The case studies have been carried out in this sequence:
The first case study is about a seven-year-old boy who has difficulties in understanding a meal situation. Charles is verbal, but he uses language in his own way, very little of which includes using words in a conversation. It is also difficult to understand how much he understands what he is told. He appeared to be unaware of the meaning of food during the meal and the rules for how to join in social activities at the table. He ate if someone gave him food on his plate, but he was unable to ask for food if it was not served. On the other hand, he often asked for food when he was not sitting at the dinner table.
The first analysis resulted in an assumption that he did not ask because he did not see where the food that was put on his plate came from. If he did not experience the food aspect when it was served in deep opaque dishes, and was not able to make the connection as to how others got food on their plates by serving themselves, it is obvious he could not act differently. However, when the food was served in deep transparent glass dishes, he showed exactly the same lack of ability. Seeing the food aspect was not the problem.
The second analysis resulted instead in an assumption that he was used to being served, and so he just waited until someone served him. A meal situation was constructed in which he was not served any food while the others at the table had their dinner. He did not make any attempt to ask for food or serve himself during the meal. A while after the meal was completed, and the other people left the table, he was served, otherwise he would not have had anything to eat. Although he was able to see the food and to serve himself, there was no change in his way of participating.
In the third analysis the focus was on the patterns of variation possible for Charles to experience during a meal situation. There were four to seven people sitting at the table. During the meal, they conversed and the sound of cutlery against china could be heard. Along with all the movements the people made while serving themselves, it seemed as if there were too many varied aspects. We had assumed that Charles had better previous knowledge than he in fact had. This was shown by how he acted when the ‘cues’ he needed to act were removed. We found that he turned off his attention during the meal. So, not seeing the food and not serving himself turned out not to be the critical aspects. We had observed this in other situations. It was, in fact, the number of varied aspects that made him unable to focus on the aspects which were critical to developing knowledge about how to act at meals. The balance was not maintained between the learner's previous knowledge and the complexity offered in the contrasted aspects as shown in Figure 2 .
The learning diagonal. The relationship between the learner's ability to learn by discerning the critical aspects offered in the learning situation in relation to their previous knowledge.
In the third analysis we examined which aspects could vary and which were kept invariant. To focus on the food dimension (represented by many different aspects, such as potatoes, vegetables, etc.) we chose to let Charles eat alone initially. In so doing, we maintained invariance in the situation at the table. The aspects of food did vary in order to make him understand how to choose and ask for different things. The food was represented in pictures, which were kept near Charles's plate. To get a desired food he had to ask by words or signs (pointing at a picture). He also had to discern the quantity aspect, since he had to serve himself from the deep dishes. The teacher asked him how much he wanted, if it was too much or too little, while he served himself. In this way, he could understand the meaning of the expressions ‘too much’ and ‘too little’ and connect them to how much he could eat; at the same time he was to learn how to get food on his plate. In this situation, he was able to understand which aspects were critical to focus on in a meal situation and which were not. Other people's conversation and movements should be unfocused, which was demonstrated to him by reducing them initially. Charles was placed in a situation where he only had to discern the critical aspects concerning food. This was necessary as he otherwise focused on aspects not critical for understanding a meal situation. When he had developed knowledge about the learning object, this ability was transferred to a situation including more people also eating sitting around the table. By analysing Charles's activities, we found that he was able to understand and act appropriately in a meal situation, with the pictures as support. In this way, variation was decreased in the beginning to make him focus on the critical aspects, and then increased in terms of letting more people join the meal situation without losing the focus on the critical aspects in it (see Table 2 ). The results show how Charles improved his ability to act more appropriately in a meal situation. He now asks for different items, and he focuses on eating when sitting at the table.
Case study 1: Charles | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 | Representation of food. Deep dishes containing food. People at the table. Verbally unfocused discussion between different people. | Adult help from people sitting next to him. The quantity of food he got on his plate, served by the adult. |
Situation 2 | Representation of food. People at the table. Verbally unfocused discussion between different people. | Deep transparent dishes containing food. |
Situation 3 | Representation of food shown by pictures. Quantity of food he could take from the dishes. | Adult sitting with him at the table. Pictures of food as a reminder of what to ask for. Verbally focused discussion between him and one person. |
Situation 4 | Representation of food shown by pictures. Quantity of food he could take from the dishes. People at the table. Verbally unfocused discussion between different people. | Pictures of food as a reminder of what to ask for. |
The second case study involved a 10-year-old girl with difficulties in understanding her work schedule. Liza can speak a few words. Her independent work activity was observed, and critical aspects were identified. Her schedule consisted of pieces of paper on which there were circles in different colours. They were fastened to a board with four paper clips and were attached to the board in the same order every day, as shown in Figure 3 .
Schedule board.
The teacher said this was because Liza became confused if the order was changed. Four plastic boxes were placed on a shelf beside her workstation. They had the same symbols as the pieces of paper on the schedule and were placed in exactly the same order every time.
To learn the concept of the schedule, the situation was studied from theoretical assumptions about discernment, simultaneity and variation. We found that Liza had not been offered the opportunity to discern the function of the schedule. It was removed and a second observation of how she handled her work without the schedule was carried out. It showed that she managed to perform the tasks anyway, by taking the boxes from the shelf in the order from left to right.
The aim of this activity was for her to develop an understanding of a schedule, which could be used in new situations. Liza had learned to find the work material in the different boxes, but not the intended object of learning, i.e., to use a schedule. To provide her with possibilities to do so, she had to focus on the critical aspects of the object of learning. One critical aspect was to follow the schedule, in one direction, no matter what colour the shapes were. The invariant aspect was to read the symbols from left to right every time, and the varied was where the symbols were placed on the board. The second critical aspect was the connection between the symbols on the schedule and the symbols on the boxes. To get Liza to focus on this connection, variation was built in by changing the order of the boxes, avoiding duplication of the order on the schedule. In this case, the invariant aspect was to match the symbols on the schedule with the symbols on the boxes and the varied was the order in which to pick the boxes from the shelf. Learning how to use a work schedule includes discerning the connection between the schedule and workboxes simultaneously. This became possible by building a necessary amount of variation in the educational situation. Liza has since developed the ability to use the schedule which includes accepting changes indicated by it (see Table 3 ).
Case study 2: Liza | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 | Content of work material in the boxes on the shelf. | Order of how to read the schedule: left–right. Order of symbol cards on the schedule. Order of boxes with work material on the shelf. Order of how to pick the boxes on the shelf: left–right. |
Situation 2 | Content of work material in the boxes on the shelf. | Order of boxes with work material on the shelf. Order of how to pick the boxes on the shelf: left–right. |
Situation 3 | Content of work material in the boxes on the shelf. Order of symbol cards on the schedule. Order of boxes with work material on the shelf. | Order of how to read the schedule; left–right. Order of how to pick the boxes on the shelf guided by the colours of the symbol cards. |
Max is an 11-year-old boy. He does not speak but he uses sound to communicate how he feels. His problem is to join in a meal situation, as was the problem for Charles, but Max's situation is the opposite of Charles's. Charles did not serve himself, but Max did not know which food he was allowed to eat and which he had to share with others. He was totally focused on food the whole day and would run to the kitchen as soon as he could to get more. When he did not succeed, he became angry with the staff and would bite and hit if they did not give him food.
The first analysis resulted in identifying two critical aspects: food as a thing to share with others and the distinction between food on his plate and that in the serving dishes. Max saw all food as his, and he could eat from any plate or dish he found. He did not seem to understand that others at the table should share the food served. He also ate any food he saw, as if he could not see food without eating it. He was very worried when he saw the symbol for food on the schedule, and he was focused on it instead of the present activity. If food was represented as the third symbol, he did not read the schedule from top to bottom; instead he focused on the symbol for food without noticing the order of the activities in the schedule. This was another critical aspect – the understanding of a schedule – also present in the case study of Liza.
The two learning objects (to read a schedule and to reduce the obsession with food) had to be dealt with in two different ways. To get him to focus on the order of the schedule instead of the activities in it, he was offered a schedule with decreased variation. By using a schedule which only showed activities in a sequence where the first always is a meal (constant) and the other activities are not connected to food, he did not have to wait for the meal situation in the same way. The varied aspect in the schedule was the order and content of the other activities, while the meal sign was kept invariant or constantly first in the sequence. Secondly, to provide him with a new experience of food, he was offered increased variation of the food dimension. This was a variation in which different shapes of food were introduced and in activities where he had to work with food without eating it. He could also watch TV programmes about cooking, an activity he liked very much. After a few weeks, a change in his behaviour at meals was observed. He did not run to the kitchen any more; he did not eat everything he saw and he really seemed to enjoy cooking for both himself and others. The way variation was used ( Table 4 ) to change his experience of the environment has been very effective in making him understand the critical aspects needed to participate in activities concerning food. In addition, he is much calmer in other situations, as his food obsession has decreased and he now allows himself to concentrate on other activities. Max has developed an ability to handle food without eating it in situations where it is not supposed to be eaten, and to act in an acceptable way at the table.
Case study 3: Max | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 | All activities on the schedule. | Food on a food trolley from the kitchen. Food served at the lunch table. |
Situation 2 | Food in different situations. Food shared by many people. Activities after the meal sign in the schedule. | Meal activity first on the schedule. Only eat food on his own plate. |
Situation 3 | Representation of food in reality and on television. Food in different situations. Food shared by many people. Activities after the meal sign in the schedule. | Meal activity first on the schedule. Always put food from the dishes on his own plate without eating. Only eat food on his own plate. Food in cooking situations, no eating allowed. |
The fourth case study is about a 12-year-old girl who finds it difficult to shop. Jessica does not use verbal language. By observing her, information was gathered about which dimensions of learning she seemed able to experience. The teacher assumed it would be too difficult to buy more than one thing at a time. She went to the shop with a teaching assistant to buy rolls for the afternoon break at school. She showed a great amount of anxiety in the shop, and it was hard for her to find the rolls as well as to understand how to queue up. Instead of understanding the meaning of shopping, she was totally focused on getting the rolls. She was not able to focus on more than one item at a time. A situation was designed where she had to focus on the whole shop simultaneously with the function of queuing to pay. To give her opportunities to focus on the varied range of items, variation was offered in the educational situation, represented by pictures of items in the shop other than the rolls. After looking at the pictures, the teaching assistant and Jessica went to the shop to find the items in the natural setting. The connection between shopping and just visiting the shop was shown in the distinction of paying or not paying.
The teaching assistant wrote an instructional story, describing the critical aspects of going shopping, based on a theoretical analysis of the girl's observed experience. She also made a portable schedule, with pictures of different articles in the shop, and pictures of the different steps required for queuing to pay for the articles. After three shopping trips, Jessica showed no anxiety at all, rather excitement. In the last videotape from her shopping, she even turned to the teaching assistant with a smile, pointing at a box of soft cheese (which had not been introduced as an article for her) with a face expression saying: ‘Could we buy this too?’. Shopping has become more to her than just picking up rolls for the afternoon break. She continued her shopping skills with her family. Instead of having experienced shopping as a chaotic activity for all involved, she now helps her mum choose articles instead of having outbursts because she does not have a clear and understandable picture of the activity. Shopping includes doing different things every time, which in turn blocks every attempt to create an ‘everyday theory’. This frustrated her when she went to the shop because she did not experience the invariant aspects of the concept.
In this case she now is able to make connections at a more developed level, which helps her to deal with the variation she has to consider simultaneously with queuing in the shop. Jessica was given the opportunity to become aware of the connection between choosing items and paying for them as a sort of exchange. The other difficulty, queuing up to pay, showed she had not realized how to move when the queue moved forward. Finally, she was not able to see the connection between when to place her own items on the conveyor belt in relation to the other customer's items. For Jessica to gain these insights, i.e., to focus on the needed critical aspects, the teaching assistant initially enabled her to focus on the ‘next customer’ stick. Jessica was shown to put her items on the conveyor belt after this divider. The problem was that she did not wait to put the ‘next customer’ stick on the conveyor belt until the previous customer had placed all of her items on the conveyor belt, which resulted in a mix-up of items. In the next learning session, the teacher instead had her focus on the belt having to be empty before Jessica could put her items on it. This was presented to Jessica by photos of an empty conveyor belt the next time they visited the shop. In this study, the shop was kept invariant, while the things that varied were the items to buy. Further development includes a variation of shops, as described in situations 4 and 5 in Table 5 . Jessica has improved her ability to go shopping in how to choose items, how to queue before paying and how to act when she is supposed to pay for her items.
Case study 4: Jessica | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 | Queue to pay. Put items on conveyer belt. | Item. Shop. Help by teaching assistants. |
Situation 2 | Items. The order of items to buy shown in the portable schedule. | Shop. Portable schedule. Move forward in the queue as the person in front moves. Put items on conveyer belt after next customer stick. |
Situation 3 | Items. The order of items to buy shown in the portable schedule. | Shop. Portable schedule. Move forward in the queue as the person in front moves. Put items on conveyer belt when it is empty. |
Planned situation 4 | Shop. | Items. Portable schedule. Move forward in the queue as the person in front moves. Put items on conveyer belt when it is empty. |
Planned situation 5 | Shop. Items. The order of items to buy shown in the portable schedule. | Portable schedule. Move forward in the queue as the person in front moves. Put items on conveyer belt when it is empty. |
Mary is 13 years-old. She is verbal and on a higher level than the four previous presented participants. Because of this, her disabilities are not as explicit. However, the learning object is in an area not often talked about in public. Mary's difficulty in understanding her menstruation cycle is grounded in her strong connection between blood and death. She thought she was very badly hurt when she saw blood in her bed one morning. In the interviews with Mary, critical aspects concerning what blood is and when blood is dangerous, along with the biological differences between men and women, had to be discussed. First, the teacher showed Mary a picture of a woman (similar to a paper doll) where the blood circulation was depicted ( Table 6 ). Then, she showed the same doll with the menstruation cycle depicted. By that she was able to discern the difference between losing blood from the two systems, one as dangerous and the other as normal. After that, she gave Mary the opportunity to see how she could predict when she would have her period. This was done from two perspectives: according to the cycles of the months and according to the life cycle. From the second perspective she was also informed that young girls and women over 50 usually do not menstruate, which is related to becoming pregnant. From this perspective she also realized that the reason men were not able to have babies was because they do not menstruate. Finally, she was informed how to take care of her feminine hygiene. The critical aspects found in this learning object were the differences between toilet facilities. Those for women usually have a waste box for sanitary towels, which is not the case in toilets for both women and men. Because of that, she had to be instructed in two different ways to take care of this. The teacher considered the variation needed to create new learning about the two different learning objects, and the outcome showed that Mary was able to take care of her situation independently and her anxiety disappeared. She had developed an understanding of how to handle her period and the related hygiene.
Case study 5: Mary | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 Learning object A | Representation of blood in the circulatory system. Different kinds of injuries causing bleeding. | |
Situation 2 Learning object A | Representation of blood (in the circulatory system and in the menstruation cycle). Weeks with and weeks without menstruation. | Women have both systems because they can be pregnant. Men, children and older women do not have a menstruation cycle. |
Situation 3 Learning object B | Toilets for women to use to take care of their intimate hygiene. | How sanitary towels are disposed of in a toilet with special waste boxes. |
Situation 4 Learning object B | Mixed toilets to use to take care of intimate hygiene. | How sanitary towels are disposed of in a toilet without special waste boxes. |
Paul is in puberty and is rather high functioning. He is 15 years-old and his problem is connected to physical movements. He still moves as though he was a little boy, even though he is a tall young man. His movements include jumping, ritualistic walking patterns and other repetitive movements. Since he is so tall, he risks hurting himself by hitting the ceiling when he jumps indoors. People who do not know him find this behaviour very odd and it results in more repudiation than needed. Paul is able to understand written instructions and social stories have been used to help him shop independently. However, his physical movements appear as if he has no control over them. But since he seems to enjoy these activities, the aim is not to forbid them entirely; it is instead to give him the chance to learn other, more age-appropriate ways of moving, and the situations when his own ways of moving are allowed. The critical aspects identified are Paul's very limited ways of moving his body and his limited understanding of what are typical movements for his age and in different settings. To get him to focus on these aspects, the teacher offered him an increased variation concerning physical activities and a decreased variation concerning where the different physical activities were allowed to take place. These two aspects needed to be focused on simultaneously. First of all, the teacher introduced different ways of walking instead of letting him walk back and forth in the same spot as usual. Then she showed him examples of how other boys his age move, compared to how younger boys move. He was told that every person has his own private area, where he can do things that are not appropriate in other places. If he was not supposed to jump inside at school and in public where other people could see him, he was still allowed to jump in his room, as long as he did not hurt himself ( Table 7 ). Although this has been a problem for Paul for four years, he now moves more like other teenagers. His parents have reported that they see him now and again coming from his room, warm and red in his face from jumping. But no comments are made and he feels secure in his privacy. Obviously, this activity is something that gives him pleasure and forbidding it would not be fair. If variation had not been used in this learning situation, the teacher would have probably forbidden all jumping to reduce such behaviour. In this case, Paul has been given the opportunity to learn the connection between his acts and those of other people. By doing so, he developed an understanding of the differences between doing things when people are present and when they are not, a difference he was not aware of initially.
Case study 6: Paul | Varied aspects | Constant aspects |
---|---|---|
Situation 1 | Physical activities in different places. | The physical activities performed. |
Situation 2 | Different physical activities. | Each activity connected to a place where it is allowed. |
Situation 3 | Boys in different ages and their physical activities. | The physical activity. |
Teaching children and young people with autism has often been characterized by training rather than focusing on learning as a development in experiencing the environment. The training is often built on routines, carried out over and over again, and reinforcers are used, aiming to correct the person's behaviour. If learning is seen as a new way to experience, and by that to act in the environment, such school situations cannot be seen as learning activities. The results in this study show how variation, in a planned and structured way, makes the respondents in the six case studies more aware of what is expected of them in a situation. By making them discern connections between isolated events, their ability to act appropriately seems to develop. The disability has been described in terms of weak central coherence, lack of executive functions or theory of mind, etc. Findings show agreement in the literature that special needs education is the best way to help these individuals. Despite this, in most of the studies it is unclear how learning and special education are defined in a theoretical perspective. Because of this, educational practices seem to lack theory, at least in terms of theories on learning. In this study, we have examined how to use a theory to develop more insight into knowing what it takes to learn. Educational situations were designed in a way that made the pupil focus on the critical aspects of learning, and to make connections between different aspects, thus creating a better way of understanding the object of learning. The studies have demonstrated the advantage of making the student focus on the connections between different aspects rather than single aspects, one at a time. To make this happen, the balance between variation/invariance has to be considered. Instead of creating methods for teaching children with autism, the focus has been on the relation between how an individual experiences the world and which critical aspects have to vary to make it possible for the individual to discern new aspects. The variation presented in the case studies is very distinct in identifying the critical aspects in a learning situation. Even if the same method is used, a change in those aspects which vary and those which do not vary makes the difference. Variation theory is powerful both in the analysis and the planning of learning activities. The results of the case studies also show possible connections among different assumptions of the features of autism. This has to be further developed, but it is worth considering the fact that these individuals show an extremely atomistic way of organizing experiences in the environment, which results in a weakness in central coherence and executive functions. As a result, it is hard for them to develop a theory of mind or inter-subjectivity. This might be the reason that special education seems to be the most powerful way to assist children with autism in their development. If the main characteristic of autism is a way to experience the world as extremely atomistic, the other features are a result of this extremely atomistic way of organizing experiences. By that, the behaviour is the expression of the extremely atomistic way of organizing experiences. Even if finding the cause or the main feature of autism was not the aim of this study, the results raise new, interesting questions in such a research area.
This study was made possible through financial support from the Swedish Research Council. The children, teachers and families in this study have contributed in an important way for which I am also very grateful.
American Psychiatric Association . 1994 . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders , 4th ed. (DSM-IV) . [city: Washington ] , [state: DC ] : American Psychiatric Association .
Austin , J. , and J. Carr , 2000 . Handbook of applied behavior analysis . [city: Reno ] : Context Press .
Baron-Cohen , S. (1995). . Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind . , [city: Cambridge ] , [state: MA ] : : MIT Press. .
Bransford , J. and Schwartz , D. (1999). . Rethinking transfer: A simple proposal with multiple implications . Review of Research in Education, a Publication of the American Educational Research Association , 24: : 61.–.
Brown , A.L. (1992). . Design experiments: Theoretical and methodological challenges in creating complex interventions in classroom settings . The Journal of the Learning Sciences , 2((2)): : 141.–.
Carlgren , I. , and F. Marton . 2002 . Lärare av imorgon [Teachers of tomorrow] . Stockholm : [city: Lärarförbundet ] .
Cohen , S. (1998). . Targeting autism. What we know, and can do to help young children with autism and related disorders . , [city: California ] : : California University Press. .
Duvner , T. 1994 . Barnneuropsykiatri. MBD/DAMP, autistiska störningar, dyslexi [Child neuropsychiatry, MBD/DAMP, autistic disorders, dyslexia] . Stockholm : [city: Liber Utbildning ] .
Frith , U. (1989). . Autism. Explaining the enigma . , [city: Oxford ] : : Basil Blackwell. .
Gibson , J. (1986). . The ecological approach to visual perception . , [city: Hillsdale ] , [state: NJ ] : : LEA. .
Gillberg , C. 1995 . Autism och autismliknande tillstånd hos barn, ungdomar och vuxna [Autism and autism related states by children, teenagers and adults] . [city: Stockholm ] : Natur och Kultur .
Grandin , T. (1986). . Emergence labelled autistic . , [city: California ] : : Arena Press. .
Gray , C. 1995 . Teaching children with autism to ‘read’ social situations . In Teaching children with autism , A. Quill , 219 – 241 . [city: New York ] : Delmar Publisher .
Happé , F. (1994). . Autism. An introduction to psychological theory . , [city: Cambridge ] , [state: MA ] : : Harvard University Press. .
Happé , F. , J. Briskman and U. Frith . 2001 . Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak ‘central coherence’ in parents and siblings of children with autism, I. Experimental tests . Journal of Childhood Psychology and Psychiatry 42 3 299 307
Holmqvist , M. 1995a . Autism. Uppfostran, undervisning och förståelse för personer med extremt atomistisk omvärldsuppfattning [Autism. Bringing up, educating and understanding individuals with extremely atomistic environmental experiences] . Göteborg : [city: Kompendiet ] .
Holmqvist , M. . 1995b . Autism. Specialpedagogisk träning för utökad social kompetens och omvärldsförståelse. [Autism. Special educational training to enhance social competence and environmental understanding] Nordisk Tidskrift för Specialpedagogik 4 174 81
Holmqvist , M. . 1996 . Understanding, bringing up and educating people with autism . LINK Autisme-Europa 18 : 12 – 14
Holmqvist , M. 2004a . Improving the educational outcomes of disabled children by using a theory of variation at school . Paper presented at Oxford Round Table, March 21–26, in Oxford .
Holmqvist , M. 2004b . En främmande värld. Om lärande och autism [A strange world. Learning and austim] [city: Lund ] : Studentlitteratur .
Holmqvist , M. , L. Gustavsson , and A. Wernberg . 2008 . Variation theory – An organizing principle to guide design research in education In Handbook of design research methods in education , M. Holmqvist , L. Gustavsson , and A. Wernberg , 111 – 30 . [city: New York ] Routledge .
Howlin , P. (1998). . Children with autism and Asperger syndrome. A guide for practitioners and carers . , [city: Chichester ] : : Wiley. .
Howlin , P. , S. Baron-Cohen , and J. Hadwn . 1999 . Lära barn med autism att läsa andras tankar och känslor [Teaching children with autism to mind-read] [city: Stockholm ] : Cura .
Jordan , R. and Powell , S. (1995). . Understanding and teaching children with autism . , [city: Chichester ] : : Wiley. .
Kanner , L. (1943). . Autistic disturbances of affective contact . Nervous Child , 2: : 217.–.
Kelly , A. , and R. Lesh , 2000 . Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education . [city: London ] : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates .
Lovaas , I. and Buch , G. (1997). . “Intensive behavioural intervention with young children with autism ”. In Prevention and treatment of severe behaviour problems: Models and methods in developmental disabilities . , Edited by: Singh , N.N. [city: Pacific Grove ] , [state: CA ] : : Books/Cole. .
Lord , C. and P. McGee 2001 . Educating children with autism . [city: Washington ] : National Academic Press, National Research Council .
Marton , F. (2000). . The practice of learning . Nordisk Pedagogik , 4: : 230.–.
Marton , F. and Booth , S. (1997). . Learning and awareness . , [city: Mahwah ] , [state: NJ ] : : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. .
Marton , F. , and P.M. Fai . 1999 . Two faces of variation . Paper presented at the 8th European Conference for Learning and Instruction August 24–28at Göteborg University , Sweden. http://www.ped.gu.se/biorn/phgraph/civil/graphica/newph.html .
Marton , F. , and K. Trigwell . 2000 . Variatio Est Mater Studiorum . Higher Education Research and Development 19 3 381 – 95 .
Marton , F. and Tsui , A. (2004). . Classroom discourse and the space of learning . , [city: London ] : : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. .
Marton , F. and Tsui , A. (2004). . Classroom discourse and the space of learning . , [city: Mahwah ] , [state: NJ ] : : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. .
McClannahan , L. and Krantz , P. (1999). . Activity schedules for children with autism . , [city: Bethesda ] , [state: MD ] : : Woodbinehouse. .
McNiff , J. and Whitehead , J. (2005). . Action research for teachers. A practical guide . , [city: London ] : : David Fulton Publishers Ltd. .
Quill , K. (1995). . Teaching children with autism. Strategies to enhance communication and socialisation . , [city: New York ] : : Delmar Publishers Inc. .
Reynhout , G. and Carter , M. (2006). . Social stories for children with disabilities . Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 36: : 445.–.
Rubin , E. 1915 . Synoplevende figurer, studier i psykologisk analyse, forste del [Visual based figures, studies in psychological analysis, the first part] . [city: Köpenhamn ] : Gyldendalsk .
Runesson , U. 1999 . Variationens pedagogik. Skilda sätt att behandla ett matematiskt innehåll [The pedagogy of variation] . [city: Göteborg ] : Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis .
Russell , J. 1997 . Autism as an executive disorder . [city: Oxford ] : Oxford University Press .
Schopler , E. and Mesibov , G. (1995). . Learning and cognition in autism . , [city: New York ] : : Plenum Press. .
Schwartz , D. , and J. Bransford . 1998 . A time for telling . Cognition and Instruction 16 4 475 – 522 .
Tomal , D. (2003). . Action research for educators . , [city: Oxford ] : : Rowman and Littlefield Education. .
Tomasello , M. (1999). . Human cognition . , [city: London ] : : Harvard University Press. .
Vygotskij , L. (1997). . Educational psychology . , [city: Boca Raton ] : : St. Luge cop. .
Wertheimer , M. (1959). . Gestalt psychology: Thought and thinking . , [city: New York ] : : Harper. .
Wing , L. (1996). . The autistic spectrum: A guide for parents and professionals . , [city: London ] : : Constable. .
Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser .
Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.
Download Free PDF
Journal for ReAttach Therapy and Developmental Diversities, 2020
Introduction: Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental condition that occurs within the first 3 years of life, which is characterised by poor social skills, communication problems and stereotyped patterns of behaviour. Autism is a lifelong disorder that has a substantial effect on the individual, their family, and society. The purpose of this paper is to provide an overview about the psychosocial aspects of autism spectrum disorders. Methods: An analysis of relevant literature, sources from the internet and published literature, personal experience and observations of the author. Findings: Despite widespread research and greater public awareness, ASD has an unclear etiology and no known cure, making it difficult to acquire an accurate and timely diagnosis. Psychologic functions such as attention , executive function, academic functioning, memory, emotions, and sensory processing are described. There is a need for continuous psycho-social support for people with ASD and their relatives during the diagnostics and early intervention period, as well as resources that better represent the diversity of experiences and symptoms associated with ASD across the lifespan. Conclusion: It is clear that more special education services are needed, together with timely and ongoing psy-chosocial support to parents of children with ASD.
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental condition that is marked by both social and cognitive impediments (Lipinski et al., 2019). Currently, about 1% of the population in North America will meet the diagnostic criteria for ASD (Schmidt et al., 2015). With a few rare exceptions, the majority of individuals with this diagnosis require ongoing support and care (Schmidt et al., 2015). In addition to the challenges associated with the disorder, there is a high rate of comorbidity amongst the ASD community, indicating the need for skilled intervention (Lipinski et al., 2019). However, there are few psychotherapists who are trained to work with this population. In light of these growing concerns, the following paper will explore how clinicians can offer effective, evidence-based interventions for client’s with ASD. In particular, the exercise will address diagnostic criteria, development and course, etiology, epidemiology, diagnostic issues, prognosis, assessment, and theoretical treatments.
This article aims to observe all the manifestations of the behavior of a child with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), which shows deficits mainly in the communication sector. Also, the child shows repetitive and stereotypical behaviors throughout the lesson (Stasinos, 2016). Initially the paper describes the methodology followed. It then describes the child's cognitive profile and the deficits he presents. He then analyzes the intervention that was applied in order to improve the difficulties he faces and to further strengthen the skills he has already acquired. Finally, the paper presents the main conclusions as they emerged from the intervention.
Contents: Foreword. Preface. Part I: The Study of Autism: Perspectives of a Field in Transition. J.D. Bregman, Definitions and Characteristics of the Spectrum. L.Y. Tsai, Recent Neurobiological Research in Autism. V.M. Durand, Past, Present, and Emerging Directions in Education. A. Hecimovic, S. Gregory, The Evolving Role, Impact, and Needs of Families. Part II: The Early Years. E.M. Gabovitch, N.D. Wiseman, Early Identification of Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Scott, W.L. Baldwin, The Challenge of Early Intensive Intervention. J.S. Bloch, J. Weinstein, M. Seitz, School and Parent Partnerships in the Preschool Years. Part III: Educational Programming, Interventions, and Medical Treatment for Persons With Autism. J.S. Handleman, L.M. Delmolino, Assessment of Children With Autism. D. Zager, N. Shamow, Teaching Students With Autism Spectrum Disorders. A.M. Wetherby, B.M. Prizant, Enhancing Language and Communication Development in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Assessment and Intervention ...
Malaysian Journal of Public Health Medicine, 2018
Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have significant challenges in their daily life including social communication and interaction, emotional awareness and management, as well as behavioural issues. Many interventions are conducted based on theoretical backgrounds and past literature. There is lack of research study that interview and explore the real psychological needs of children with ASD in Malaysia. It is a significant component as their needs may vary depending on the cultural background, lifestyle, and social norms. Besides, there is a need to develop standardised intervention module to enhance intervention fidelity and replication of future study. The current study aims to scrutinise the psychological needs of children with ASD in Malaysia, develop a standardised group intervention module based on the identified needs, and then examine the feasibility of the developed module. Nine children with moderate to high functioning of ASD (7 to 12 years old) and their parent...
European Journal of Special Education Research, 2019
The purpose of this study is to present a case report of an autism incident, specifically of a toddler's, and of an educational intervention program that was built in order to promote his inclusion in the group and to compensate for adjustment difficulties. The paper presents a description of an educational experience through presentation of an autism incident and the analysis of the educational intervention and strategies followed. In particular, the educational intervention was implemented in a Centre for the design and implementation of personalized and group education programs, which creates a variety of social stimuli of inclusion philosophy towards the child and the design and implementation of each program is based on working with the family. Overall, various forms of organizing the education of the child with autism were intentionally used, due to the variety of special educational needs which appear in this category of children and flexible access to a wide range of opportunities required. Finally, it should be noted that after the daily review of child performance data and decision-making on the part of the educator, on the teaching strategy that was being promoted each time, this child had obvious progress in the way of approaching and communicating within the team, as well as in issues of self-service, as well as fine and tangible mobility. Apparently, his communication and social skills have been improved to a good extent and his inclusion has proved to be a realistic goal in the context of this systematic pedagogical intervention.
Zenodo (CERN European Organization for Nuclear Research), 2021
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.
Neuropsychology Review, 2008
Boletín médico del Hospital Infantil de México
CoDAS, 2013
Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2019
IntechOpen eBooks, 2023
Khatija Tul Kubra, 2024
International Journal of Social Science Research, 2020
"An Attempt to Get Acquainted with Autism Spectrum Disorders". EC Psychology and Psychiatry 8.12(2019): 01-04, 2019
Journal of Early Intervention, 2011
Social Science Research Network, 2021
… and Training in …, 2007
Autism Spectrum Disorder - Recent Advances, 2015
National Center For Special Education Research, 2007
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1999
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2009
A mother's insight into using social stories with a child who has autism..
Posted October 17, 2024 | Reviewed by Jessica Schrader
Social stories have been a constant companion in my parenting journey (with my two neurodivergent boys).
I, as well as our speech and occupational therapists, crafted social stories whenever we wanted to expand my boys’ understanding about which behaviors were safe and nourishing (which we wanted them to choose) and which ones were less safe and associated with greater discomfort and distress (which we wanted them to stop engaging in, or at least decrease). Or, we crafted social stories to break down instructions about how to perform a task, for instance how to play a “Guess Who” game with a friend, or how to play with trucks at the sand pit on a play date.
The social stories we crafted always involved explicit descriptions of various situations and addressed a myriad of "wh" questions; you know the ones: who, what, where, when, why, and how. They also included photos that my boys loved looking at. I loved seeing their reactions, "It's my picture!” “It's a book about me!" Personally, we found social stories incredibly effective, hence I continued using them over the years.
For instance, when we welcomed a new puppy into our home, a social story crafted by our speech therapist helped to clarify what behaviors were safe and nurturing toward the puppy. It outlined ideas for appropriate play and gentle handling, while also explaining that when a puppy barks, it is likely communicating that he doesn’t like what we are doing and would like us to be gentler. At other times, he may bark to get our attention , say hello, or be guarding our space.
Another social story crafted by our occupational therapist taught my boys to not run away from their granddad when they were out and about with him, at a trying time when they were testing their independence, as he cannot chase them. I also recall our occupational therapist crafting an incredibly effective social story for teaching my little one to not demand that we say a phrase repeatedly in a very precise way, or else… (he would feel distressed).
One of my favorite social stories was one I crafted about sharing toys, at a time my youngest was approaching an age where he felt curious about his big brother’s things and enjoyed reaching for them. Desperate to ease the occasional outbursts of temper that flared from the resultant shifts in boundaries , the social story outlined specific strategies for "sharing management ," and was quite effective for our situation. While it wasn’t a miraculous cure for easing all of our frustrations around sharing, it was a resource that we returned to, over and again, that afforded us greater control and clarity over managing this situation.
I continue to make use of various social stories to help my own children and my little clients find ways to respond to their feelings, for instance expanding their awareness about appropriate and inappropriate responses for managing frustration.
Despite finding social stories personally invaluable, evaluating their effectiveness can be challenging. Social stories, by definition, are unique and tailored to an individual child, their family, and their environment, making comparisons difficult. In addition, they are delivered by unique individuals with various flavors of engagement and differences in the way they administer social stories. These include differences in joint attention and expression of encouragement and positive reinforcement. Social stories can also be delivered in many ways, such as via iPad, paper copy, audio, or video, and can vary in the quality of formatting, illustrations, and photos, and in how much these resonate with the child’s inner experience and understanding of a situation.
Many studies report positive outcomes associated with the use of social stories, including when social stories are administered by parents (Camilleri et al., 2022). These are usually measured via changes in the child's behavior (that is targeted in the social story). Camilleri et al. (2022) indicated that there has been an overall improvement in the quality of research regarding social stories within the last decade or so, which has been accompanied by an overall increase in reports of the effectiveness of social stories for autistic children (Karal & Wolfe, 2018; Qi et al., 2018; Aldabas, 2019). However, Camilleri et al. (2022) generally concluded that "further research is needed," in response to generally mixed findings regarding the strength of the effectiveness of social stories. It would be helpful to better understand what variables influence the effectiveness of social stories, in what settings, and for what individuals.
If you're looking to write your own social story, consider following the criteria of Carol Gray, an expert in this area, whose published work on this topic originated in the 1990s. Her latest comprehensive criteria for crafting social stories are available via her website (see the references section).
In general, Gray recommends that social stories:
Personally, I like to take matters into my own hands and test whether, for the purposes I am crafting a social story, it is having an impact. You, too, can be your own researcher and gather data before you introduce a social story, in between its various presentations, as well as at follow-up. Given the uniqueness of you, your child, and the social story, it may be the most effective way to see whether it will have an impact. Happy writing.
Comparison of Social StoriesTM 10.0 – 10.2 criteria . carolgraysocialstories.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/09/Social-Stories-10.0-10.2-Comparison-Chart.pdf (Accessed: 17 October 2024).
Camilleri, L. J., Maras, K., & Brosnan, M. (2022). Autism Spectrum Disorder and Social Story Research: a Scoping Study of Published, Peer-Reviewed Literature Reviews. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 9 (1), 21–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-020-00235-6
Bozena Zawisz is a psychologist and award-winning author specializing in psycho-educational resources for young Autistic persons and mindful journaling interventions for women.
It’s increasingly common for someone to be diagnosed with a condition such as ADHD or autism as an adult. A diagnosis often brings relief, but it can also come with as many questions as answers.
A not-for-profit organization, IEEE is the world's largest technical professional organization dedicated to advancing technology for the benefit of humanity. © Copyright 2024 IEEE - All rights reserved. Use of this web site signifies your agreement to the terms and conditions.
Objective To compare the clinical presentations, management and outcomes of avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) across paediatric and child and adolescent (C&A) psychiatric settings.
Study design Prospective surveillance study.
Methods Data were collected during a 13-month prospective surveillance study of children and adolescents with ARFID in the UK and Republic of Ireland. Paediatricians reported cases via the British Paediatric Surveillance Unit and psychiatrists through the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Surveillance System. A follow-up questionnaire was sent at 12 months after a case of ARFID was reported.
Results 319 cases were included, 189 from paediatricians and 130 from C&A psychiatrists. Patients presenting to paediatricians were younger (9.8 years vs 13.7 years), more often male (62.4% vs 43.1%), and had more chronic symptoms (80.4% vs 67.0%), selective eating (63.7% vs 46.6%) and comorbid autism (67.6% vs 50.0%) than to psychiatrists. Psychiatrists saw patients with more fear of aversive consequences from eating (13.1% vs 3.2%), weight loss (76.7% vs 65.0%) and comorbid anxiety (78.2% vs 47.4%). Patients presenting to paediatricians more often received medical monitoring (74.6% vs 53.1%), dietetic advice (83.1% vs 70.0%) and nutritional supplements (49.2% vs 30.0%). At follow-up, both cohorts improved in nutritional status. However, the psychiatric cohort improved more regarding disordered eating behaviours.
Conclusions The presentation and management of ARFID differs across clinical settings. Findings suggest the need to develop clinical pathways for ARFID assessment and management across paediatrics and mental health. Our findings highlight the potential benefits of psychiatric input for some patients with ARFID.
Data availability statement.
Data are available upon reasonable request. Deidentified participant data that underlie the results reported in this article can be shared upon specific requests by researchers who provide a methodologically sound proposal. The proposal will be considered by the investigators of this article. Requests should be directed by email to the corresponding author.
This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ .
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2024-327032
Request permissions.
If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.
Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) is an eating disturbance which leads to: weight loss; nutritional deficiency; dependence on enteral feeding/nutritional supplements; or psychosocial impairment.
ARFID is an umbrella term covering a number of clinical presentations. Existing descriptions of ARFID presentations have drawn from specific clinical settings, limiting their generalisability.
Clinical guidance on the management of ARFID is limited by a lack of research evidence.
This is the first study to examine the presentation and management of ARFID across different clinical settings.
It highlights the potential benefits of collaborative working and availability of psychological interventions in improving outcomes for children and young people with ARFID.
This will guide the development of evidence-based pathways for ARFID assessment and management to ensure they are tailored to the specific needs of each patient.
Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) is a persistent eating disturbance resulting in inability to meet nutritional or energy needs. 1 Unlike anorexia nervosa, food restriction is not driven by concerns about weight or body image; instead, three primary rationales behind food restriction in ARFID have been proposed, including: sensitivity to sensory aspects of food; lack of interest in eating; and fear of aversive consequences (eg, choking, vomiting). 1 2 These presentations may occur independently or coexist, and differ by age and sex, among other factors. 3 Prevalence estimates range from 0.3% and 15.5% in non-clinical settings up to 64% in specialist eating disorder clinics. 4
ARFID frequently presents together with various medical and psychiatric comorbidities, including anxiety, 5 autism spectrum disorder (ASD) 6 or obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). 7 Medical sequelae of ARFID can be severe, with multiple potential complications due to low weight and malnutrition. 8
As a relatively new diagnosis, research on ARFID remains limited. Current management strategies are formulated using clinical experience rather than research evidence due to lack of randomised controlled trials. 7 9 Patients with ARFID require assessment and treatment using a multimodal multidisciplinary approach. 10
Existing literature characterising ARFID is highly heterogeneous depending on the clinical setting. Studies have been conducted in paediatric settings 11–15 but, to our knowledge, no previous research has compared clinical presentations and management of ARFID between paediatric and psychiatric settings. We aimed to characterise ARFID presentations in children and young people (CYP) and the care received in each setting. We hypothesised differences in demographic and clinical characteristics, medical symptoms, psychiatric comorbidities, management approaches and outcomes between cases seen in paediatric and psychiatric services.
Data were from a prospective surveillance study of CYP with ARFID in the UK and the Republic of Ireland (ROI) presenting to secondary care. The study was undertaken over 13 months, 1 March 2021 to 31 March 2022, with 1-year follow-up. Cases were identified through the British Paediatric Surveillance Unit (BPSU) and the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Surveillance System (CAPSS), which employ active surveillance methodology to facilitate research into rare paediatric conditions. 16 Surveillance report cards were emailed monthly to 4298 consultant paediatricians and 695 child and adolescent psychiatrists, requesting notification of new cases meeting ARFID reporting criteria. A 1-year follow-up questionnaire was then sent to clinicians reporting confirmed cases.
Participants were CYP newly diagnosed with ARFID attending secondary care. A surveillance case definition ( online supplemental table 1 ) based on modified Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition criteria 1 was approved by BPSU and CAPSS committees. Paediatricians reported cases from 5 to 16 years as indicated in BPSU guidelines. 17 Psychiatrists reported cases from 5 to 18 years. Clinicians reporting a suspected case received a questionnaire requesting more information. A detailed analytical case definition was used by the study team to confirm cases based on the questionnaire data ( online supplemental table 2 ).
Data were collected using initial and follow-up questionnaires developed for this study by the authors. All data were depersonalised using Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap), 18 a secure, web-based software platform, and stored in a protected environment at Imperial College London. Questionnaires included items on demographic, presentation, clinical features and management of each case using yes/no questions supplemented with free text or multiple-response options. Additionally, clinicians were asked to report their overall clinical impression regarding the patient’s eating behaviours at follow-up. We compared responses between paediatricians and psychiatrists.
Body mass index (BMI) was calculated and z -scores for height, weight and BMI were determined using UK 1990 growth reference data. 19 20 BMI z-score cut-offs were: obesity >+2 SD; overweight >+1 to ≤+2 SD; normal weight ≤+1 SD to >−2 SD; underweight ≤−2 to >−3 SD; and severe underweight <−3 SD. 21
We classified cases using four mutually exclusive ARFID subtypes defined elsewhere (Combined, Sensory, Lack of interest, Fear). 3
Data were coded and analysed with IBM Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) V.29.0. For continuous outcomes, Mann-Whitney U test and t-tests were used. Categorical outcomes were evaluated with χ 2 and McNemar’s tests. Statistical significance was set as two-sided p<0.05. As the age range for paediatric cases and psychiatric cases differed, a sensitivity analysis excluding individuals ≥16 years was performed. 18
319 cases of ARFID fulfilled analytical case definition and were included in this study. Of those, 189 cases were reported by paediatricians and 130 by psychiatrists. 265 cases were reported from England, 7 from Wales, 13 from Scotland, 7 from Northern Ireland, 6 from ROI and one each from Jersey and the Isle of Man. Four confirmed cases were reported to both BPSU and CAPSS (duplicates, figure 1 ). Follow-up data were available for 197 (61.8%) participants (113 from paediatricians and 84 from psychiatrists).
Flow diagram of case ascertainment. This figure shows the flow of individuals from notification to case validation: after reporting a case to BPSU or CAPSS, clinicians were contacted to complete a questionnaire. Reporting errors (such as prevalent cases or confirmed diagnosis of anorexia nervosa) were excluded prior to baseline questionnaire completion after contacting the clinician. Unable to follow-up cases were those excluded due to clinicians stating that they did not wish to be included in the study (due to retirement, shortage of reporting capacity and so on). Cases were excluded if no response was obtained after multiple attempts to contact the notifying clinician or their team (no baseline data received). Completed questionnaires by reporting clinicians were examined to confirm cases were eligible for inclusion. Duplicates were identified and excluded. Additional cases from other sources that met inclusion criteria were added. A 1-year follow-up questionnaire was sent to clinicians reporting confirmed cases. BPSU, British Paediatric Surveillance Unit; CAPSS, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Surveillance System.
Demographics
Table 1 summarises demographic characteristics. 174 (54.5%) male and 145 (45.5%) female cases were reported. Paediatricians were significantly more likely to see males than psychiatrists (χ 2 (1, n=319)=11.64, p=0.001, ϕ=0.19). The median age of participants was 11.9 years (IQR 7.6, 14.4). Individuals in the psychiatric cohort had a higher median age (13.7 years; IQR 10.5, 15.1) compared with the paediatric cohort (9.8 years; IQR 6.5, 13.2; U=17 103, z=5.95, p<0.001, r=0.33). Paediatricians more often managed children aged 5–9 years, whereas psychiatrists more so treated CYP aged 10–14 and 15–18 (χ 2 (2, n=319)=31.97, p<0.001, ϕ=0.32). Psychiatrists were more likely to see white British CYP, while paediatricians saw more from African, Caribbean, black British and other ethnic backgrounds (χ 2 (5, n=295)=14.46, p=0.013, ϕ=0.22). Duration of symptoms at presentation ranged from 0 to 16 years, with a median of 3.0 years (IQR 0.9, 6.0). CYP with longer duration of symptoms more frequently presented to paediatricians (χ 2 (1, n=272)=6.25, p=0.012, ϕ=0.15).
Paediatric cases (79 (41.8%)) were more likely to present with Combined subtype than psychiatric cases (43 (33.1%)). Psychiatrists reported a higher proportion of CYP with Fear subtype (17 (13.1%)) than paediatricians (6 (3.2%); χ 2 (3, n=319)=13.00, p=0.005, ϕ=0.20).
Clinical presentation
Clinical characteristics are shown in table 2 . 53.4% participants were normal weight, 34.2% underweight and 12.3% overweight or obese at presentation. Paediatricians were more likely to report overweight CYP (18 (13.0%)) than psychiatrists (2(2.1%); χ 2 (4, n=234)=10.29, p=0.036, ϕ=0.21) and mean SD BMI z -scores were lower in psychiatry settings (−1.70 (1.76)) than in paediatrics (−0.99 (1.79); t(232)=3.02; p=0.003; ɳ 2 =0.037). A higher proportion of cases presented with weight loss in the psychiatric cohort (99 (76.7%)) than the paediatric cohort (119 (65.0%); χ 2 (1, n=312)=4.93, p=0.026, ϕ=0.13). Paediatric cases (92 (50%)) were more frequently prescribed nutritional supplements than psychiatric cases (46 (37.4%); χ 2 (1, n=307)=4.73, p=0.030, ϕ=0.12), most commonly for iron and vitamin D deficiencies. The paediatric cohort also exhibited a higher tendency to exclude whole food groups (‘selective eating’) (116 (63.7%)) compared with those presenting to psychiatry (55 (46.6%); χ 2 (1, n=300)=8.57, p=0.003, ϕ=0.17).
Constipation (70 (21.9%)) was the most prevalent medical symptom/sign, followed by dizziness (56 (17.6%)), bradycardia (15 (14.4%)) and muscle wasting (26 (8.2%)). The psychiatric cohort (36 (27.7%)) experienced dizziness significantly more than the paediatric cohort (20 (10.6%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=15.58, p<0.001, ϕ=0.22). The paediatric cohort (53 (28.0%)) had higher rates of constipation than the psychiatric cohort (17 (13.1%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=10.07, p=0.002, ϕ=0.18). Menstrual status was reported in 28 of 145 females (19.3%). Of these, 10 (35.7%) had documented secondary amenorrhoea, 5 (33.3%) in the paediatric and 5 (38.5%) in the psychiatric cohort.
The psychiatric cohort were more likely to be reported as having comorbid anxiety (79 (78.2%)), depression (24 (25.5%)) or OCD (14 (15.7%)) than the paediatric cohort (72 (47.4%); χ 2 (1, n=253)=24.00, p<0.001, ϕ=0.31; 9 (6.3%); χ 2 (1, n=237)=17.51, p<0.001, ϕ=0.27; 8 (5.8%); χ 2 (1, n=226)=6.01, p=0.014, ϕ=0.16) and more likely to show deliberate self-harm (16 (17.6%)) than the paediatric cohort (12 (8.4%); χ 2 (1, n=234)=4.46, p=0.035, ϕ=0.14). More children with ASD (96 (67.6%)) and intellectual disabilities (ID) (55 (33.1%)) were reported by paediatricians than psychiatrists (43 (50.0%); χ 2 (1, n=228)=6.98, p=0.008, ϕ=0.18; 10 (10.2%); χ 2 (1, n=264)=17.46, p<0.001, ϕ=0.26).
Median time to diagnose ARFID was significantly higher in paediatric (1.1 months—IQR 0, 17.2) than psychiatric settings (0.5 months—IQR 0, 2.9; U=7568, z=−2.21, p=0.027, r=0.13). Management strategies are described in table 3 . Paediatricians were more likely to provide dietetic advice (157 (83.1%)) and medical monitoring (141 (74.6%)) and to prescribe nutritional supplements (93 (49.2%)) and tube feeding (16 (8.5%)) than psychiatrists (91 (70.0%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=7.60, p=0.006, ϕ=0.15; 69 (53.1%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=15.87, p<0.001, ϕ=0.22; 39 (30%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=11.71, p=0.001, ϕ=0.19; 4 (3.1%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=3.81, p=0.051, ϕ=0.11). Paediatricians drew input from other health professionals more frequently (66 (34.9%)), including dieticians and occupational therapists, than psychiatrists (29 (22.3%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=5.86, p=0.015, ϕ=0.14). Psychiatrists used more psychoeducation (87 (66.9%)) and individual psychological therapy (48 (36.9%)) than paediatricians (77 (40.7%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=21.14, p<0.001, ϕ=0.26; 36 (19.0%); χ 2 (1, n=319)=12.69, p<0.001, ϕ=0.20). Paediatricians referred 75 cases (39.7%) to a psychiatrist or psychologist; psychiatrists referred 11 cases (8.5%) to a paediatrician.
Management strategies
Table 4 displays the changes in nutritional status and eating behaviours after 1-year follow-up. SD BMI z -scores increased from baseline (−1.50 (1.71)) to follow-up (−1.26 (1.81); t(111)=−3.46; p=0.001; ɳ 2 =0.10) for the whole sample. CYP receiving psychiatric care were reported as having greater improvement in overall eating behaviour (57 (81.4%)) than paediatric patients (51 (53.7%); χ 2 (3, n=165)=21.89, p<0.001, ϕ=0.36) ( table 5 ).
Outcomes at 1-year follow-up
Overall clinical impression regarding the patient’s eating behaviours at follow-up
Four cases were reported to both BPSU and CAPSS: two girls and two boys. Median age was 12.8 years (IQR 11.4, 15.3) and median duration of symptoms was 2.4 years (IQR 1.0, 6.8). Three cases (75%) were from England. Three cases (75%) presented with weight loss (mean SD BMI z-score −2.5 (1.5)).
All results remained statistically significant in the sensitivity analysis except for comorbid OCD and input from other health professionals ( online supplemental material ).
To our knowledge, this is the first study to characterise and compare how ARFID presents in CYP between paediatric and psychiatric services. Our findings reveal that presentations varied by specialty, with distinct cohorts treated in the two settings. Significant differences between specialties included symptom duration, eating behaviours, clinical features, comorbidities, management approaches and long-term outcomes. Our findings serve as a key step towards developing evidence-based management pathways for this patient group.
Compared with psychiatry, the paediatric cohort consisted of more males and younger patients with longer symptom duration and more selective eating, nutritional deficiencies, constipation and higher BMIs. Literature suggests that lacking food diversity over long time periods predisposes individuals to malnutrition, altered bowel habits and weight gain, particularly with exclusion of fruits and vegetables. 22–24 Results suggest that psychiatric teams manage cases with shorter duration of symptoms, often a more acute clinical picture, characterised by anxiety, weight loss and dizziness. This presentation is consistent with prior research on manifestation of acute malnutrition in ARFID, including dizziness and fainting due to dehydration, hypotension or bradycardia 25 which correlates with the rate of weight loss. 26 The age difference between cohorts may be explained by distinct referral pathways, presenting symptoms and comorbidities. The distribution of comorbid psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders differed in each setting; cases seen by psychiatric teams reported as having higher rates of comorbid anxiety and depression, while more CYP with ID and ASD are treated within paediatrics. These profiles are consistent with prior research. 6 25 27 28
Despite consensus supporting a multidisciplinary approach, there was little overlap in cases presenting to paediatrics and psychiatry in our sample. Duplicates were more underweight than the general sample, suggesting more acuteness and therefore seen by both specialties; however, firm conclusions cannot be drawn from this small sample. Our data suggest clinicians in each specialty had distinct management approaches and both cohorts improved in rate of reported weight loss, nutritional deficiencies and exclusion of food groups. The psychiatric cohort additionally exhibited significantly greater improvements in disordered eating behaviours. Moreover, more psychiatrists reported case improvement at follow-up by clinical impression. To note, however, paediatricians and psychiatrists may have different perceptions of clinical improvement.
Patients with ARFID presenting to paediatric settings may also benefit from psychiatric services and psychological interventions, and it is important that clinicians identify these CYP. A multimodal medical and mental health approach to ARFID assessment and treatment on an ARFID-specific referral pathway 29 that facilitates access to and close collaboration between paediatrics and psychiatry would improve care of CYP with ARFID.
This is the first study comparing the clinical characteristics of ARFID in CYP accessing paediatric and psychiatric services. Active surveillance methodology enabled ascertainment of national prospective data from paediatricians and psychiatrists in the UK and ROI. This large sample included CYP of different ages and geographical areas, ensuring data were representative. However, in our prospective research design, loss to follow-up may introduce bias in data analysis. The design of the study meant we were unable to compensate for missing data in analyses. Age criteria for paediatric surveillance (5–16) differed from psychiatric surveillance (5–18), possibly accounting for differences between cohorts. A sensitivity analysis, excluding patients ≥16 years, showed our results were robust. We asked clinicians to report their overall clinical impression of patients’ eating behaviours at follow-up but did not ask them to report this at baseline. We may have introduced reporting bias as no comparisons could be made between cohorts at follow-up regarding medical conditions.
This study reveals variations in the presentation and management of newly diagnosed ARFID cases in different clinical settings. CYP in paediatrics presented more frequently with complications of chronic symptoms due to lack of food variety, often associated with comorbid ASD or ID. Psychiatric teams managed more acute presentations of food restriction, with more weight loss, fears of aversive consequences from eating and mental disorders such as anxiety. Patients managed by psychiatrists responded well to psychological interventions, suggesting that all ARFID CYP should be assessed for suitability for this. Results from this study add support to multidisciplinary management of ARFID and collaborative working across paediatrics and psychiatry.
Patient consent for publication.
Not applicable.
This study involves human participants and was approved by the West Midlands–Black Country Research Ethics Committee (Integrated Research Application System ID 273665; REC 20/WM/0256). Due to the nature of the study, using surveillance methodology, patient and parental consent was not required. As consent was not sought and minimal identifier data were required, approval under England and Wales Section 251 via Confidentiality Advisory Group of the Health Research Authority (20/CAG/0120) was obtained. Data were collected in Scotland following advice from the Public Benefit and Privacy Panel for Health and Social Care (HSC-PBPP) (2021-0113). Northern Ireland Privacy Advisory Committee requirements were met to collect data.
Our thanks go to all the busy paediatricians and child and adolescent psychiatrists who participated in the surveillance and reported cases and to the charities Autistica and ARFID Awareness UK for supporting the study.
EH and JS-C are joint first authors.
X @JaviSanCer1, @DashaNicholls
Contributors DN conceptualised the study. DN, JS-C, JN, RL and LDH contributed to the design and development of the study. EH and JS-C performed the analysis and drafted the manuscript. JS-C, JN and DN accessed and verified the data underlying the study. EH and JS-C contributed equally to this paper. All authors edited and approved the final version of the article. All authors confirmed that they had full access to all the data in the study and accepted responsibility to submit for publication. JS-C is the guarantor of this manuscript and accepted full responsibility for the work and conduct of the study, had access to the data and controlled the decision to publish.
Funding This study was funded by the Former EMS Ltd (charity number 1098725, registered 9 Oct 2017). JS-C is supported by a Fellowship funded by the Fundación Alicia Koplowitz. DN is supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration Northwest London and the NIHR Imperial Biomedical Research Collaboration.
Disclaimer The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care, NHS England and NHS Improvement.
Competing interests None declared.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.
IMAGES
COMMENTS
The following case studies present three different children with ASD and describe the SLP's strategies to enhance communication and quality of life. The three case studies demonstrate various options in AAC intervention that can be used by children of different ages. —Ann-Mari Pierotti, MS, CCC-SLP. Case Study 1: Anderson | Case Study 2 ...
The following case studies illustrate some common experiences of school-age children with autism. Case Study 3: Academic Challenges and Accommodations. Emma, a 9-year-old girl with high-functioning autism, excelled in mathematics but struggled with reading comprehension and writing. Her case study focuses on the academic challenges faced by ...
Abstract. This article aims to observe all the manifestations of the behavior of a child with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), which shows deficits mainly in the communication sector. Also, the ...
model language throughout the day by labeling objects and actions at least five times each day for two months, read bed time stories to Tomeika three times each week for two months, eek for two months, and provide Tomeika with the opportunity to request a desired item a minimum of five. s a day. for two months. Step 3. Devel.
These policy documents can help clinicians navigate the case studies presented below and assist with their own decisions about assessment and intervention tools and strategies. The following case studies present three different children with ASD and describe the SLP's strategies to enhance communication and quality of life.
Background. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by defective communication capacity, social impairment, stereotyped behaviors, and limited interests ().The latest prevalence of ASD in the United States is 2.3% ().There are at least 78 million people with ASD which makes it one of the fastest growing diseases in the world ().
Case Studies: sharing real-life outcomes and positive experiences. Getting it right for Early Intervention by Grandparent of a former pupil "Our grandson went to a small, local playschool where the kind staff tried valiantly to help him, but it was quite disastrous for him, the staff assigned to him and also the other children.
Autism Case Training: A Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics Curriculum 4. Distribute Case Study Part I. Slide 3. Case Study Part I. Billy is a 3 ½ -year-old boy you are seeing for the first time in your resident practice. He was born full term following a normal pregnancy and delivery. His newborn screen and neonatal hearing test were normal.
Focus of the case study Strategies to support the inclusion of an 8-year old autistic pupil with significant anxiety Principles illustrated 1. Understanding the strengths, interests and challenges of the autistic child and young person 2. Enabling the voice of the autistic child and young person to contribute to and influence decisions 3.
There are 8 individual case studies which consider the impact of Good Autism Practice on autistic children and young people in Early Years, Schools and Post-16. Download all the case studies above. The Good Autism Practice Guidance: Full Report. The Full Report and Practitioner Guide presents eight principles of good autism practice.
This case study is primarily aimed at improving the quality of life of children with ASD through architectural aspects of the indoor built environment. ... a case study of a child with autism. Int J Dev Disabil. (2020) 66:160-8. 10.1080/20473869.2019.1642640 [PMC free article] [Google Scholar] 77. Dirix H, Ross V, Brijs K, Vermeiren E ...
Participants. The six children (three girls and three boys) participating in the present case study were recruited from a large longitudinal population-based study, performed at the Child Neuropsychiatric Clinic (CNC) in Gothenburg (Kantzer et al., Citation 2013, Citation 2018).The children were initially identified by a general ASD screening at age 2.5 years at the public child health-care ...
The current study provides a case study of an early intensive behavioral intervention based on the CABAS® system for a young child diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder and tracks the progress made based on independent psychological assessments and behavioral assessment tools. Method Participant The participant was born on March 11th 2000 ...
This case study of an inclusive classroom provides a comprehensive description of writing processes allowing for demonstration of an assessment procedure and goal development. The case studied is a 6th grade inclusion classroom whereby 2 students were included and diagnosed with an ASD, their typically developing classmates served as a control ...
In previous studies, at least three profiles based on these skills have been suggested; autism with language and non-verbal cognitive skills within the average/normal range (ALN), autism with ...
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a blanket term which describes a range of individuals having atypical behaviours in two diagnostic domains: social communication and restricted or repetitive behaviours [1].ASD is also associated with language delays and intellectual disability in a large proportion of cases [2].Atypicalities in social interaction may manifest as difficulty in understanding ...
This article presents exploratory research on the feasibility of non-directive play therapy for children with autism. Video recordings of 16 sessions of play therapy with a 6-year-old boy with severe autism were analysed qualitatively and quantitatively. The study concluded that this child was able to enter into a therapeutic relationship and demonstrated attachment behaviour towards the ...
The diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is made on the basis of detailed information obtained from the child's caregivers, careful observation and assessment of the child, and the use of standardized tools designed to aid in the diagnosis of ASD. Key Learning Points of This Case. 1.
In this study, already-existing teaching methods are used to instruct children with autism, such as the TEACCH-method (Schopler and Mesibov 1995), social stories (Reynhout and Carter 2006), activity schedules (McClannahan and Krantz 1999) and how children read social situations and emotions (Howlin, Baron-Cohen, and Hadwin 1999; Gray 1995 ...
Case Study of a Child with the Autism Spectrum Disorder. IOSR Journals. This article aims to observe all the manifestations of the behavior of a child with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), which shows deficits mainly in the communication sector. Also, the child shows repetitive and stereotypical behaviors throughout the lesson (Stasinos, 2016).
Autism Spectrum Disorder and Social Story Research: a Scoping Study of Published, Peer-Reviewed Literature Reviews. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 9 (1), 21-38. https ...
This study used both exploratory (EFA) and confiiatory factor analy-ses (CFA) to examine the factor structure of the WISC-III among White and Black students from the WISC-III standardization ...
This research focuses on the assessment of attention to identify the design needs for optimized learning technologies for children with autism. Within a single case study incorporating a multiple baseline design involving baseline, intervention, and post-intervention phases, we developed an application enabling personalized attention strategies. These strategies were assessed for their ...
Objective To compare the clinical presentations, management and outcomes of avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) across paediatric and child and adolescent (C&A) psychiatric settings. Study design Prospective surveillance study. Methods Data were collected during a 13-month prospective surveillance study of children and adolescents with ARFID in the UK and Republic of Ireland ...