparison or transfer of prevention techniques used potentially incompatible.

Beyond the categorical and geographic similarities between TS, there exists a shared aetiology in the crimes that affect them: TS are targeted for financial gain, subsistence and/or sport, which could mean that prevention techniques used for wild species may be transferable to farmed species and vice versa.

The contextual information of a given location is used to select or design suitable interventions that may increase the risks and effort required by the criminal, reduce the rewards and provocation and/or remove excuses, as perceived by offenders (see Table 1 ). A group of strategies used at a local level are collectively referred to as Situational Crime Prevention (SCP) techniques. Whilst all of the SCP techniques are potentially effective in preventing crime, they are not all suited to every given situation. For this reason, it is important to establish ‘What Works’ in relation to given types of crime, in this case crimes against species.

The absence of literature advising stakeholders as to what works best in preventing crimes against species, could increasingly lead to the poor investment of the already limited funds to tackle criminal activity involving vulnerable species internationally. A systematic review of the existing literature should provide stakeholders with an initial overview of what information is available, what methods have been evaluated and what works in preventing crimes against species. Researchers can use systematic reviews to identify the gaps in the literature, and undertake more outcome evaluation studies for future reviews.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The systematic review was performed using the following criteria:

Date of Research Publication—No exclusion criteria. The research must have been produced prior to September 2016 when systematic searching began.

Published and Unpublished Research—A comprehensive search of the available literature was performed, including unpublished ‘grey’ literature, to mitigate the effect of publication bias (Mlinaric et al. 2017 ).

International Literature—There was no restriction on the countries from which publications originated, but they must have been written in, or be available, in English.

Intervention Type—Interventions included were deemed to be in line with the core situational crime prevention (SCP) techniques; i.e., those aiming to influence the perceived effort, rewards and risks of committing crimes, as well as removing the provocations and excuses associated with criminal behaviour (Cornish and Clarke 2003 ). Situations, and by extension situational crime prevention measures, exist throughout the crime commission process (Cornish 1994 ). From a poacher’s perspective, for example, they may be found in the wild where animals live but also in villages where accomplices are briefed, tools prepared, and meat sold (Lemieux 2020 ).

Intervention Type—The interventions examined in the identified studies were included if situational crime prevention techniques have been implemented and an outcome evaluation conducted, therefore, theoretical or ‘proof of concept’ studies (e.g. Borrion et al. 2019 ) were not selected for analysis.

Literature relating to international or national policies such as those published by NGOs and governments are not included in the review, as literature relating to policy has insufficient data for assessment to establish its effectiveness on species crime prevention (Pires and Moreto 2011 ).

Study Type—Interventions used to specifically reduce the incidence of crime against species were included in the review. Interventions that may have indirectly had a positive impact on the incidence of crimes against species or species numbers were not included. This is because the knock on effects of interventions can be difficult to evaluate, especially as changes in species numbers can be attributed to a variety of factors that go beyond crime, including land use changes, sustainable development and legal hunting.

Location: Rural Areas —Studies explicitly described as occurring in an urban setting were excluded. The terms used in primary research to describe rural areas vary greatly, and included forest, farmland, agricultural land, national park, area of outstanding natural beauty, area of scientific interest, and village . Due to this variety of terms, and to avoid the exclusion of relevant articles, articles that did not specify a particular location, and those using generic rural terminology were automatically progressed to the next screening stage, if they met the other inclusion criteria.

Search strategy

The following search engines were used:

General Databases: International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS); ProQuest; PsychINFO; Scopus; Web of Knowledge; Zetoc.

Agricultural / Environmental: AgEcon Search – which covers research in Agricultural and Applied Economics—It is a free, open access repository of full-text scholarly literature on agricultural and applied economics; RSPCA—Wildlife Centre Research

Criminological Databases: Australian Government—Institute of Criminology; COPAC—UK Library Catalogue Database; National Criminal Justice Reference Service (NCJRS).

Grey Literature Databases: British Library EThOS; System for Information on Grey Literature in Europe (SIGLE).

In addition, the following journals were hand-searched for relevant studies: American Society of Agricultural and Biological Engineers (ASABE); Crime Prevention & Community Safety; International Journal of Agricultural Management; Journal of Applied Ecology; Journal of research in crime and delinquency; Journal of Rural Affairs ; Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences; Rural Sociological Society ; Southern Rural Sociology ; Understanding and managing threats to the environment in South Eastern Europe. As were the following books: Crime & Conflict in the Countryside; Situational Prevention of Poaching; Crimes Against Nature: Environmental criminology and ecological justice.

Keywords for Boolean Searches —The search terms chosen were based on broad keywords that could be associated with species crime from animal type to prevention methods:

Search Terms were separated into three categories:

Filtering Stages

Initial article filtering was achieved by reading article titles and abstracts for relevance (as denoted by the inclusion/exclusion criteria described earlier). EPPI Reviewer software was used to manage the inclusion/exclusion process and the collation of relevant studies. A hierarchy of exclusion is shown in Table 2 and includes: Theme, Geography, Intervention and Species.

Articles that were considered ambiguous based on their abstract and title were progressed to the second filtering stage, and read in full to prevent the loss of relevant studies in the filtering process.

Quality assessment and data extraction

A data identification and extraction document was developed in line with examples used in previous systematic reviews as described on the Cochrane & Campbell systematic review website, and in line with the EMMIE framework. The document was used to standardise the extraction of relevant information from the articles being reviewed. Additional subfields were included in an iterative process to address when new aspects emerged during the initial stages of reading and assessing the information available in the filtered articles. Reasons as to why articles were not progressed were also recorded for reference.

The EMMIE framework was used to organize the synthesis of information extracted from the final included studies (Johnson et al., 2015 ). Rather than focusing exclusively on the effect size of interventions, the framework was developed to emphasise the need to explicitly synthesise (and assess the quality of research concerned with) what is known about other important dimensions of interventions that are relevant to policy-makers and other stakeholders. The five dimensions of EMMIE are: Effect, which considers the size of the impact of an intervention; the Mechanisms through which an intervention is believed to bring about its intended effects; the contextual Moderators that may influence the likelihood that an intervention has its intended effects; the key aspects of Implementation that are required for the delivery of the intervention; and, the Economic costs and benefits associated with the intervention. As well as synthesising what is known, the aim of the framework is to help explicitly identify gaps in knowledge.

Results and Discussion

Figure  1 illustrates the stages of document screening and shows that, of the 29,252 articles initially identified, only five remained after the application of the study criteria.

figure 1

The process used to filter and assess articles

Research on species crime often combines unknown volumes of criminal activity and unknown populations of species, creating a complex field of research, where the methods adopted are the best fit for the data available, rather than those with the greatest internal validity.

Of the five studies that met the inclusion criteria, one examined the impact of community outreach, while the remaining four examined the impact of anti-poaching patrols. Outreach programmes have been included as they have direct relevance to the five situational crime prevention principles (e.g. “aimed to build trust, raise awareness , motivate, offer opportunities for action, increase perceived behavioural control of villagers and generate social pressure against poaching”). Anti-poaching patrols have also been included as they are equivalent to ‘security guards’, one of the 25 situational crime prevention techniques.

In what follows, given the limitations in the data available, the two interventions identified, and the analytic methods used in the primary studies, the overall findings for each type of intervention are presented in the form of a narrative synthesis, following the basic structure of the EMMIE framework.

Community outreach

A study by Steinmetz et al ( 2014 ) assessed the effectiveness of a community outreach intervention. The situational crime prevention technique employed by the community outreach team was aiming to remove the excuses used by locals, by educating them of the impact of the illegal activity involving the species. This was implemented between 2008 and 2011 in Kui Buri National Park, Thailand. Over the four-year period 116 outreach events were ran which reached approximately 7500 people across 24 villages. The outreach work was estimated to have covered 83% of villages within 5 km of the park, with some visited more than once. The outreach programme aimed to build trust, raise awareness, motivate, offer opportunities for action, increase perceived behavioural control of villagers and generate social pressure against poaching. The mechanisms identified by the authors as to how the community outreach was expected to have had an effect are directly related to the situational crime prevention principles, particularly raising awareness, increasing perceived behavioural control and generating social pressure. The results of the study suggest that species crime has reduced as a result of the outreach programme.

Wildlife abundance over study period

The wildlife populations of four species at three sites (4 species × 3 sites = 12 measurements) were monitored using observation surveys conducted annually from 2006 to 2011 (in the dry season: November to June) at three sites (each being 30–50 km 2 ). The surveys revealed that three of the monitored species increased significantly: Pig occupancy almost doubled at Klong Kui (p = 0.034), Muntjac roughly trebled (p = 0.018) and Pig increased by roughly half at Hup Inthanin (p = 0.045), other species such as Gaur in Klong Kui were nearly extinct from the area but began repopulating. Whilst the increase in Gaur occupancy was not statistically significant (p = 0.17), it was an important positive outcome for the local area with the repopulation of the species. The only species to see a decline was Sambar, which was stable at Hup Inthanin but declined in the two other monitoring sites. This decline was, however, not considered statistically significant (p > 0.07).

Camera trapping was incorporated into the study to complement the occupancy surveys. Cameras were placed in 25–28 locations in 2007, 2009 and 2011, in a 130 m 2 that encompassed two of the occupancy survey areas. The image results corroborated the survey findings, with estimated increases in species numbers identified over the study period for Pig (p = 0.007), Porcupine (p = 0.037) and Gaur (p = 0.002), for which the numbers nearly doubled. For the Muntjac (p > 0.28) and Sambar (p = 0.086) the numbers were found to be stable.

Poaching pressure over study period

Poaching pressure was calculated using the encounter rate of poaching signs (shotgun shells, tree stands, snares, carcasses, hunting camps) per 100 km. Poaching pressure declined by fourfold (p = 0.059) between 2009 and 2011, reducing from 10.1 hunting signs per 100 km in 2009, to 6.8 in 2010, and finally 2.4 in 2011.

The authors conducted two analyses to assess the influence of existing patrols on the observed decrease in poaching pressure and increase in wildlife abundance: (1) Deterrence effect of patrolling on poaching pressure—whether any changes in poaching pressure were the result of the established anti-poaching patrols in the study area the previous month, (2) Effect of patrolling on wildlife trends—patrol effort was used in and around the three wildlife monitoring sites; patrol effort was used as the predictor variable, and the wildlife occupancy trends as the dependent variable.

Two additional analyses were carried out to verify that the observed decrease in poaching pressure and increase in wildlife abundance were due to the outreach campaign: (3) Effect of intensive outreach on poaching—from June to November 2010, outreach events were held in close succession next to eight patrol zones allowing the authors to examine the effects of intensive outreach by looking at patrol effort and poaching data between two periods before and after outreach work took place, and (4) Effect of outreach on poaching, as perceived by locals—multiple choice questionnaires were used to elicit the opinion of locals as to levels of poaching before and after the outreach work took place. The results of these analyses are presented below:

Patrolling effects on poaching pressure and wildlife abundance

To account for existing patrolling the authors assessed the influence of patrolling on the reduction in poaching over the same period of time the outreach work was conducted. No correlation was found between patrol effort and poaching pressure (p = 0.43). There was also no relationship between annual patrol effort and the mean occupancy trends of the monitored species in the same year (p = 0.532) or subsequent years (p = 0.792). Note: there was no significant difference (p = 0.10) in the mean monthly patrol effort per zone in 2009 (1.7 days), 2010 (0.94 days) and 2011 (1.2 days). Patrol effort was not found to differ significantly before and after the intense outreach campaign (median before  = 1.0, median after  = 1.7, p = 0.161) either.

Outreach effects: deterrence effect of intensive outreach on poaching

To examine the short-term spatial effects of intensive outreach, the authors used patrol effort (mean number of patrol days per month) and poaching index data (number of poaching signs per 100 km) and tested whether differences existed in the months prior (7 – 19 months) and post (2 – 8 months) the outreach campaign. As explained above, patrol effort was not found to differ significantly before and after the intense outreach campaign. However, poaching was found to decline after the outreach campaign (p = 0.017) with a median number of poaching signs per 100 km falling from 4.7 to 0.

Outreach effects: perceptions and attitudes questionnaire

Of the 7500 members of the community estimated to have been involved across the 12 areas where community outreach had been conducted around the park, 311 adults completed a survey to assess their perceptions of poaching related behaviours (consumption of wildlife, sale of wildlife within village, sale to outsiders, hunting by villagers, hunting by outsiders, hiring of villagers to hunt by outsiders), the overall poaching trend over the last 5 years (covering the time of the outreach work), and nine potential causes for change in poaching trends (park patrolling, park outreach, wildlife abundance, market demand, number of hunters, time available for hunting, income, conservation awareness, interest in consuming wildlife). Finally, respondents were asked about their attitude towards wildlife recovery (support, oppose, indifferent). Most respondents indicated that they had perceived a decline in the six types of poaching behaviours, with 88% believing that there had been a decline in poaching overall. The survey respondents were asked what contributed to this perceived decline in their view: ‘Increased park outreach’ was the main answer (67% of the locals), followed by ‘increased patrolling’ and ‘conservation awareness’ (61%).

In summary, the results suggest that a decline in poaching behaviour occurred, with the locals believing the outreach work was the main reason for this decline. In addition to the outreach work, locals also believed increased patrolling (despite data indicating no significant change in patrol effort before and after the outreach work) and increased conservation awareness (indirect benefit of building stronger relationships between park staff and the local community through the outreach work) had also been influential on the decline in poaching.

The authors presented outreach participants with three ways in which they could positively impact the occurrence of poaching locally: (1) educate other community members on the issues facing local wildlife, (2) curb their own hunting and consumption of wildlife, and (3) ostracizing/inconveniencing those involved in poaching.

The central mechanism to explain how community outreach activities would prevent specific crimes against species, involves tackling neutralisation and removing excuses. Neutralisation is a psychological approach to distance oneself from acting contrary to social norms and personal values. Neutralisation techniques include denial of injury, denial of victim, and condemnation of condemners (Sykes and Matza 1957 ). One of the most common neutralisation techniques associated with criminal behaviour is the denial of responsibility. An individual will define a situation in a way to relinquish personal responsibility for their behaviour or actions. By using community outreach to educate individuals about the impact of poaching, including the direct and indirect effects of their actions, the intention of the intervention was to make it harder for some individuals to utilise neutralisation techniques to appease their conscience, in relation to species crime.

By removing some of the excuses associated with species crime, such as ignorance of the impact, or belief that no other opportunities exist, the individuals involved come under increasing pressure both from their local community and their own morality to desist in taking part (Maruna and Copes 2005 ).

The following moderators were identified as factors that could influence the outcome of community outreach schemes designed to protect species:

Access to other alternative livelihood opportunities

Recognition of the location specific context is important when considering the likely impact of any intervention. The villages concerned had an agricultural base which many poachers could turn to for work and food. However, the authors admitted not all individuals associated with poaching will have alternative means of income available and, therefore, outreach work would have a variable impact (Cooney et al. 2017 ). The identification of alternative livelihood opportunities is thus important to consider when implementing such interventions.

Target Audience

The authors reflected on historical outreach work from other fields and focused significant amounts of educational outreach at schools and towards children, hypothesising that the children would then relate this information to their parents and thus use social pressure to encourage positive behaviours. They did not measure the perception of the social-psychological processes used and therefore could not attribute the behaviour changes observed to any one aspect. However, social pressure seems to have played a large role in the change in poaching over the study period. By targeting audiences with greater outreach potential such as local leaders, park staff and children, the authors attempted to maximize the impact of the work being undertaken.

Number of poachers

The authors of the study referred to research in South–East Asia, where only a minority of the local population were involved in poaching. This meant there was significant social pressure from those not involved in poaching. Future research should assess whether the outcomes of outreach activities will be more limited where there is greater proportion of the community involved in poaching.

Implementation

The community outreach work required researchers to work in conjunction with the local government agencies and NGOs to connect with and obtain the permission of local chiefs to reach a large number of community members. Steinmetz et al ( 2014 ) targeted six social or psychological conditions to create behavioural change: Trust, Justification, Motivation, Ethical, Feasible Actions and Confidence.

The first four issues were dealt with through face-to-face interactions with locals, providing them with education and evidence of the importance of preventing species crime and explaining the benefits and responsibility of locals in maintaining healthy environments and species numbers.

The outreach work was conducted by 6–10 park staff, as well as the authors, who held events including those at village meetings, schools, temple fairs, youth camps, and 2–3 h government meetings. The outreach sessions were interactive and included 10–20 min presentations, a quiz with prizes, a Q&A session, and musical performances by the park ranger band. Between 2008 and 2011 the researchers and park staff completed 116 outreach events. School based events also included additional games and encouraged students to create ‘wildlife recovery plans’ which detailed actions students could take to help wildlife. The education of locals was supplemented by suggesting feasible actions to change the incidence of poaching in their local area, and by providing locals with the confidence to control their environment.

The associated costs of the Community Outreach Scheme were not described in this study.

Community outreach is increasingly used to tackle security problems internationally, with schemes such as Neighbourhood Watch, Farm Watch and others, being actively used to encourage the community to take responsibility and preventative action against crimes in their local area (What Works 2015 ). Community outreach in relation to crime is not limited to developing countries or rural areas: many schemes simply aim to increase the awareness of illegal activity amongst local people, and to build community bonds which encourage intervention by locals when crimes are witnessed, or the provision of information on illicit activity to the authorities. The intervention tried to promote responsibility and awareness of the impacts of species crime. The findings suggest that, with sufficient intensive outreach work, involving gaining the trust of influential members of society (e.g. local leaders), educating the local people about the negative impact of illicit activities, and advising locals of alternative livelihoods to illicit activities, can contribute to the reduction in species crime. However, with data only available from one study (which did not have an untreated control site), further evaluation research is clearly necessary to determine whether the impacts reported are replicable and, if so, whether they are context specific.

Anti-poaching patrols

The four remaining articles examined the effectiveness of anti-poaching and protected-area patrolling. Patrols for the prevention of species crimes such as poaching, typically involve rangers/soldiers moving through protected areas usually on foot, searching for poachers or poaching paraphernalia (Moreto et al. 2014 ). The situational crime prevention technique employed in these studies was equivalent to the use of Security Guards, with the aim being to increase the perceived risk associated to committing crimes against terrestrial species by strengthening the formal surveillance in the study area/s.

The studies reviewed used quasi-experimental methods, where a control group may not exist, or if it does, it may not receive the same experimental treatment as the treatment group. Several of the studies combined the results with qualitative information collected through interviews or surveys of stakeholders. The variability between areas (e.g. accessibility, terrain, target species, socio-political factors), and patrols (e.g. methods, resources, rangers) make it difficult to plan and execute ‘gold standard’ experiments such as randomised control trials. Moreover, most of the studies conducted have relied on historical data, which makes randomization impossible.

The targets of such crimes were some of the most iconic species associated with poaching across Africa and Asia, including elephants, rhinos, buffalo and tigers. All of the included studies (shown in Table 3 and 4 ) concluded that anti-poaching patrols in their various forms were effective to varying degrees, in altering the prevalence of species crime.

Hilborn et al. ( 2006 ) estimated the effectiveness of patrolling in the Serengeti National Park using three datasets recorded over several decades (see Table 3 ). In 1977 Tanzania’s economy declined and cuts to the park budget meant that poaching was believed to have increased. In the 1980s the park budgets increased, which allowed park staff to resume patrolling activities. Hilborn et al. ( 2006 ) reviewed historical datasets that suggested poaching declined, and species populations improved or recovered when anti-poaching funds were made available, and patrolling was actively implemented in the National Park.

The census of Buffalo abundance was used as an indicator of poaching intensity. A simple population dynamics model illustrated that between 1955 and 2005, the variations in buffalo numbers could be accounted for by changes in poaching behaviour, which in turn could be accounted for by the changes in patrolling effort.

Whilst this study covered the longest time period, it provided minimal details relating to the patrols (as shown in Table 3 ) and did not account for confounding variables that may have contributed to the variation in poaching and patrolling levels. Overall this study provided the lowest quality assessment of the effectiveness of patrolling as a preventative technique, but it relied on historical data, which perhaps explains this.

Leader-Williams et al. ( 1990 ) conducted research in Luangwa Valley in Zambia on the anti-poaching patrols aimed at combating the poaching of elephant and rhinos. Their findings corroborate those of Hilborn et al. ( 2006 ), also suggesting that the presence of patrols were associated with a deterrent effect on poaching. Data was derived from 781 foot patrols conducted between 1979 and 1985, using evidence of elephant and rhino sightings as well as the detection of skulls/trophies witnessed by rangers. Patrols were made up of three to five scouts, and varied in duration between a few days to several weeks, with an average of 5–9 days per patrol.

This study examined various indicators of illegal activity (including carcasses, camps and poachers apprehended) as well as a number of other covariates (shown in Table 4 ), with the analysis being one of the most robust assessments of effectiveness of the identified studies.

The study identified that the observations of elephant abundance and subsequent changes in this value, were a composite measure of loss due to illegal activity and local immigration/emigration, which could not be quantified separately. Due to this, the authors could not definitively conclude that the number were representative of the relationship between patrolling and elephant abundance but could conclude that the patrolling provided the elephants with a safe haven that other elephants moved into.

In contrast to elephants, rhinos were not found to move to areas of increased safety, and therefore their abundance values were considered to be representative of the species and any losses.

The authors identified that between 1979 and 1985 there was a decline in elephant and rhino numbers in the Luangwa Valley. However, this decline in numbers was identified as not being the result of a lack of motivation by patrols, but instead was more likely the result of insufficient numbers of patrol officers to cover the size of the National Parks in Luangwa Valley.

Patrols were found to be effective where they were implemented with sufficient manpower. Foot patrols and vehicle patrols were found to catch large numbers of offenders over the study period. In these locations, patrol effort was found to have a reductive effect on the distribution of illegal activity and in turn increase the abundance of elephants and rhinos, with findings showing a negative relationship between patrol effort and the discovery of poaching camps or fresh carcasses (Elephants p = 0.05; Rhinos p = 0.01).

The authors of this study reiterate that the decline in species numbers in the Luangwa Valley is not reflective of the effectiveness of the patrols, as the existing patrols were found to be effective where deployed. The decline in species abundance is an indicator of the need for more patrols to cover the entirety of the Valley effectively.

Linkie et al. ( 2015 ) researched the performance of anti-poaching patrols in Kerinci Seblat National Park in Sumatra that aimed to protect tigers and their ungulate prey. The research looked at foot patrols conducted between 2000 and 2010. The study was one of the most comprehensive studies (see Table 3 ) of patrolling effectiveness, measuring patrol frequency and patrol effort, snare trap occurrence, and species (tiger and prey) abundance (using patrol data and camera trap data).

Over the study period, the researchers reviewed 642 forest patrols (see Table 4 ) covering 8885 km during which time they removed 122 snares set specifically for tigers and 4311 traps set for the ungulate prey. Detection histories for each patrol year were used to calculate the snare detection probability between 2000 and 2010.

Detection Probability is used in situations when total abundance cannot be accurately identified (e.g. counting animals in the wild or poaching paraphernalia). Detection probabilities allow researchers to account for unavoidable variability, by taking into account the number of targets detected, the number of visits to sites as well as allowing researchers to account for confounding factors that may make the target population change temporally or spatially.

The study showed a (statistically insignificant) decline in snare trap occurrence of 24%, between 2000 and 2010. However, the authors were unable to control for the influence the introduction of new patrols would have had on the overall number of snares detected in the study area.

The authors suggest that the reduction (albeit non-significant) in snare trap occurrence, combined with no significant changes in the occupancy of tiger prey species over this period, is indicative of the park’s anti-poaching strategies contributing to a stable tiger and prey population. The frequency of patrols was found to have a greater impact on snare detection compared to increasing the distance covered by the foot patrols.

Patrols appeared to gain experience in detecting snares, shown in the detection probability increasing annually between 2000 and 2006 before plateauing.

The study incorporated several covariates relating to accessibility of the landscape to both poachers and patrols. Accessibility was found to be a key factor in snare detection. The more accessible areas require less effort to reach them and are therefore more practical target locations for both poachers and patrols.

Intelligence–based patrols were assessed for the period 2009–2010 in addition to the traditional foot patrols. Intelligence patrols used informant tip-offs, which significantly increased patrol effectiveness, when compared to ordinary foot patrols. The detection probabilities of intelligence-based patrols were 48% higher than foot patrols in 2009 and 41% higher in 2010.

Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) conducted research in Central Luangwa Valley in Zambia, into resource allocation and elephant poaching between 1988 and 1995. During the study period, 149 elephant carcases were discovered, with all but two having been killed for ivory. The results of Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) suggest that patrolling had a positive impact, preventing illegal activity relating to elephants. They also identified specific variables that appear to have influenced the efficiency and effectiveness of patrolling.

The authors looked at nine variables associated with resource allocation (see Table 3 ). The results of the study indicated that five of these had a significant effect on the number of elephant carcasses discovered. With respect to the discovery of elephants found killed illegally, effective investigation days (p = 0.04), and scout density (p = 0.04) were found to be significant predictors. So too were the number of bonus claims paid (p = 0.003), personal salary per scout month (p = 0.04), and Law enforcement expenditure per km 2 (p = 0.05). Based on these findings, Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) recommend that stakeholders involved in species protection focus resources towards increasing the number of scouts/rangers and supporting the collection and rewarding of intelligence and informants.

The two main mechanisms by which patrols are believed to affect poaching activity are increasing the perceived risks of being caught and the perceived cost of carrying out illegal activity.

Increased risks

One of the core principles of the SCP framework involves increasing the (actual or perceived) risks of offending. Increasing the number, distance and size of patrols, therefore has the potential to act as a deterrent to those considering poaching. The rangers also used informal surveillance in the form of community informants, who reported poachers and poaching activity in their particular areas.

Increased costs

The removal, confiscation and destruction of poaching paraphernalia (such as snares, weapons, vehicles, etc.) has a financial impact on those committing such crimes, which in turn can discourage their activity. If snares are removed by anti-poaching patrols repeatedly, the costs associated with replacing the snares may deter an individual from being involved in such crimes in future. Several studies mentioned that increased patrolling or patrolling in new areas could lead to displacement of poaching activity, where the poacher changes their spatial movements in an effort to avoid the patrols. However, displacement and its potential impacts was not investigated by any of the studies.

Factors that influence the detection rates for patrols varied greatly between different patrol teams and over time. Moderating factors identified in the literature that could influence the outcome of anti-poaching patrols:

Accessibility

Accessibility is an important factor in the spatio-temporal analysis of TS crimes, with locations being influenced in a variety of ways by natural features including terrain and vegetation, man-made features such as road networks, and potentially the political/safety considerations of the areas being patrolled. Linkie et al. ( 2015 ) incorporated accessibility factors into their analysis of poaching and patrol effectiveness, and found it significantly influenced the likelihood of poaching and patrolling activity.

Ranger experience and ranger numbers

The experience and number of rangers are considered influential factors in the efficiency of the patrols. The increase in number of rangers had an impact on number of patrols and coverage, both of which influence the likelihood of detection thereby having a positive impact on the effectiveness of patrols for the purposes of preventing crime against species. Linkie et al ( 2015 ) noted that over time the patrols increasing experience lead to an increase in the detection of snares.

Time spent patrolling

Patrol variables such as time and distance, are related to the type of species being targeted. Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) noted that the number of effective patrol days was not a significant factor in the number of elephants killed. These findings contrast with those of Linkie et al. ( 2015 ) who found that the duration spent patrolling was the most significant factor for effective patrolling, compared to other factors such as distance patrolled. The detection of snares for tigers, as assessed in the work of Linkie et al. ( 2015 ), suggested that increased frequency of patrolling over long periods of time (2 years) had a strong influence on snare detection rates in the areas being patrolled. The variation in target (e.g. elephant carcasses, tiger snares, species of interest, poachers) is therefore likely to significantly influence the relevance of predictor variables.

Intelligence-led operations vs. foot patrols

Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) assessed the detection rate of intelligence-led operations (based on informant information), as opposed to routine uninformed foot patrols. There results suggest that intelligence-led operations were more efficient than conventional foot patrols. In relation to arrests, one man-day of intelligence-led operations equated to 23 man-days of foot patrols. However, the costs (discussed later) of intelligence-led operations were 6 times higher than those for foot patrols. Therefore, intelligence-led operations were a factor of four better than routine foot patrols, in terms of the costs associated with arrests.

Target type

Leader-Williams et al. ( 1990 ) discussed the difference in movement between elephants and rhino, with elephants immigrating/emigrating between areas. The variation in target type (e.g., elephant carcasses, tiger snares, species of interest, poachers) would impact patrol variables (e.g. time and distance) and subsequently the effectiveness of patrolling.

Bonuses and Incentives

Jachmann and Billiouw’s ( 1997 ) study was the only one to examine the influence of bonuses and incentives on the effectiveness of patrolling. Bonuses were found to have a significant effect on the number of elephants found killed. The use of financial incentives to encourage others to cooperate with patrols is controversial but was shown to be effective in this study.

The search for evidence of criminal behaviour involves a variety of implementation stages and procedures. The studies identified in this review all retrospectively assessed the effectiveness of anti-poaching patrols. Information pertaining to patrolling such as number of rangers, distances travelled, and equipment used was limited or absent across the articles reviewed, which reflects the inconsistency and difficulties faced when using historical data (Hilborn et al. 2006 ).

Changes in data recording procedures

What rangers observed during patrols was typically recorded on paper using maps to record the location of the incident they had intercepted. While technology for recording patrolling information has advanced (e.g. Global Positioning System (GPS) equipment can now be used to accurately log patrol routes), it is important to acknowledge that access to such equipment is not widespread, and therefore significant differences exist in the recording method for different areas. The rangers’ awareness and ability to navigate new recording systems can impact the accuracy of recorded data. Variability in data recording can also have a significant impact on subsequent data analysis (Linkie et al. 2015 ).

Changes in the behaviour of rangers are unlikely to be quick, and this needs to be taken into account when analysing data, and estimating the relative ‘success’ of an intervention that may still be in the process of being fully integrated ten years after its first introduction.

Technological limitations

GPS coverage can vary and may be limited in places covered by dense canopies or thick vegetation. Failure of equipment to automatically record ranger locations accurately will impact on the accuracy of the information recorded and any subsequent analysis (Martin 2013 ).

As previously stated, the specific financial costs of interventions are not commonly documented in primary research, and again there existed limited information to include in this section from the articles identified. Jachmann and Billiouw ( 1997 ) described some of the financial costs associated with patrolling, where cash rewards were offered for information on poaching and poachers, with arrests or recovered firearms/trophies receiving additional cash awards to compliment patrolling. The article describes the estimated costs associated with patrolling (including salaries and bonuses) and proposes that efficient patrolling could be achieved with a total enforcement budget (based on the given circumstances of the region) of US$50 per km 2 .

Leader-Williams et al. ( 1990 ) conducted research in the same area as Jachmann and Billouw and provided estimates of cost-effectiveness. According to their estimates, between 1979 and 1985 spending on patrols equated to about US$1.1 M. Over the same period, 1483 offenders were caught by four anti-poaching units. Taken together, they estimate that the cost per offender caught was US$730, and they suggest that this was comparatively cheap when compared to other forms of law enforcement. Of course, the reviewed papers were published in the 1990s and the financial costs will have increased in the last 20 years, but the estimates provide a basis for estimating likely current costs.

Anti-poaching patrols are used to detect and deter the exploitation of species and natural capital found in national parks and rural areas internationally. Whilst a number of variables can impact the effectiveness of patrols such as terrain, weather, time patrolling, distance patrolled, investment in rangers and equipment, the overall findings suggest that anti-poaching patrols are effective in deterring poaching. However, the articles that assessed the effectiveness of anti-poaching patrols had a variety of data quality issues:

Data quality issues: retrospective data and species population measures

All of the articles identified had similar issues associated with the quality of the data available, and the methods employed given that the authors were limited to analysing largely retrospective secondary data. The issues encountered were as follows:

Animal population—Estimating the population of a target species is fraught with difficulty, and due to this, it is impossible to accurately quantify the proportion of animals illegally killed; instead, researchers must assume that the detection of animals and poached carcasses provides a proxy measure of the poaching pressure or variation in animal abundance.

Movement of animals—Calculations are complicated further by having to account for the emigration and immigration of animals into and out of the areas of interest.

Poaching pressure—It is assumed that what is detected by patrols (camps, carcasses, poaching paraphernalia) is directly proportional to the poaching pressure in a given area. However, these figures only reflect the areas actually patrolled and hence provide only a partial picture.

Patrol coverage—In relation to the patrolling of protected areas, most areas have little to no patrol coverage, and ranger patrols are not uniformly distributed. As such, accurate levels of crime prevention are difficult to estimate (Moreto et al. 2014 ).

Such issues need to be considered when reviewing articles on this topic and in future work.

The limited number and in some cases the age of the anti-poaching articles (> 20 years old) as well as the data quality issues described above, bring into question the quality of the articles, and would make it difficult to provide any definitive recommendations on the use of such a technique for the prevention of terrestrial species crimes. Further evaluation research is needed to determine whether current anti-poaching patrolling continues to show a positive effect in the reduction of poaching activity.

Systematic reviews are used to bring existing empircal evidence together and identify ‘What works?’ This study is the first systematic review to the authors knowledge that has attempted to bring together all the available literature relating to the effectiveness of situation crime prevention techniques for the prevention of crimes against species.

Empirical evaluations are a valuable tool to inform resource allocation. However, the subfield of criminology that focuses on the causes of, and responses to, ‘ecological’, ‘environmental’ and ‘green’ crimes, harms and hazards, suffers from a lack of empirical quantitative studies (Lynch et al. 2017 ; White 2013 ). This systematic review confirms the severely limited amount of evaluations on the effectiveness of techniques for preventing terrestrial species crime.

The five articles identified in this review focused on the criminal activity of poaching, and reported on the effectiveness of two methods of situational crime prevention: increasing associated risks (anti-poaching patrols) and removing excuses (community outreach). A single study suggested that Community Outreach was found to be effective in reducing poaching, whilst the other studies provide evidence to suggest that Patrols can be effective, although the quality of the anti-poaching studies varied greatly.

The focus on the prevention of poaching in the studies included in this review mean the results of this review cannot be considered generalisable to other criminal acts involving terrestrial species, or environments outside of Africa and Asia (the locations of the included articles), and highlights the need for further research in this area.

As the protection of the environment and the species within it becomes more mainstream, governments and businesses are investing large sums to support the prevention of crimes against species. The investment of large sums into research and prevention requires stakeholders to accurately invest in what has been proven to be effective. This review provides a starting point for decision makers, but based on the very limited research available, it is impossible to be certain if these two types of intervention are the most effective in terms of preventing crime against the target species.

This review provides researchers in a variety of fields with a basis on which to plan future research to evaluate the effectiveness of prevention techniques for the protection of species, and justify the use of funds to fill the large gap in the existing literature with more targeted outcome evaluations of existing and novel SCP techniques.

In the process of conducting this review, several limitations were identified relating to the studies included in this survey, as well as the limitations of studies relating to this topic in general. These will now be discussed with a view to informing future primary evaluations.

Limitations of studies

Accuracy of data —an ever-present issue with species crime relates to the ‘dark figure’ of illegal activities, that is, a large proportion of crimes are likely to go undiscovered (Biderman and Reiss 1967 ; Lemieux 2014 ). This presents a significant problem for primary studies of intervention, and subsequent systematic reviews. It makes the collection of accurate primary data a complex but important task for future research to better assess and understand the impact of illegal activities involving species.

Age of existing studies —across studies, the methods employed, and the accuracy of the data varied significantly. The studies included in this review were published between 1990 and 2016, with the data originating from the 1970s onwards. The age of the data and the age of some of the studies means the results cannot be considered an up to date representation of the effect of patrolling for the current prevention of crime against species. More up to date outcome evaluations are needed to establish the effectiveness of prevention techniques internationally.

Changes in practices —as increasing funding and technology has been channelled to anti-poaching patrols since the 1970s, the systematic nature of record keeping has steadily improved. Researchers must take this into account when comparing data from one period, where rangers were using paper maps and notebooks, to another, where GPS equipment, drones and other technology were used. Any observed differences concerning, for example, the detection rates between such periods may reflect better detection, or more accurate data-recording. In addition, differences between areas may be due to variable access to such technology, which is unlikely to be ubiquitous across places.

Variation in terminology —the terminology used in different fields of research has led to a situation where some keywords have become used generically to describe a multitude of scenarios from disparate fields of research. Several conservation studies discussed activities such as ‘poaching’ and ‘by-catch’ as being one of many elements impacting the local ecology. However, the focus of their research, whilst aiming to benefit the local ecology in general, did not focus specifically on preventing wildlife crimes. Studies relating to conservation may have dealt with issues that were detrimental to the environment but were not technically illegal. As the focus of this review was to establish what interventions exist to prevent terrestrial wildlife crime and how effective these methods are, the conservation studies initially identified did not ultimately meet the inclusion criteria.

Variation in legal/illegal activities —actions that impact upon species may be deemed illegal in one country but not in another. Due to differences in law between countries, the authors of this review selected articles where prevention methods were being used to tackle illicit activities against species. Kurland et al ( 2017 ) incorporated both conservation and crime prevention methods, in a literature review. This systematic review could be supplemented with information from other systematic reviews of methods of prevention/intervention techniques focused solely on wildlife conservation. By encouraging the production and updating of reviews focused on TS crime, researchers and decision makers will have a larger quantitative and qualitative data set on the effectiveness of methods for protecting terrestrial species.

Diversity of prevention methods —in their analysis of a decade of projects funded by The Tiger Funds, Gratwicke et al. ( 2007 ) argued that the variety of intervention types and methodologies used were too diverse for them to effectively conduct a meta-analysis. The same can be said here. The validity of future work depends on standardising as far as possible the data (e.g. including recording practices) and analytic approaches taken in primary studies to make it possible for future systematic reviews to include a quantitative and qualitative synthesis.

Funding —Limited funding for projects relating to wildlife crime is a continuing issue internationally. Investments have been made in recent years to tackle the problem of international wildlife crime, but continued financial support is not guaranteed. Moreover, the majority of the research undertaken to date has concerned conservation. Whilst conservation studies are a useful source of information relating to the topic of interest, conservation studies tend to indirectly examine the impact of interventions on crime associated with the international wildlife trade. Clearly, future work that seeks to also examine the latter will be necessary if we are to learn what works to reduce this form of offending.

The expediency of solutions is one of the major issues with transposing ideas for tackling crime. Whilst technological solutions may theoretically provide some deterrence and detection benefits, it is only when there is sufficient funding for training, deployment, operation and maintenance, that such prevention methods are truly feasible. And, without evidence to show that particular approaches work, limited resources may be squandered on good ideas that fail to reduce crime or costs effectively.

Publication Bias —publication bias is a complex issue in relation to conservation research internationally. The most prominent countries in relation to biodiversity and conservation, are also those with developing economies. A study carried out by Fazey et al. ( 2005 ) examined the main barriers that prevent conservation research conducted in developing countries from reaching international audiences. These included language barriers, as well as access to technology and funding to be used to conduct and publish the results of the research. Unless these issues are addressed valuable research and potential solutions to crimes involving TS may continue to be overlooked.

The conservation and crime science fields should actively seek to support the retroactive publication of research that has to this date not been published due to the issues described above, and provide a platform by which research and researchers from around the world can present their findings. As books, journals and subsequently research databases incorporate a wider variety of text from international sources, there is likely to be more relevant data and literature available, therefore, the authors hope that future systematic reviews will incorporate literature that has at this time been omitted due to the publication bias described above.

Displacement of illegal activity —the displacement of illegal activity was not discussed directly in any of the studies reviewed but alluded to as an important avenue for future research. Identifying the impact of interventions in areas beyond the focus of the intervention would indicate whether poaching activity is being actively reduced by patrolling or is being spatially displaced to nearby locations (Linkie et al. 2015 ). In addition to spatial displacement, target displacement where criminals may choose to target other species should also be considered in future research. In the case of urban crime, it has been shown that police patrols do not appear to displace crime (see Bowers et al. 2011 ), but context matters and this may not be the case for poaching (Johnson et al. 2014 ).

Interrater-reliability —The identification of eligible articles and the extraction of information was performed by the main author, without additional checks completed by the other authors to establish if the original filtering and data extraction were performed accurately. The Systematic Review would have benefited from other individuals performing data identification and extraction to assess interrater reliability (Belur et al. 2018 ).

Future research into the effectiveness of Situational Crime Prevention techniques for preventing crimes against species, should not only look at assessing the effectiveness of novel technological solutions, but also review methods already being used that are assumed to be effective, but for which there exists no supporting evidence. Once more outcome evaluations as to the effectiveness of SCP methods exist in the literature relating to species crime, a more comprehensive systematic review can be carried out to update stakeholders on the existing literature and evidence to support decision making.

The articles identified in this review provide an insight into the difficulties faced by various stakeholders in identifying the most applicable methods for preventing crime against species. It should be clear from the small number of articles that were included in this review that there is very little research on what works to prevent species crime. This issue could be addressed in two ways: Firstly, through retrospective publication of assessment research not readily available. Secondly, through conducting new research designed to assess the effectiveness of existing and proposed prevention measures. The impact of crimes against species, such as dwindling numbers and impending extinctions that were described in the included research (written over three decades ago) remains a significant issue that needs to be addressed. Many organisations are devoted to preventing the extinction of iconic species internationally, often involving significant financial investment, but research informing or evaluating their impact is lacking. Without more empirical evidence to present to such conservation organisations about the effectiveness of prevention methods, it will be a continuing challenge to justify the need for funding and supporting prevention efforts, such as community outreach and patrolling. With increasing financial pressure, the limited evidence to support current prevention techniques and developing new methods, the challenge to prevent the extinction of species is likely to continue.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Belur, J., Tompson, L., Thornton, A., & Simon, M. (2018). Interrater reliability in systematic review methodology: exploring variation in coder decision-making. Sociological Methods & Research . https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124118799372 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Biderman, A. D., & Reiss, A. J., Jr. (1967). On exploring the" dark figure" of crime. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 374 (1), 1–15.

Blattner, C. E. (2019). The recognition of animal sentience by the law. Journal of Animal Ethics, 9 (2), 121–136.

Borrion, H., Amiri, A., Delpech, D., & Lemieux, A. M. (2019). Experimental assessment of the viability of using ground penetrating radar for metal wire-snare detection. Crime Science, 8 (1), 1–10.

Bowers, K. J., Johnson, S. D., Guerette, R. T., Summers, L., & Poynton, S. (2011). Spatial displacement and diffusion of benefits among geographically focused policing initiatives: a meta-analytical review. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 7 (4), 347–374.

Cooney, R., Roe, D., Dublin, H., Phelps, J., Wilkie, D., Keane, A., & Biggs, D. (2017). From poachers to protectors: engaging local communities in solutions to illegal wildlife trade. Conservation Letters, 10 (3), 367–374.

Cornish, D. B. (1994). The procedural analysis of offending and its relevance for situational prevention. Crime prevention studies, 3, 151–196.

Google Scholar  

Cornish, D. B., & Clarke, R. V. (2003). Opportunities, precipitators and criminal decisions: a reply to Wortley’s critique of situational crime prevention. Crime Prevention Studies, 16, 41–96.

Daerr, E. G. (2001). Parks attract ginseng poachers. National Parks (p. 12). Ann Arbor: ProQuest Central.

Donnermeyer, J. F., & Barclay, E. (2005). The policing of farm crime 1. Police Practice and Research, 6 (1), 3–17.

Driscoll, C. A., Macdonald, D. W., & O’Brien, S. J. (2009). From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 (Supplement 1), 9971–9978.

Fazey, I., Fischer, J., & Lindenmayer, D. B. (2005). Who does all the research in conservation biology? Biodiversity & Conservation, 14 (4), 917–934.

Gratwicke, B., Seidensticker, J., Shrestha, M., Vermilye, K., & Birnbaum, M. (2007). Evaluating the performance of a decade of Save The Tiger Fund’s investments to save the world’s last wild tigers. Environmental Conservation, 34 (03), 255–265.

Hilborn, R., Arcese, P., Borner, M., Hando, J., Hopcraft, G., Loibooki, M., & Sinclair, A. R. (2006). Effective enforcement in a conservation area. Science, 314 (5803), 1266–1266.

Jachmann, H., & Billiouw, M. (1997). Elephant poaching and law enforcement in the central Luangwa Valley, Zambia. Journal of Applied Ecology, 34, 233–244.

Johnson, S. D., Guerette, R. T., & Bowers, K. (2014). Crime displacement: what we know, what we don’t know, and what it means for crime reduction. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 10 (4), 549–571.

Johnson, S. D., Tilley, N., & Bowers, K. J. (2015). Introducing EMMIE: an evidence rating scale to encourage mixed-method crime prevention synthesis reviews. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 11 (3), 459–473.

Kurland, J., Pires, S. F., McFann, S. C., & Moreto, W. D. (2017). Wildlife crime: a conceptual integration, literature review, and methodological critique. Crime Science, 6 (1), 4.

Larkcom, E., & Delpech, R. (2013). Biology for Edexcel Internation GCSE . London: Hodder Education.

Leader-Williams, N., Albon, S. D., & Berry, P. S. M. (1990). Illegal exploitation of black rhinoceros and elephant populations: patterns of decline, law enforcement and patrol effort in Luangwa Valley, Zambia. Journal of Applied Ecology, 27, 1055–1087.

Lemieux, A. M. (Ed.). (2014). Situational prevention of poaching . London: Routledge.

Lemieux, A. M. (Ed.). (2020). The Poaching Diaries (vol. 1): Crime Scripting for Wilderness Problems (pp. 108–109). Phoenix, AZ: Center for Problem Oriented Policing, Arizona State University.

Linkie, M., Martyr, D. J., Harihar, A., Risdianto, D., Nugraha, R. T., Leader Nugrahaugraha, Nu., & Wong, W. M. (2015). EDITOR’S CHOICE: safeguarding Sumatran tigers: evaluating effectiveness of law enforcement patrols and local informant networks. Journal of Applied Ecology, 52 (4), 851–860.

Lynch, M. J., Barrett, K. L., Stretesky, P. B., & Long, M. A. (2017). The neglect of quantitative research in green criminology and its consequences. Critical Criminology, 25, 1–16.

Martin, E., Martin, C., & Vigne, L. (2013). Successful reduction in rhino poaching in Nepal. Pachyderm, 54, 66–73.

Maruna, S., & Copes, H. (2005). What have we learned from five decades of neutralization research? Crime and Justice , 221–320.

Miranda Montero, J. J., Wright, E. M., & Khan, M. N. (2019). Illegal logging, fishing, and wildlife trade: the costs and how to combat it (pp. 1–70). Washington, DC: The World Bank.

Mlinarić, A., Horvat, M., & Šupak Smolčić, V. (2017). Dealing with the positive publication bias: why you should really publish your negative results. Biochemia medica: Biochemia medica, 27 (3), 1–6.

Moreto, W. D., Lemieux, A. M., Rwetsiba, A., Guma, N., Driciru, M., & Kirya, H. K. (2014). Law enforcement monitoring in Uganda. In A. M. Lemieux (Ed.), Situational Prevention of Poaching (pp. 82–101). London: Routledge.

NFU Mutual. (2019). Rural Crime Report 2019. https://www.nfumutual.co.uk/farming/ruralcrime/

Nellemann, C., Henriksen, R., Raxter, P., Ash, N., & Mrema, E. (2014). The environmental crime crisis: threats to sustainable development from illegal exploitation and trade in wildlife and forest resources. A UNEP Rapid Response Assessment . Nairobi: United Nations Environment Programme.

Nurse, A. (2016). Beyond the property debate: animal welfare as a public good. Contemporary Justice Review, 19 (2), 174–187.

PANIU. (2015a). Plant & Agricultural National Intelligence Unit—Metropolitan Police PANIU 24th Report. http://content.met.police.uk/Article/Quarterly-reports/1400030610576/1400030610576

PANIU. (2015b). Plant & Agricultural National Intelligence Unit—Metropolitan Police PANIU 7 Year Report. http://content.met.police.uk/Article/Quarterly-reports/1400030610576/1400030610576

Petticrew, M. (2001). Systematic reviews from astronomy to zoology: myths and misconceptions. British Medical Journal, 322 (7278), 98.

Pires, S. F., & Moreto, W. D. (2011). Preventing wildlife crimes: solutions that can overcome the ‘Tragedy of the Commons.’ European Journal on Criminal Policy and Research, 17 (2), 101–123.

Sidebottom, A. (2013). On the application of CRAVED to livestock theft in Malawi. International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 37 (3), 195–212.

Sollund, R. (2016). The animal other: legal and illegal theriocide. In Greening criminology in the 21st century (pp. 93–113). London: Routledge.

Steinmetz, R., Srirattanaporn, S., & Mor-Tip, & Seuaturien, N. . (2014). Can community outreach alleviate poaching pressure and recover wildlife in South-East Asian protected areas? Journal of Applied Ecology, 51 (6), 1469–1478.

Swanson, C. R. (1981). Rural and agricultural crime. Journal of Criminal Justice, 9 (1), 19–27.

Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: a theory of delinquency. American sociological review, 22 (6), 664–670.

UN News. (2016). UN-backed fund expands wildlife protection plan to 19 countries in Africa and Asia. http://www.un.org/apps/news/story.asp?NewsID=54193#.WOT6WLGZOt8

UNODC. (2017). Global Programme for Combatting Wildlife and Forest Crime. https://www.unodc.org/unodc/en/wildlife-and-forest-crime/global-programme.html

White, R. (2013). Crimes against nature: environmental criminology and ecological justice . Cullompton: Willan.

Book   Google Scholar  

What Works. (2015). Neighbourhood Watch. http://whatworks.college.police.uk/toolkit/Pages/Intervention.aspx?InterventionID=8

WWF. (2017). Wildlife crime initiative. http://wwf.panda.org/what_we_do/how_we_work/our_global_goals/species_programme/wildlife_trade/wildlife_crime_initiative/

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Thank you to the EPSRC for providing funding to support the PhD research that provide the basis for this article. No other outside funding was used to support this work.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Security & Crime Science, University College London (UCL), 35 Tavistock Square, London, WC1H 9EZ, UK

Dorothea Delpech, Herve Borrion & Shane Johnson

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dorothea Delpech .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate, consent for publication, competing interests.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Delpech, D., Borrion, H. & Johnson, S. Systematic review of situational prevention methods for crime against species. Crime Sci 10 , 1 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40163-020-00138-1

Download citation

Received : 29 June 2020

Accepted : 17 December 2020

Published : 06 January 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40163-020-00138-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Community Outreach

Crime Science

ISSN: 2193-7680

crime prevention literature review

Advertisement

Advertisement

When crime prevention harms: a review of systematic reviews

  • Published: 21 January 2014
  • Volume 10 , pages 245–266, ( 2014 )

Cite this article

crime prevention literature review

  • Brandon C. Welsh 1 , 2 , 5 &
  • Michael Rocque 3 , 4  

4350 Accesses

54 Citations

29 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

In a series of important scholarly works, Joan McCord made the case for the criminological community to take seriously harmful effects arising from individual-based crime prevention programs. Building on these works, two key questions are of central interest to this paper: What has been the state of research on harmful effects of these crime prevention programs since McCord’s works? And what are the theoretical, methodological, and programmatic characteristics of individual-based crime prevention programs with reported harmful effects?

This paper reports on the first empirical review of harmful effects of crime prevention programs, drawing upon 15 Campbell Collaboration systematic reviews. Altogether, 574 experimental and quasi-experimental studies (published and unpublished) with 645 independent effect sizes were reviewed.

A total of 22 harmful effects from 22 unique studies of individual-based crime prevention programs were identified. Almost all of the studies have been reported since 1990, all but 2 were carried out in the United States, and two-thirds can be considered unpublished. The studies covered a wide range of interventions, from anti-bullying programs at schools, to second responder interventions involving police, to the Scared Straight program for juvenile delinquents, with more than half taking place in criminal justice settings. Boot camps and drug courts accounted for the largest share of studies with harmful effects.

Conclusions

Theory failure, implementation failure, and deviancy training were identified as the leading explanations for harmful effects of crime prevention programs, and they served as key anchors for a more focused look at implications for theory and policy. Also, the need for programs to be rigorously evaluated and monitored is evident, which will advance McCord’s call for attention to safety and efficacy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

The continued importance of measuring potentially harmful impacts of crime prevention programs: the academy of experimental criminology 2014 joan mccord lecture.

crime prevention literature review

Conclusion: What Works in Crime Prevention Revisited

crime prevention literature review

Preventing future offending of delinquents and offenders: what have we learned from experiments and meta-analyses?

In addition to her academic writings on these topics, McCord was a founding member of the Crime and Justice Steering Committee of the Campbell Collaboration and a president of the Academy of Experimental Criminology.

Publication dates for these reviews range from 2005 to 2012, with 28 published between 2008 and 2012.

As noted by Petrosino et al. ( 2010 , p. 13), “Because the system processing condition is usually the control group in the experiments, it is often not described further.”

All the 574 studies met the criteria for inclusion in the systematic reviews that we consulted, and all the effect sizes were based on treatment versus control comparisons.

References marked with an asterisk (*) are studies with harmful effects included in the review.

Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2010). The psychology of criminal conduct (5th ed.). New Providence: Matthew Bender.

Google Scholar  

Boisjoli, R., Vitaro, F., Lacourse, E., Barker, E. D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Impact and clinical significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys: 15-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 191 , 415–419.

Article   Google Scholar  

*Bonta, J., Wallace-Capretta, S., & Rooney, J. (2000). A quasi-experimental evaluation of an intensive rehabilitation supervision program. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27 , 312–329.

*Boyles, C. E., Bokenkamp, E., & Madura, W. (1996). Evaluation of the Colorado juvenile regimented training program . Golden: Colorado Department of Human Services, Division of Youth Corrections.

Braga, A. A., & Weisburd, D. L. (2012). The effects of focused deterrence strategies on crime: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 49 , 323–358.

Braga, A. A., Papachristos, A. V., & Hureau, D. M. (2012). The effects of hot spots policing on crime: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Justice Quarterly . doi: 10.1080/07418825.2012.673632 .

Cécile, M., & Born, M. (2009). Intervention in juvenile delinquency: Danger of iatrogenic effects? Children and Youth Services Review, 31 , 1217–1221.

Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings . Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Cullen, F. T. (2013). Rehabilitation: Beyond nothing works. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice in America, 1975–2025 (pp. 299–376). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

*Davis, R. C., & Medina, J. (2001). Results from an elder abuse prevention experiment in New York City. National Institute of Justice Research in Brief . Washington: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.

Dishion, T. J., & Dodge, K. A. (2005). Peer contagion in interventions for children and adolescents: Moving toward an understanding of the ecology and dynamics of change. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33 , 395–400.

Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist, 54 , 755–764.

Dodge, K. A., Dishion, T. J., & Lansford, J. E. (Eds.). (2005). Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: Problems and solutions . New York: Guilford.

Dodge, K. A., Dishion, T. J., & Lansford, J. E. (2006). Deviant peer influences in intervention and public policy for youth. Social Policy Report, 20 , 1–19.

Eckenrode, J., Campa, M., Luckey, D. W., Henderson, C. R., Cole, R., Kitzman, H., et al. (2010). Long-term effects of prenatal and infancy nurse home visitation on the life course of youths: 19-year follow-up a randomized trial. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164 , 9–15.

Ekblom, P., & Pease, K. (1995). Evaluating crime prevention. In M. Tonry & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Building a safer society: Strategic approaches to crime prevention (pp. 585–662). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Farrington, D. P., & Welsh, B. C. (2006). A half century of randomized experiments on crime and justice. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice: A review of research (Vol. 34, pp. 55–132). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Farrington, D. P., Weisburd, D., & Gill, C. E. (2011). The Campbell Collaboration Crime and Justice Group: A decade of progress. In C. J. Smith, S. X. Zhang, & R. Barberet (Eds.), Routledge handbook of international criminology (pp. 53–63). New York: Routledge.

*Finckenauer, J. O. (1982). Scared Straight and the panacea phenomenon . Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

Gatti, U., Tremblay, R. E., & Vitaro, F. (2009). Iatrogenic effect of juvenile justice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50 , 991–998.

*Gilbertson, T. (2009). 2008 DWI court evaluation report . Bemidji: Criminal Justice Department, Bemidji State University.

Gottfredson, D. C. (2010). Deviancy training: Understanding how preventive interventions harm: The Academy of Experimental Criminology 2009 Joan McCord Award lecture. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 6 , 229–243.

*Gransky, L. A., & Jones, R. J. (1995). Evaluation of the post-release status of substance abuse program participants . Chicago: Illinois Criminal Justice Information Authority.

Green, J. (2000). The role of theory in evidence-based health promotion practice. Health Education Research, 15 , 125–129.

Greene, M. B. (2004). Implications of research showing harmful effects of group activities with anti-social adolescents. In Persistently safe schools: The national conference of the Hamilton Fish Institute on school and community violence (pp. 73–83). Washington, DC: George Washington University.

Greenwood, P. W. (2006). Promising solutions in juvenile justice. In K. A. Dodge, T. J. Dishion, & J. E. Lansford (Eds.), Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: Problems and solutions (pp. 278–295). New York: Guilford.

Greenwood, P. W., & Welsh, B. C. (2012). Promoting evidence-based practice in delinquency prevention at the state level: Principles, progress, and policy directions. Criminology and Public Policy, 11 , 491–513.

Handwerk, M. L., Field, C. E., & Friman, P. C. (2000). The iatrogenic effects of group intervention for antisocial youth: Premature extrapolations? Journal of Behavioral Education, 10 , 223–238.

*Harrell, A. (1991). Evaluation of court-ordered treatment for domestic violence offenders. Final report. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.

Hough, M. (2010). Gold standard or fool’s gold? The pursuit of certainty in experimental criminology. Criminology and Criminal Justice, 10 , 11–22.

*Hovell, M. F., Seid, A. G., & Liles, S. (2006). Evaluation of a police and social services domestic violence program: Empirical evidence needed to inform public health policies. Violence Against Women, 12 , 137–159.

Johnson, B. R., De Li, S., Larson, D. B., & McCullough, M. (2000). A systematic review of the religiosity and delinquency literature: A research note. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 16 , 32–52.

*Jones, M., & Ross, D. L. (1997). Is less better? Boot camp, regular probation and rearrest in North Carolina. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 21 , 147–161.

Lillienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2 , 53–70.

Lopes, G., Krohn, M. D., Lizotte, A. J., Schmidt, N. M., Vasquez, B. E., & Bernberg, J. G. (2012). Labeling and cumulative disadvantage: The impact of formal police intervention on life chances and crime during emerging adulthood. Crime and Delinquency, 58 , 456–488.

MacKenzie, D. L. (2006). What works in corrections: Reducing the criminal activities of offenders and delinquents . New York: Cambridge University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

MacKenzie, D. L. (2012). Preventing future criminal activities of delinquents and offenders. In B. C. Welsh & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of crime prevention (pp. 466–486). New York: Oxford University Press.

MacKenzie, D. L. (2013). First do no harm: A look at correctional policies and programs today: The 2011 Joan McCord Prize lecture. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 9 , 1–17.

*MacKenzie, D. L., & Shaw, J. W. (1993). The impact of shock incarceration on technical violations and new criminal activities. Justice Quarterly, 10 , 463–487.

*MacKenzie, D. L., & Souryal, C. (1994). Multi-site evaluation of shock incarceration: Executive summary . Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.

MacKenzie, D. L., Wilson, D. B., & Kider, S. B. (2001). Effects of correctional boot camps on offending. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 578 , 126–143.

McCord, J. (1978). A thirty-year follow-up of treatment effects. American Psychologist, 33 , 284–289.

McCord, J. (2002). Counterproductive juvenile justice. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 35 , 230–237.

McCord, J. (2003). Cures that harm: Unanticipated outcomes of crime prevention programs. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 587 , 16–30.

*Meekins, B. J. (2003). Deterrence in the drug court setting: Case study and quasi experiment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation . Alexandria: University of Virginia.

*Michigan Department of Corrections (1967). A six month follow-up of juvenile delinquents visiting the Ionia Reformatory. Research Report No. 4. Lansing, MI: Michigan Department of Corrections.

Mitchell, O., Wilson, D. B., Eggers, A., & MacKenzie, D. L. (2011). Drug courts’ effects on criminal offending for juveniles and adults. Campbell Collaboration . doi: 10.4073/csr.2012.4 .

Mitchell, O., Wilson, D. B., Eggers, A., & MacKenzie, D. L. (2012a). Assessing the effectiveness of drug courts on recidivism: A meta-analytic review of traditional and non-traditional drug courts. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40 , 60–71.

Mitchell, O., Wilson, D. B., & MacKenzie, D. L. (2012b). The effectiveness of incarceration-based drug treatment on criminal behavior: A systematic review. Campbell Collaboration . doi: 10.4073/csr.2012.18 .

*NPC Research. (2009). Baltimore City Circuit Court adult treatment court and felony diversion initiative: Outcome and cost evaluation . Portland: NPC Research.

Paternoster, R., & Iovanni, L. (1989). The labeling perspective and delinquency: An elaboration of the theory and an assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly, 6 , 359–394.

Petrosino, A., & Lavenberg, J. (2007). Systematic reviews and meta-analyses: Best evidence on ‘what works’ for criminal justice decision makers. Western Criminology Review, 8 , 1–15.

Petrosino, A., Turpin-Petrosino, C., & Buehler, J. (2004). “Scared Straight” and other juvenile awareness programs for preventing juvenile delinquency. Campbell Collaboration . doi: 10.4073/csr.2004.2 .

Petrosino, A., Turpin-Petrosino, C., & Guckenburg, S. (2010). Formal system processing of juveniles: Effects on delinquency. Campbell Collaboration . doi: 10.4073/csr.2010.1 .

Petticrew, M., & Roberts, H. (2006). Systematic reviews in the social sciences: A practical guide . Malden: Blackwell.

*Porter, R. (2002). Breaking the cycle: Technical report . New York: Vera Institute of Justice.

Poulin, F., Dishion, D. J., & Burraston, B. (2001). 3-year iatrogenic effects associated with aggressive high-risk adolescents in cognitive-behavioral preventive interventions. Applied Developmental Science, 5 , 214–224.

Rhule, D. M. (2005). Take care to do no harm: Harmful interventions for youth problem behavior. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36 , 618–625.

Rorie, M., Gottfredson, D. C., Cross, A., Wilson, D., & Connell, N. (2011). Structure and deviancy training in after-school programs. Journal of Adolescence, 34 , 105–117.

*Rosenbluth, B., Whitaker, D. J., Sanchez, E., & Valle, L. A. (2004). The Expect Respect Project: Preventing bullying and sexual harassment in US elementary schools. In P. K. Smith, D. Pepler, & K. Rigby (Eds.), Bullying in schools: How successful can interventions be? (pp. 211–233). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The ‘file drawer problem’ and tolerance of null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86 , 638–641.

Sampson, R. J. (2010). Gold standard myths: Observations on the experimental turn in quantitative criminology. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 26 , 489–500.

Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1997). A life-course theory of cumulative disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In T. P. Thornberry (Ed.), Developmental theories of crime and delinquency . New Brunswick: Transaction.

*Scarpitti, F. R., Butzin, C. A., Saum, C. A., Gray, A. R., & Leigey, M. E. (2005). Drug court offenders in outpatient treatment: Final report to National Institute of Drug Abuse . Newark: University of Delaware.

Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference . Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Sherman, L. W. (2003). Preface: Misleading evidence and evidence-led policy: Making social science more experimental. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 589 , 6–19.

Sherman, L. W. (2007). The power few: Experimental criminology and the reduction of harm. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 3 , 299–321.

Sherman, L. W., Farrington, D. P., Welsh, B. C., & MacKenzie, D. L. (Eds.). (2006). Evidence-based crime prevention (rev. ed.). New York: Routledge.

*Siegal, H. A., Wang, J., Falck, R. S., Rahman, A. M., & Carlson, R. G. (1997). An evaluation of Ohio's prison-based therapeutic community treatment programs for substance abusers: Final report . Dayton: School of Medicine, Wright State University.

*State of New York Department of Correctional Services Division of Parole (NY DCS). (2003). The fifteenth annual shock legislative report . New York: Department of Correctional Services and the Division of Parole.

*Terry, W. C., III. (1995). Repeat offenses of the first year cohort of Broward County, Florida’s drug court . Miami: Florida International University.

Tremblay, R. E., McCord, J., Boileau, H., Charlebois, P., Gagnon, C., Le Blanc, M., et al. (1991). Can disruptive boys be helped to become competent? Psychiatry, 54 , 148–161.

*Vaughn, M. S., Deng, F., & Lee, L. J. (2003). Evaluating a prison-based drug treatment program in Taiwan. Journal of Drug Issues, 33 , 357–384.

Weisburd, D. (2010). Justifying the use of non-experimental methods and disqualifying the use of randomized controlled trials: Challenging the folklore in evaluation research in crime and justice. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 6 , 209–227.

Weisburd, D., & Hinkle, J. (2012). The Importance of randomized experiments in evaluating crime prevention. In B. C. Welsh & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of crime prevention (pp. 446–465). New York: Oxford University Press.

Weiss, B., Caron, A., Ball, S., Tapp, J., Johnson, J., & Weisz, J. R. (2005). Iatrogenic effects of group treatment for antisocial youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73 , 1036–1044.

Welsh, B. C., & Farrington, D. P. (2012a). Science, politics, and crime prevention: Toward a new crime policy. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40 , 128–133.

Welsh, B. C., & Farrington, D. P. (Eds.). (2012b). The Oxford handbook of crime prevention . New York: Oxford University Press.

Werch, C. E., & Owen, D. M. (2002). Iatrogenic effects of alcohol and drug prevention programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63 , 581–590.

Wilson, D. B. (2009). Missing a critical piece of the pie: Simple document search strategies inadequate for systematic reviews. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 5 , 429–440.

Wilson, D. B., MacKenzie, D. L., & Mitchell, F. N. (2008). Effects of correctional boot camps on offending. Campbell Collaboration . doi: 10.4073/csr.2003.1 .

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to David Wilson and the anonymous reviewers for helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA

Brandon C. Welsh

Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Maine Department of Corrections, Augusta, ME, USA

Michael Rocque

University of Maine, Orono, ME, USA

School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Northeastern University, Churchill Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA, 02115, USA

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brandon C. Welsh .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Welsh, B.C., Rocque, M. When crime prevention harms: a review of systematic reviews. J Exp Criminol 10 , 245–266 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-014-9199-2

Download citation

Published : 21 January 2014

Issue Date : September 2014

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-014-9199-2

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Crime prevention
  • Harmful effect
  • Systematic review
  • High quality evaluation
  • Theory failure
  • Implementation failure
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government, Department of Justice.

Here's how you know

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock A locked padlock ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

NCJRS Virtual Library

Prevention of crime and delinquency - a review of the literature, additional details.

S.I. Newhouse Ctr at Rutgers , 15 Washington St., Fourth Floor , Newark , NJ 07102 , United States

No download available

Availability, related topics.

  • English (United States) Select this as your preferred language
  • WIC Program
  • Food Protection for Businesses
  • Submit a Request or Report (311)
  • Pay a Healthcare Bill
  • Pay for Parking
  • Pay for a Permit or License
  • Find My Collection Day
  • Household Trash Collection
  • Bulk Collection
  • Yard Waste Collection
  • Plan Your Own Litter Cleanup
  • How to Dispose or Recycle
  • Housing Programs
  • Financial Assistance Programs
  • Apply for Residential Tax Incentives
  • Apply for a Short Term Rental Permit
  • Power Outage Map
  • Report a Water Main Break
  • About Public Utilities
  • Download the Mobile Parking App
  • Parking Services
  • Report a Pothole
  • Find Street & Road Projects
  • Get a Birth or Death Certificate
  • Medical Records Information
  • Make a Payment
  • Report an Illness or Problem
  • Find Healthcare Resources
  • Environmental Safety
  • Health Inspection Results
  • Public Health Careers
  • About Public Health
  • File a Police Report
  • Find a Police Report
  • Division of Police
  • Division of Fire
  • Police & Fire Careers
  • Alternative Crisis Response
  • Emergency Communications Center
  • Department of Public Safety Resources
  • City of Columbus Recreation & Parks
  • Parks & Trails
  • Activities & Events
  • Upcoming Events
  • Find a Program
  • Volunteer Opportunities
  • Facilities & Venues
  • Reserve a Shelter
  • Get an Animal Permit
  • Dog Tag Search
  • One Health Intern Program
  • Rat Control Program
  • Search the Knowledge Base
  • Healthy Places
  • Project Blueprint
  • Columbus Cares Coalition
  • Keep Columbus Beautiful
  • What's in My Neighborhood?
  • Area Commission Map
  • New Resident Information
  • Public Transportation
  • Bicycle Registration
  • Data and Interactive Maps (GIS)
  • Get or Track a Building Permit
  • Get a Right of Way Permit
  • Get a Short Term Rental Permit
  • Online Services
  • Documents & Forms
  • Code Enforcement
  • BZS Frequently Asked Questions
  • Boards and Commissions
  • Division of Weights & Measures
  • Contractor Licenses
  • Business Resources
  • Notices to Bid
  • Vendor Resources
  • SmartColumbus
  • Business Attraction & Expansion
  • Infrastructure Improvement
  • Small Business
  • Site Selection Webpage
  • Accelerate Columbus
  • Power Circuit Project
  • Stormwater CIPS
  • Blueprint CIPs
  • Citizen Access Portal
  • City Codes & Regulations
  • Street & Road Projects
  • Upcoming Contracts
  • Project Information
  • Public Service Project Fact Sheets
  • Infrastructure Management
  • About Mayor Andrew J. Ginther
  • Press Releases
  • City Boards, Commissions & Committees
  • Initiatives
  • Sustainable Columbus
  • Office of Diversity and Inclusion
  • Executive Orders
  • Watch Meetings (CTV)
  • Meeting Schedules & Agendas
  • News Releases & Information
  • Councilmembers
  • Standing Committees & Commissions
  • Council Residential Districts
  • City Treasurer
  • About the City Auditor
  • About This Office
  • AAA Bond Rating
  • Bond and Note Reports
  • Office News
  • PAFR Reports
  • Financial Reports
  • Income Tax Division
  • Civilian Police Review Board
  • City Attorney
  • Municipal Court
  • Civil Service Exam Information
  • Police Jobs
  • Firefighter Jobs
  • All Public Service Jobs
  • Public Health Jobs
  • Municipal Court Clerk
  • Departments
  • Watch CTV Live Stream
  • Ethics & Campaign Finance
  • Download the Mobile App
  • Account Services

Crime Prevention Tips and Literature

Crime prevention programs regarding block watches, security surveys, workplace violence reduction and Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design (CPTED) are all available through the Community Liaison Section. Contact your Community Liaison Officer to discuss and schedule the different services that can be provided.

Upon request, Community Liaison Officers, can speak at your Block-Watch, Community Civic Association, groups/organizations regarding crime problems. Community Liaison Officers may also provide published general information about specific crimes or tailor or assist in tailoring programs to address community concerns of a specific or unusual crime pattern.

Crime Prevention Literature

  • Automatic Teller Machine (PDF, 115KB)
  • Blockwatch Handbook (PDF, 352KB)
  • Care to add a Little Spice to your Blockwatch? (PDF, 168KB)
  • Carjacking (PDF, 129KB)
  • Children's Gun Safety (PDF, 206KB)
  • Common Court Terminology - Spanish (PDF, 30KB)
  • Common Court Terminology - Somali (PDF, 37KB)
  • Confidence Games and Swindles (PDF, 265KB)
  • Contact Numbers and Web Sites (PDF, 138KB)
  • Crime Prevention through Environmental Design (PDF, 97KB)
  • Criminal Complaint Info - Spanish (PDF, 72KB)
  • Criminal Complaint Info - Somali (PDF, 103KB)
  • Crisis Referral List (PDF, 116KB)
  • Defenses Against Sexual Assault and Rape (PDF, 101KB)
  • Family Travel Security (PDF, 116KB)
  • Graffiti Information (PDF, 162KB)
  • Hate Crimes - Spanish (PDF, 327KB)
  • Information on Protection Orders - Spanish (PDF, 32KB)
  • Information on Protection Orders - Somali (PDF, 33KB)
  • Know your Neighbors, Know your Neighborhood (PDF, 123KB)
  • National Blockwatch Manual (PDF, 7MB)
  • Operation Identification (PDF, 44KB)
  • Personal Safety (PDF, 131KB)
  • Preparing your Home for Vacation (PDF, 228KB)
  • Residential Security (PDF, 951KB)
  • Robbery Risk Reduction for Businesses (PDF, 65KB)
  • Steps in Prosecution of a Felony - Spanish (PDF, 76KB)
  • Steps in Prosecution of a Felony - Somali (PDF, 70KB)
  • Steps in Prosecution of a Misdemeanor - Spanish (PDF, 66KB)
  • Steps in Prosecution of a Misdemeanor - Somali (PDF, 64KB)
  • Street Gang and Family Life (PDF, 136KB)
  • Summer Safety Tips (PDF, 119KB)
  • Theft from Auto Prevention (PDF, 83KB)
  • Tips for Handling Panhandlers (PDF, 152KB)
  • Top 10 tips (PDF, 119KB)
  • Victim of Domestic Violence, Stalking and Sexual Assault - Spanish (PDF, 40KB)
  • Victim of Domestic Violence, Stalking and Sexual Assault - Somali (PDF, 39KB)
  • What to do if stopped by an officer - Spanish (PDF, 42KB)
  • What to do if stopped by an officer - Somali (PDF, 39KB)

Related Links

Homicides/ Unsolved Murders

Police Media Releases

Victim's Services

Central Ohio Crime Stoppers

Report Suspected Terrorist Activity

Public Records Request

9-1-1 Emergency (PDF, 359KB)

File a Report

File an Offense Report

File an Accident Report

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Towards Crime Prevention Using Big Data Analytics: A Literature

    crime prevention literature review

  2. Crime Prevention: Approaches, Practices, and Evaluations 10th Edition

    crime prevention literature review

  3. What Works in Crime Prevention and Rehabilitation: Lessons from

    crime prevention literature review

  4. (PDF) Crime Prevention through Environmental Design (CPTED): A Review

    crime prevention literature review

  5. Crime Prevention Through Social Development: A Literature Review (1995

    crime prevention literature review

  6. (PDF) Information retrieval in systematic reviews: a case study of the

    crime prevention literature review

VIDEO

  1. SecurOS POS

  2. A Sociology Let's evaluate Crime Prevention strategies

  3. What Do These Retail Loss Prevention Terms Mean?

  4. Legitimacy of crime prevention wardens: Dr Mgcini Tshwaku

  5. Crime and Punishment

  6. CEO Interviews about Crime Prevention Training for Corporations

COMMENTS

  1. Artificial intelligence & crime prediction: A systematic literature review

    Consequently, the application of artificial intelligence (AI) in crime prediction is a significant and well-researched area. This study investigates AI strategies in crime prediction. We conduct a systematic literature review (SLR). Our review evaluates the models from numerous points of view, including the crime analysis type, crimes studied ...

  2. (PDF) A Review of the Effectiveness of Crime Prevention by Design

    In the USA, the National Crime Prevention Institute (1986, p.1-1), defines crime prevention as: "The anticipation, recognition and appraisal of a crime risk and the initiation of some action to ...

  3. PDF Street Violence Crime Reduction Strategies: A Review of the Evidence

    This literature review examines the available empirical evidence on a variety of police-led violence reduction strategies (offender-focused, place-based, and community- based), as well as community-led, public health-based violence prevention interventions.

  4. Prevention of Violent Crime: A Review of the Literature

    Conclusion. We have reviewed literature concerning prevention of violent crime and have identified the definitions and prevalence of violent crime, characteristics of both victims and offenders, modus operandi, and suggestions for effective strategies against it, as well as implications for policy and officer training.

  5. Crime and safety in rural areas: A systematic review of the English

    This systematic literature review follows and complements the existent compilations of rural crime literature by Hubbard et al. ... 2010s. One of the relatively earliest publications in our time frame (1980-2020) was a compilation of North American literature on rural crime prevention and criminal justice (Hubbard et al., ...

  6. A literature review on police patrolling problems

    Police patrol is an effective crime prevention tool and boosts public confidence in urban security. Many interesting decision making problems appear in route design, resource allocation and jurisdiction planning. Many cities across the world have adopted a structured and intelligent method of police patrol due to the presence of a variety of operational and resource constraints. In this paper ...

  7. (PDF) A Review and Current Status of Crime Prevention through

    Paul Cozens. 1. and Terence Love. 2. Abstract. This article reviews the current status of the concept of Crime Prevention through Environmental Design (CPTED). It provides an. overview of its ...

  8. Crime prevention, swarm intelligence and stigmergy: Understanding the

    To demonstrate the stigmergic and swarm intelligence characteristics of community crime prevention facilitated through social media, our article unfolds as follows. In the following literature review section, we review research on the mechanisms underpinning, and issues associated with, Neighbourhood Watch-style community crime prevention groups.

  9. A Review and Current Status of Crime Prevention through Environmental

    Terence Love has been involved for over fifteen years in environmental design and research relating to reducing criminal and antisocial behavior in physical and digital environments. He is a director of the Design out Crime Research Centre (www.designoutcrime.org).Recent research projects have involved indigenous justice and community night patrols, reducing youth crime and antisocial behavior ...

  10. Hot spots policing and crime reduction: an update of an ongoing

    Objectives This updated systematic review assesses the effects of focused police crime prevention interventions at crime hot spots. The review also examined whether focused police actions at specific locations result in crime displacement or diffusion of crime control benefits. Methods Systematic review protocols and conventions of the Campbell Collaboration were followed to identify eligible ...

  11. Technological innovation in policing and crime prevention: Practitioner

    Technological innovation in policing and crime prevention: Practitioner perspectives from London ... Even though the literature discusses this issue primarily with regard to achieving broad coverage of smart technologies across a city, the examples above give insights into the potential side effects on a micro-level (i.e. the effects of just ...

  12. The impact and policy relevance of street lighting for crime prevention

    University, Churchill Hall, . Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA . Email: [email protected]. Abstract. Research Summary: This article reports on an updated. systematic review and meta-analysis ...

  13. Crime prevention and reduction programs: How does knowing about

    Hale C. (1996) Fear of crime: A review of the literature. International Review of Victimology 4: 50-79. Crossref. Google Scholar. Hart, T. C. (2016). Using typologies of victimization worry to create strategies for reducing fear of crime. ... Rosenbaum D. P. (2006) Community crime prevention: A review and synthesis of the literature. Justice ...

  14. Systematic review of situational prevention methods for crime against

    Due to differences in law between countries, the authors of this review selected articles where prevention methods were being used to tackle illicit activities against species. Kurland et al incorporated both conservation and crime prevention methods, in a literature review. This systematic review could be supplemented with information from ...

  15. When crime prevention harms: a review of systematic reviews

    Methods. This paper reports on the first empirical review of harmful effects of crime prevention programs, drawing upon 15 Campbell Collaboration systematic reviews. Altogether, 574 experimental and quasi-experimental studies (published and unpublished) with 645 independent effect sizes were reviewed.

  16. Community crime prevention: A review and synthesis of the literature

    Dennis P. Rosenbaum. In the absence of effective formal means for controlling crime in the Western world, community crime prevention has emerged as a major alternative and supplement to the criminal justice system. This article attempts to review what is known currently about the nature, extent, and effectiveness of community-based efforts to ...

  17. PROTOCOL: Increased Police Patrol Presence Effects on Crime and

    Our emphasis is on the role of patrol presence for immediate crime prevention purposes. Based on the deterrence literature, crime should be reduced if police patrol can effectively increase the certainty that criminal activity will be observed and punished. One mainstay of policing since the 1930s has been random preventive patrol by automobile.

  18. PDF Preventing Crime: What Works, What Doesn't, What's Promising

    on a systematic review of more than 500 scientific evaluations of crime prevention practices. This Research in Brief summa-rizes the research methods and conclu-sions found in that report. In 1996, a Federal law required the U.S. Attorney General to provide Con-gress with an independent review of the Many crime prevention programs work. Others ...

  19. A Review and Current Status of Crime Prevention through Environmental

    Some reviews offer partial confirmation of the benefits of CPTED. For example, in a review of 122 evaluations of crime prevention projects, Poyner (1993) found that over half of those in the area of environmental design (24 out of 45) demonstrated firm evidence of crime reductions across all crime. Positive evaluations were focused on lighting ...

  20. What Works in Situational Crime Prevention? A Literature Review

    The review focused on studies published since 1975. The analysis revealed that no universal solutions exist and that initiatives need to be developed for each context. Crime prevention strategies need to be tailored to particular crimes in specific locations. Using a variety of nonspecific approaches is far less likely to be effective than are ...

  21. Technology and crime prevention: A systematic review of literature

    [email protected]. Abstract - In order to identify the application of. technology in crime prevention, a systematic analysis of. literature on technological tools applied to crime prevention ...

  22. PDF Incidence of Crimes and Effectiveness of Interventions in the ...

    Review of Related Literature In what follows, previous works related to crimes and interventions are reviewed. Included in this review are studies about the most prevalent crimes in ... Urban crime prevention strategies include both social crime prevention and situational crime prevention policies. Social crime prevention measures are those

  23. Prevention of Crime and Delinquency

    distinguishing between crime prevention and corrections, this comprehensive review of the literature offers a basic outline for the prevention of crime and delinquency. Abstract THE FIRST STEP IN DEVISING A PREVENTION STRATEGY IS TO DEFINE ACCEPTABLE LEVELS OF CRIME IN TERMS OF THE BALANCE OF SOCIAL GOALS WHICH MUST BE MAINTAINED TO PREVENT ...

  24. Cybercrime Intention Recognition: A Systematic Literature Review

    In this systematic literature review, we delve into the realm of intention recognition within the context of digital forensics and cybercrime. The rise of cybercrime has become a major concern for individuals, organizations, and governments worldwide. Digital forensics is a field that deals with the investigation and analysis of digital evidence in order to identify, preserve, and analyze ...

  25. Crime Prevention Tips and Literature

    Crime Prevention Tips and Literature. Crime prevention programs regarding block watches, security surveys, workplace violence reduction and Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design (CPTED) are all available through the Community Liaison Section. Contact your Community Liaison Officer to discuss and schedule the different services that can ...

  26. People with intellectual disability and their risk of exposure to

    A literature review was chosen as it offers a flexible way of working that suits this study's broad research questions. We were however inspired by the procedure of scoping review and the methods employed followed the guidance by Armstrong et al. (2011) : preparation, searches, assessment of relevance, mapping, and compilation.