- Science, Tech, Math ›
- Chemistry ›
- Chemical Laws ›
Null Hypothesis Definition and Examples
PM Images / Getty Images
- Chemical Laws
- Periodic Table
- Projects & Experiments
- Scientific Method
- Biochemistry
- Physical Chemistry
- Medical Chemistry
- Chemistry In Everyday Life
- Famous Chemists
- Activities for Kids
- Abbreviations & Acronyms
- Weather & Climate
- Ph.D., Biomedical Sciences, University of Tennessee at Knoxville
- B.A., Physics and Mathematics, Hastings College
In a scientific experiment, the null hypothesis is the proposition that there is no effect or no relationship between phenomena or populations. If the null hypothesis is true, any observed difference in phenomena or populations would be due to sampling error (random chance) or experimental error. The null hypothesis is useful because it can be tested and found to be false, which then implies that there is a relationship between the observed data. It may be easier to think of it as a nullifiable hypothesis or one that the researcher seeks to nullify. The null hypothesis is also known as the H 0, or no-difference hypothesis.
The alternate hypothesis, H A or H 1 , proposes that observations are influenced by a non-random factor. In an experiment, the alternate hypothesis suggests that the experimental or independent variable has an effect on the dependent variable .
How to State a Null Hypothesis
There are two ways to state a null hypothesis. One is to state it as a declarative sentence, and the other is to present it as a mathematical statement.
For example, say a researcher suspects that exercise is correlated to weight loss, assuming diet remains unchanged. The average length of time to achieve a certain amount of weight loss is six weeks when a person works out five times a week. The researcher wants to test whether weight loss takes longer to occur if the number of workouts is reduced to three times a week.
The first step to writing the null hypothesis is to find the (alternate) hypothesis. In a word problem like this, you're looking for what you expect to be the outcome of the experiment. In this case, the hypothesis is "I expect weight loss to take longer than six weeks."
This can be written mathematically as: H 1 : μ > 6
In this example, μ is the average.
Now, the null hypothesis is what you expect if this hypothesis does not happen. In this case, if weight loss isn't achieved in greater than six weeks, then it must occur at a time equal to or less than six weeks. This can be written mathematically as:
H 0 : μ ≤ 6
The other way to state the null hypothesis is to make no assumption about the outcome of the experiment. In this case, the null hypothesis is simply that the treatment or change will have no effect on the outcome of the experiment. For this example, it would be that reducing the number of workouts would not affect the time needed to achieve weight loss:
H 0 : μ = 6
Null Hypothesis Examples
"Hyperactivity is unrelated to eating sugar " is an example of a null hypothesis. If the hypothesis is tested and found to be false, using statistics, then a connection between hyperactivity and sugar ingestion may be indicated. A significance test is the most common statistical test used to establish confidence in a null hypothesis.
Another example of a null hypothesis is "Plant growth rate is unaffected by the presence of cadmium in the soil ." A researcher could test the hypothesis by measuring the growth rate of plants grown in a medium lacking cadmium, compared with the growth rate of plants grown in mediums containing different amounts of cadmium. Disproving the null hypothesis would set the groundwork for further research into the effects of different concentrations of the element in soil.
Why Test a Null Hypothesis?
You may be wondering why you would want to test a hypothesis just to find it false. Why not just test an alternate hypothesis and find it true? The short answer is that it is part of the scientific method. In science, propositions are not explicitly "proven." Rather, science uses math to determine the probability that a statement is true or false. It turns out it's much easier to disprove a hypothesis than to positively prove one. Also, while the null hypothesis may be simply stated, there's a good chance the alternate hypothesis is incorrect.
For example, if your null hypothesis is that plant growth is unaffected by duration of sunlight, you could state the alternate hypothesis in several different ways. Some of these statements might be incorrect. You could say plants are harmed by more than 12 hours of sunlight or that plants need at least three hours of sunlight, etc. There are clear exceptions to those alternate hypotheses, so if you test the wrong plants, you could reach the wrong conclusion. The null hypothesis is a general statement that can be used to develop an alternate hypothesis, which may or may not be correct.
- Kelvin Temperature Scale Definition
- Independent Variable Definition and Examples
- Theory Definition in Science
- Hypothesis Definition (Science)
- de Broglie Equation Definition
- Law of Combining Volumes Definition
- Chemical Definition
- Pure Substance Definition in Chemistry
- Acid Definition and Examples
- Extensive Property Definition (Chemistry)
- Radiation Definition and Examples
- Valence Definition in Chemistry
- Atomic Solid Definition
- Weak Base Definition and Examples
- Oxidation Definition and Example in Chemistry
- Definition of Binary Compound
- Mathematics
- Number System and Arithmetic
- Trigonometry
- Probability
- Mensuration
- Maths Formulas
- Integration Formulas
- Differentiation Formulas
- Trigonometry Formulas
- Algebra Formulas
- Mensuration Formula
- Statistics Formulas
- Trigonometric Table
Null Hypothesis
Null Hypothesis , often denoted as H 0, is a foundational concept in statistical hypothesis testing. It represents an assumption that no significant difference, effect, or relationship exists between variables within a population. It serves as a baseline assumption, positing no observed change or effect occurring. The null is t he truth or falsity of an idea in analysis.
In this article, we will discuss the null hypothesis in detail, along with some solved examples and questions on the null hypothesis.
Table of Content
What is Null Hypothesis?
Null hypothesis symbol, formula of null hypothesis, types of null hypothesis, null hypothesis examples, principle of null hypothesis, how do you find null hypothesis, null hypothesis in statistics, null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis, null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis examples, null hypothesis - practice problems.
Null Hypothesis in statistical analysis suggests the absence of statistical significance within a specific set of observed data. Hypothesis testing, using sample data, evaluates the validity of this hypothesis. Commonly denoted as H 0 or simply "null," it plays an important role in quantitative analysis, examining theories related to markets, investment strategies, or economies to determine their validity.
Null Hypothesis Meaning
Null Hypothesis represents a default position, often suggesting no effect or difference, against which researchers compare their experimental results. The Null Hypothesis, often denoted as H 0 asserts a default assumption in statistical analysis. It posits no significant difference or effect, serving as a baseline for comparison in hypothesis testing.
The null Hypothesis is represented as H 0 , the Null Hypothesis symbolizes the absence of a measurable effect or difference in the variables under examination.
Certainly, a simple example would be asserting that the mean score of a group is equal to a specified value like stating that the average IQ of a population is 100.
The Null Hypothesis is typically formulated as a statement of equality or absence of a specific parameter in the population being studied. It provides a clear and testable prediction for comparison with the alternative hypothesis. The formulation of the Null Hypothesis typically follows a concise structure, stating the equality or absence of a specific parameter in the population.
Mean Comparison (Two-sample t-test)
H 0 : μ 1 = μ 2
This asserts that there is no significant difference between the means of two populations or groups.
Proportion Comparison
H 0 : p 1 − p 2 = 0
This suggests no significant difference in proportions between two populations or conditions.
Equality in Variance (F-test in ANOVA)
H 0 : σ 1 = σ 2
This states that there's no significant difference in variances between groups or populations.
Independence (Chi-square Test of Independence):
H 0 : Variables are independent
This asserts that there's no association or relationship between categorical variables.
Null Hypotheses vary including simple and composite forms, each tailored to the complexity of the research question. Understanding these types is pivotal for effective hypothesis testing.
Equality Null Hypothesis (Simple Null Hypothesis)
The Equality Null Hypothesis, also known as the Simple Null Hypothesis, is a fundamental concept in statistical hypothesis testing that assumes no difference, effect or relationship between groups, conditions or populations being compared.
Non-Inferiority Null Hypothesis
In some studies, the focus might be on demonstrating that a new treatment or method is not significantly worse than the standard or existing one.
Superiority Null Hypothesis
The concept of a superiority null hypothesis comes into play when a study aims to demonstrate that a new treatment, method, or intervention is significantly better than an existing or standard one.
Independence Null Hypothesis
In certain statistical tests, such as chi-square tests for independence, the null hypothesis assumes no association or independence between categorical variables.
Homogeneity Null Hypothesis
In tests like ANOVA (Analysis of Variance), the null hypothesis suggests that there's no difference in population means across different groups.
- Medicine: Null Hypothesis: "No significant difference exists in blood pressure levels between patients given the experimental drug versus those given a placebo."
- Education: Null Hypothesis: "There's no significant variation in test scores between students using a new teaching method and those using traditional teaching."
- Economics: Null Hypothesis: "There's no significant change in consumer spending pre- and post-implementation of a new taxation policy."
- Environmental Science: Null Hypothesis: "There's no substantial difference in pollution levels before and after a water treatment plant's establishment."
The principle of the null hypothesis is a fundamental concept in statistical hypothesis testing. It involves making an assumption about the population parameter or the absence of an effect or relationship between variables.
In essence, the null hypothesis (H 0 ) proposes that there is no significant difference, effect, or relationship between variables. It serves as a starting point or a default assumption that there is no real change, no effect or no difference between groups or conditions.
The null hypothesis is usually formulated to be tested against an alternative hypothesis (H 1 or H \alpha ) which suggests that there is an effect, difference or relationship present in the population.
Null Hypothesis Rejection
Rejecting the Null Hypothesis occurs when statistical evidence suggests a significant departure from the assumed baseline. It implies that there is enough evidence to support the alternative hypothesis, indicating a meaningful effect or difference. Null Hypothesis rejection occurs when statistical evidence suggests a deviation from the assumed baseline, prompting a reconsideration of the initial hypothesis.
Identifying the Null Hypothesis involves defining the status quotient, asserting no effect and formulating a statement suitable for statistical analysis.
When is Null Hypothesis Rejected?
The Null Hypothesis is rejected when statistical tests indicate a significant departure from the expected outcome, leading to the consideration of alternative hypotheses. It occurs when statistical evidence suggests a deviation from the assumed baseline, prompting a reconsideration of the initial hypothesis.
In statistical hypothesis testing, researchers begin by stating the null hypothesis, often based on theoretical considerations or previous research. The null hypothesis is then tested against an alternative hypothesis (Ha), which represents the researcher's claim or the hypothesis they seek to support.
The process of hypothesis testing involves collecting sample data and using statistical methods to assess the likelihood of observing the data if the null hypothesis were true. This assessment is typically done by calculating a test statistic, which measures the difference between the observed data and what would be expected under the null hypothesis.
In the realm of hypothesis testing, the null hypothesis (H 0 ) and alternative hypothesis (H₁ or Ha) play critical roles. The null hypothesis generally assumes no difference, effect, or relationship between variables, suggesting that any observed change or effect is due to random chance. Its counterpart, the alternative hypothesis, asserts the presence of a significant difference, effect, or relationship between variables, challenging the null hypothesis. These hypotheses are formulated based on the research question and guide statistical analyses.
Difference Between Null Hypothesis and Alternative Hypothesis
The null hypothesis (H 0 ) serves as the baseline assumption in statistical testing, suggesting no significant effect, relationship, or difference within the data. It often proposes that any observed change or correlation is merely due to chance or random variation. Conversely, the alternative hypothesis (H 1 or Ha) contradicts the null hypothesis, positing the existence of a genuine effect, relationship or difference in the data. It represents the researcher's intended focus, seeking to provide evidence against the null hypothesis and support for a specific outcome or theory. These hypotheses form the crux of hypothesis testing, guiding the assessment of data to draw conclusions about the population being studied.
Let's envision a scenario where a researcher aims to examine the impact of a new medication on reducing blood pressure among patients. In this context:
Null Hypothesis (H 0 ): "The new medication does not produce a significant effect in reducing blood pressure levels among patients."
Alternative Hypothesis (H 1 or Ha): "The new medication yields a significant effect in reducing blood pressure levels among patients."
The null hypothesis implies that any observed alterations in blood pressure subsequent to the medication's administration are a result of random fluctuations rather than a consequence of the medication itself. Conversely, the alternative hypothesis contends that the medication does indeed generate a meaningful alteration in blood pressure levels, distinct from what might naturally occur or by random chance.
People Also Read:
Mathematics Maths Formulas Probability and Statistics
Example 1: A researcher claims that the average time students spend on homework is 2 hours per night.
Null Hypothesis (H 0 ): The average time students spend on homework is equal to 2 hours per night. Data: A random sample of 30 students has an average homework time of 1.8 hours with a standard deviation of 0.5 hours. Test Statistic and Decision: Using a t-test, if the calculated t-statistic falls within the acceptance region, we fail to reject the null hypothesis. If it falls in the rejection region, we reject the null hypothesis. Conclusion: Based on the statistical analysis, we fail to reject the null hypothesis, suggesting that there is not enough evidence to dispute the claim of the average homework time being 2 hours per night.
Example 2: A company asserts that the error rate in its production process is less than 1%.
Null Hypothesis (H 0 ): The error rate in the production process is 1% or higher. Data: A sample of 500 products shows an error rate of 0.8%. Test Statistic and Decision: Using a z-test, if the calculated z-statistic falls within the acceptance region, we fail to reject the null hypothesis. If it falls in the rejection region, we reject the null hypothesis. Conclusion: The statistical analysis supports rejecting the null hypothesis, indicating that there is enough evidence to dispute the company's claim of an error rate of 1% or higher.
Q1. A researcher claims that the average time spent by students on homework is less than 2 hours per day. Formulate the null hypothesis for this claim?
Q2. A manufacturing company states that their new machine produces widgets with a defect rate of less than 5%. Write the null hypothesis to test this claim?
Q3. An educational institute believes that their online course completion rate is at least 60%. Develop the null hypothesis to validate this assertion?
Q4. A restaurant claims that the waiting time for customers during peak hours is not more than 15 minutes. Formulate the null hypothesis for this claim?
Q5. A study suggests that the mean weight loss after following a specific diet plan for a month is more than 8 pounds. Construct the null hypothesis to evaluate this statement?
Summary - Null Hypothesis and Alternative Hypothesis
The null hypothesis (H 0 ) and alternative hypothesis (H a ) are fundamental concepts in statistical hypothesis testing. The null hypothesis represents the default assumption, stating that there is no significant effect, difference, or relationship between variables. It serves as the baseline against which the alternative hypothesis is tested. In contrast, the alternative hypothesis represents the researcher's hypothesis or the claim to be tested, suggesting that there is a significant effect, difference, or relationship between variables. The relationship between the null and alternative hypotheses is such that they are complementary, and statistical tests are conducted to determine whether the evidence from the data is strong enough to reject the null hypothesis in favor of the alternative hypothesis. This decision is based on the strength of the evidence and the chosen level of significance. Ultimately, the choice between the null and alternative hypotheses depends on the specific research question and the direction of the effect being investigated.
FAQs on Null Hypothesis
What does null hypothesis stands for.
The null hypothesis, denoted as H 0 , is a fundamental concept in statistics used for hypothesis testing. It represents the statement that there is no effect or no difference, and it is the hypothesis that the researcher typically aims to provide evidence against.
How to Form a Null Hypothesis?
A null hypothesis is formed based on the assumption that there is no significant difference or effect between the groups being compared or no association between variables being tested. It often involves stating that there is no relationship, no change, or no effect in the population being studied.
When Do we reject the Null Hypothesis?
In statistical hypothesis testing, if the p-value (the probability of obtaining the observed results) is lower than the chosen significance level (commonly 0.05), we reject the null hypothesis. This suggests that the data provides enough evidence to refute the assumption made in the null hypothesis.
What is a Null Hypothesis in Research?
In research, the null hypothesis represents the default assumption or position that there is no significant difference or effect. Researchers often try to test this hypothesis by collecting data and performing statistical analyses to see if the observed results contradict the assumption.
What Are Alternative and Null Hypotheses?
The null hypothesis (H0) is the default assumption that there is no significant difference or effect. The alternative hypothesis (H1 or Ha) is the opposite, suggesting there is a significant difference, effect or relationship.
What Does it Mean to Reject the Null Hypothesis?
Rejecting the null hypothesis implies that there is enough evidence in the data to support the alternative hypothesis. In simpler terms, it suggests that there might be a significant difference, effect or relationship between the groups or variables being studied.
How to Find Null Hypothesis?
Formulating a null hypothesis often involves considering the research question and assuming that no difference or effect exists. It should be a statement that can be tested through data collection and statistical analysis, typically stating no relationship or no change between variables or groups.
How is Null Hypothesis denoted?
The null hypothesis is commonly symbolized as H 0 in statistical notation.
What is the Purpose of the Null hypothesis in Statistical Analysis?
The null hypothesis serves as a starting point for hypothesis testing, enabling researchers to assess if there's enough evidence to reject it in favor of an alternative hypothesis.
What happens if we Reject the Null hypothesis?
Rejecting the null hypothesis implies that there is sufficient evidence to support an alternative hypothesis, suggesting a significant effect or relationship between variables.
What are Test for Null Hypothesis?
Various statistical tests, such as t-tests or chi-square tests, are employed to evaluate the validity of the Null Hypothesis in different scenarios.
Similar Reads
- Geeks Premier League
- School Learning
- Geeks Premier League 2023
- Math-Concepts
Please Login to comment...
Improve your coding skills with practice.
What kind of Experience do you want to share?
Null hypothesis
Null hypothesis n., plural: null hypotheses [nʌl haɪˈpɒθɪsɪs] Definition: a hypothesis that is valid or presumed true until invalidated by a statistical test
Table of Contents
Null Hypothesis Definition
Null hypothesis is defined as “the commonly accepted fact (such as the sky is blue) and researcher aim to reject or nullify this fact”.
More formally, we can define a null hypothesis as “a statistical theory suggesting that no statistical relationship exists between given observed variables” .
In biology , the null hypothesis is used to nullify or reject a common belief. The researcher carries out the research which is aimed at rejecting the commonly accepted belief.
What Is a Null Hypothesis?
A hypothesis is defined as a theory or an assumption that is based on inadequate evidence. It needs and requires more experiments and testing for confirmation. There are two possibilities that by doing more experiments and testing, a hypothesis can be false or true. It means it can either prove wrong or true (Blackwelder, 1982).
For example, Susie assumes that mineral water helps in the better growth and nourishment of plants over distilled water. To prove this hypothesis, she performs this experiment for almost a month. She watered some plants with mineral water and some with distilled water.
In a hypothesis when there are no statistically significant relationships among the two variables, the hypothesis is said to be a null hypothesis. The investigator is trying to disprove such a hypothesis. In the above example of plants, the null hypothesis is:
There are no statistical relationships among the forms of water that are given to plants for growth and nourishment.
Usually, an investigator tries to prove the null hypothesis wrong and tries to explain a relation and association between the two variables.
An opposite and reverse of the null hypothesis are known as the alternate hypothesis . In the example of plants the alternate hypothesis is:
There are statistical relationships among the forms of water that are given to plants for growth and nourishment.
The example below shows the difference between null vs alternative hypotheses:
Alternate Hypothesis: The world is round Null Hypothesis: The world is not round.
Copernicus and many other scientists try to prove the null hypothesis wrong and false. By their experiments and testing, they make people believe that alternate hypotheses are correct and true. If they do not prove the null hypothesis experimentally wrong then people will not believe them and never consider the alternative hypothesis true and correct.
The alternative and null hypothesis for Susie’s assumption is:
- Null Hypothesis: If one plant is watered with distilled water and the other with mineral water, then there is no difference in the growth and nourishment of these two plants.
- Alternative Hypothesis: If one plant is watered with distilled water and the other with mineral water, then the plant with mineral water shows better growth and nourishment.
The null hypothesis suggests that there is no significant or statistical relationship. The relation can either be in a single set of variables or among two sets of variables.
Most people consider the null hypothesis true and correct. Scientists work and perform different experiments and do a variety of research so that they can prove the null hypothesis wrong or nullify it. For this purpose, they design an alternate hypothesis that they think is correct or true. The null hypothesis symbol is H 0 (it is read as H null or H zero ).
Why is it named the “Null”?
The name null is given to this hypothesis to clarify and explain that the scientists are working to prove it false i.e. to nullify the hypothesis. Sometimes it confuses the readers; they might misunderstand it and think that statement has nothing. It is blank but, actually, it is not. It is more appropriate and suitable to call it a nullifiable hypothesis instead of the null hypothesis.
Why do we need to assess it? Why not just verify an alternate one?
In science, the scientific method is used. It involves a series of different steps. Scientists perform these steps so that a hypothesis can be proved false or true. Scientists do this to confirm that there will be any limitation or inadequacy in the new hypothesis. Experiments are done by considering both alternative and null hypotheses, which makes the research safe. It gives a negative as well as a bad impact on research if a null hypothesis is not included or a part of the study. It seems like you are not taking your research seriously and not concerned about it and just want to impose your results as correct and true if the null hypothesis is not a part of the study.
Development of the Null
In statistics, firstly it is necessary to design alternate and null hypotheses from the given problem. Splitting the problem into small steps makes the pathway towards the solution easier and less challenging. how to write a null hypothesis?
Writing a null hypothesis consists of two steps:
- Firstly, initiate by asking a question.
- Secondly, restate the question in such a way that it seems there are no relationships among the variables.
In other words, assume in such a way that the treatment does not have any effect.
The usual recovery duration after knee surgery is considered almost 8 weeks.
A researcher thinks that the recovery period may get elongated if patients go to a physiotherapist for rehabilitation twice per week, instead of thrice per week, i.e. recovery duration reduces if the patient goes three times for rehabilitation instead of two times.
Step 1: Look for the problem in the hypothesis. The hypothesis either be a word or can be a statement. In the above example the hypothesis is:
“The expected recovery period in knee rehabilitation is more than 8 weeks”
Step 2: Make a mathematical statement from the hypothesis. Averages can also be represented as μ, thus the null hypothesis formula will be.
In the above equation, the hypothesis is equivalent to H1, the average is denoted by μ and > that the average is greater than eight.
Step 3: Explain what will come up if the hypothesis does not come right i.e., the rehabilitation period may not proceed more than 08 weeks.
There are two options: either the recovery will be less than or equal to 8 weeks.
H 0 : μ ≤ 8
In the above equation, the null hypothesis is equivalent to H 0 , the average is denoted by μ and ≤ represents that the average is less than or equal to eight.
What will happen if the scientist does not have any knowledge about the outcome?
Problem: An investigator investigates the post-operative impact and influence of radical exercise on patients who have operative procedures of the knee. The chances are either the exercise will improve the recovery or will make it worse. The usual time for recovery is 8 weeks.
Step 1: Make a null hypothesis i.e. the exercise does not show any effect and the recovery time remains almost 8 weeks.
H 0 : μ = 8
In the above equation, the null hypothesis is equivalent to H 0 , the average is denoted by μ, and the equal sign (=) shows that the average is equal to eight.
Step 2: Make the alternate hypothesis which is the reverse of the null hypothesis. Particularly what will happen if treatment (exercise) makes an impact?
In the above equation, the alternate hypothesis is equivalent to H1, the average is denoted by μ and not equal sign (≠) represents that the average is not equal to eight.
Significance Tests
To get a reasonable and probable clarification of statistics (data), a significance test is performed. The null hypothesis does not have data. It is a piece of information or statement which contains numerical figures about the population. The data can be in different forms like in means or proportions. It can either be the difference of proportions and means or any odd ratio.
The following table will explain the symbols:
P-value is the chief statistical final result of the significance test of the null hypothesis.
- P-value = Pr(data or data more extreme | H 0 true)
- | = “given”
- Pr = probability
- H 0 = the null hypothesis
The first stage of Null Hypothesis Significance Testing (NHST) is to form an alternate and null hypothesis. By this, the research question can be briefly explained.
Null Hypothesis = no effect of treatment, no difference, no association Alternative Hypothesis = effective treatment, difference, association
When to reject the null hypothesis?
Researchers will reject the null hypothesis if it is proven wrong after experimentation. Researchers accept null hypothesis to be true and correct until it is proven wrong or false. On the other hand, the researchers try to strengthen the alternate hypothesis. The binomial test is performed on a sample and after that, a series of tests were performed (Frick, 1995).
Step 1: Evaluate and read the research question carefully and consciously and make a null hypothesis. Verify the sample that supports the binomial proportion. If there is no difference then find out the value of the binomial parameter.
Show the null hypothesis as:
H 0 :p= the value of p if H 0 is true
To find out how much it varies from the proposed data and the value of the null hypothesis, calculate the sample proportion.
Step 2: In test statistics, find the binomial test that comes under the null hypothesis. The test must be based on precise and thorough probabilities. Also make a list of pmf that apply, when the null hypothesis proves true and correct.
When H 0 is true, X~b(n, p)
N = size of the sample
P = assume value if H 0 proves true.
Step 3: Find out the value of P. P-value is the probability of data that is under observation.
Rise or increase in the P value = Pr(X ≥ x)
X = observed number of successes
P value = Pr(X ≤ x).
Step 4: Demonstrate the findings or outcomes in a descriptive detailed way.
- Sample proportion
- The direction of difference (either increases or decreases)
Perceived Problems With the Null Hypothesis
Variable or model selection and less information in some cases are the chief important issues that affect the testing of the null hypothesis. Statistical tests of the null hypothesis are reasonably not strong. There is randomization about significance. (Gill, 1999) The main issue with the testing of the null hypothesis is that they all are wrong or false on a ground basis.
There is another problem with the a-level . This is an ignored but also a well-known problem. The value of a-level is without a theoretical basis and thus there is randomization in conventional values, most commonly 0.q, 0.5, or 0.01. If a fixed value of a is used, it will result in the formation of two categories (significant and non-significant) The issue of a randomized rejection or non-rejection is also present when there is a practical matter which is the strong point of the evidence related to a scientific matter.
The P-value has the foremost importance in the testing of null hypothesis but as an inferential tool and for interpretation, it has a problem. The P-value is the probability of getting a test statistic at least as extreme as the observed one.
The main point about the definition is: Observed results are not based on a-value
Moreover, the evidence against the null hypothesis was overstated due to unobserved results. A-value has importance more than just being a statement. It is a precise statement about the evidence from the observed results or data. Similarly, researchers found that P-values are objectionable. They do not prefer null hypotheses in testing. It is also clear that the P-value is strictly dependent on the null hypothesis. It is computer-based statistics. In some precise experiments, the null hypothesis statistics and actual sampling distribution are closely related but this does not become possible in observational studies.
Some researchers pointed out that the P-value is depending on the sample size. If the true and exact difference is small, a null hypothesis even of a large sample may get rejected. This shows the difference between biological importance and statistical significance. (Killeen, 2005)
Another issue is the fix a-level, i.e., 0.1. On the basis, if a-level a null hypothesis of a large sample may get accepted or rejected. If the size of simple is infinity and the null hypothesis is proved true there are still chances of Type I error. That is the reason this approach or method is not considered consistent and reliable. There is also another problem that the exact information about the precision and size of the estimated effect cannot be known. The only solution is to state the size of the effect and its precision.
Null Hypothesis Examples
Here are some examples:
Example 1: Hypotheses with One Sample of One Categorical Variable
Among all the population of humans, almost 10% of people prefer to do their task with their left hand i.e. left-handed. Let suppose, a researcher in the Penn States says that the population of students at the College of Arts and Architecture is mostly left-handed as compared to the general population of humans in general public society. In this case, there is only a sample and there is a comparison among the known population values to the population proportion of sample value.
- Research Question: Do artists more expected to be left-handed as compared to the common population persons in society?
- Response Variable: Sorting the student into two categories. One category has left-handed persons and the other category have right-handed persons.
- Form Null Hypothesis: Arts and Architecture college students are no more predicted to be lefty as compared to the common population persons in society (Lefty students of Arts and Architecture college population is 10% or p= 0.10)
Example 2: Hypotheses with One Sample of One Measurement Variable
A generic brand of antihistamine Diphenhydramine making medicine in the form of a capsule, having a 50mg dose. The maker of the medicines is concerned that the machine has come out of calibration and is not making more capsules with the suitable and appropriate dose.
- Research Question: Does the statistical data recommended about the mean and average dosage of the population differ from 50mg?
- Response Variable: Chemical assay used to find the appropriate dosage of the active ingredient.
- Null Hypothesis: Usually, the 50mg dosage of capsules of this trade name (population average and means dosage =50 mg).
Example 3: Hypotheses with Two Samples of One Categorical Variable
Several people choose vegetarian meals on a daily basis. Typically, the researcher thought that females like vegetarian meals more than males.
- Research Question: Does the data recommend that females (women) prefer vegetarian meals more than males (men) regularly?
- Response Variable: Cataloguing the persons into vegetarian and non-vegetarian categories. Grouping Variable: Gender
- Null Hypothesis: Gender is not linked to those who like vegetarian meals. (Population percent of women who eat vegetarian meals regularly = population percent of men who eat vegetarian meals regularly or p women = p men).
Example 4: Hypotheses with Two Samples of One Measurement Variable
Nowadays obesity and being overweight is one of the major and dangerous health issues. Research is performed to confirm that a low carbohydrates diet leads to faster weight loss than a low-fat diet.
- Research Question: Does the given data recommend that usually, a low-carbohydrate diet helps in losing weight faster as compared to a low-fat diet?
- Response Variable: Weight loss (pounds)
- Explanatory Variable: Form of diet either low carbohydrate or low fat
- Null Hypothesis: There is no significant difference when comparing the mean loss of weight of people using a low carbohydrate diet to people using a diet having low fat. (population means loss of weight on a low carbohydrate diet = population means loss of weight on a diet containing low fat).
Example 5: Hypotheses about the relationship between Two Categorical Variables
A case-control study was performed. The study contains nonsmokers, stroke patients, and controls. The subjects are of the same occupation and age and the question was asked if someone at their home or close surrounding smokes?
- Research Question: Did second-hand smoke enhance the chances of stroke?
- Variables: There are 02 diverse categories of variables. (Controls and stroke patients) (whether the smoker lives in the same house). The chances of having a stroke will be increased if a person is living with a smoker.
- Null Hypothesis: There is no significant relationship between a passive smoker and stroke or brain attack. (odds ratio between stroke and the passive smoker is equal to 1).
Example 6: Hypotheses about the relationship between Two Measurement Variables
A financial expert observes that there is somehow a positive and effective relationship between the variation in stock rate price and the quantity of stock bought by non-management employees
- Response variable- Regular alteration in price
- Explanatory Variable- Stock bought by non-management employees
- Null Hypothesis: The association and relationship between the regular stock price alteration ($) and the daily stock-buying by non-management employees ($) = 0.
Example 7: Hypotheses about comparing the relationship between Two Measurement Variables in Two Samples
- Research Question: Is the relation between the bill paid in a restaurant and the tip given to the waiter, is linear? Is this relation different for dining and family restaurants?
- Explanatory Variable- total bill amount
- Response Variable- the amount of tip
- Null Hypothesis: The relationship and association between the total bill quantity at a family or dining restaurant and the tip, is the same.
Try to answer the quiz below to check what you have learned so far about the null hypothesis.
Choose the best answer.
Send Your Results (Optional)
- Blackwelder, W. C. (1982). “Proving the null hypothesis” in clinical trials. Controlled Clinical Trials , 3(4), 345–353.
- Frick, R. W. (1995). Accepting the null hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 23(1), 132–138.
- Gill, J. (1999). The insignificance of null hypothesis significance testing. Political Research Quarterly , 52(3), 647–674.
- Killeen, P. R. (2005). An alternative to null-hypothesis significance tests. Psychological Science, 16(5), 345–353.
©BiologyOnline.com. Content provided and moderated by Biology Online Editors.
Last updated on June 16th, 2022
You will also like...
Genetic mutations.
This tutorial looks at the mutation at the gene level and the harm it may bring. Learn about single nucleotide polymorph..
Birth Control and Contraception
Different pregnancy and birth control and contraception strategies are described. Read this tutorial to learn each of th..
Abiotic and Biotic Factors
This tutorial deals with the abiotic factors of the freshwater environment that determine what sort of life would be sui..
The Gene Pool and Population Genetics
According to Charles Darwin's theory of natural selection, preferable genes are favored by nature in the gene pool, and ..
Human Biology – Food and Digestion
This tutorial recognizes the importance of food as a source of energy that will fuel many biological processes. A good d..
New Zealand’s Unique Flora
If New Zealand has lots of unique animals, it's also got a whole lot of unique plants. Find out more about some of them,..
Related Articles...
No related articles found
- Science Notes Posts
- Contact Science Notes
- Todd Helmenstine Biography
- Anne Helmenstine Biography
- Free Printable Periodic Tables (PDF and PNG)
- Periodic Table Wallpapers
- Interactive Periodic Table
- Periodic Table Posters
- Science Experiments for Kids
- How to Grow Crystals
- Chemistry Projects
- Fire and Flames Projects
- Holiday Science
- Chemistry Problems With Answers
- Physics Problems
- Unit Conversion Example Problems
- Chemistry Worksheets
- Biology Worksheets
- Periodic Table Worksheets
- Physical Science Worksheets
- Science Lab Worksheets
- My Amazon Books
Null Hypothesis Examples
The null hypothesis (H 0 ) is the hypothesis that states there is no statistical difference between two sample sets. In other words, it assumes the independent variable does not have an effect on the dependent variable in a scientific experiment .
The null hypothesis is the most powerful type of hypothesis in the scientific method because it’s the easiest one to test with a high confidence level using statistics. If the null hypothesis is accepted, then it’s evidence any observed differences between two experiment groups are due to random chance. If the null hypothesis is rejected, then it’s strong evidence there is a true difference between test sets or that the independent variable affects the dependent variable.
- The null hypothesis is a nullifiable hypothesis. A researcher seeks to reject it because this result strongly indicates observed differences are real and not just due to chance.
- The null hypothesis may be accepted or rejected, but not proven. There is always a level of confidence in the outcome.
What Is the Null Hypothesis?
The null hypothesis is written as H 0 , which is read as H-zero, H-nought, or H-null. It is associated with another hypothesis, called the alternate or alternative hypothesis H A or H 1 . When the null hypothesis and alternate hypothesis are written mathematically, they cover all possible outcomes of an experiment.
An experimenter tests the null hypothesis with a statistical analysis called a significance test. The significance test determines the likelihood that the results of the test are not due to chance. Usually, a researcher uses a confidence level of 95% or 99% (p-value of 0.05 or 0.01). But, even if the confidence in the test is high, there is always a small chance the outcome is incorrect. This means you can’t prove a null hypothesis. It’s also a good reason why it’s important to repeat experiments.
Exact and Inexact Null Hypothesis
The most common type of null hypothesis assumes no difference between two samples or groups or no measurable effect of a treatment. This is the exact hypothesis . If you’re asked to state a null hypothesis for a science class, this is the one to write. It is the easiest type of hypothesis to test and is the only one accepted for certain types of analysis. Examples include:
There is no difference between two groups H 0 : μ 1 = μ 2 (where H 0 = the null hypothesis, μ 1 = the mean of population 1, and μ 2 = the mean of population 2)
Both groups have value of 100 (or any number or quality) H 0 : μ = 100
However, sometimes a researcher may test an inexact hypothesis . This type of hypothesis specifies ranges or intervals. Examples include:
Recovery time from a treatment is the same or worse than a placebo: H 0 : μ ≥ placebo time
There is a 5% or less difference between two groups: H 0 : 95 ≤ μ ≤ 105
An inexact hypothesis offers “directionality” about a phenomenon. For example, an exact hypothesis can indicate whether or not a treatment has an effect, while an inexact hypothesis can tell whether an effect is positive of negative. However, an inexact hypothesis may be harder to test and some scientists and statisticians disagree about whether it’s a true null hypothesis .
How to State the Null Hypothesis
To state the null hypothesis, first state what you expect the experiment to show. Then, rephrase the statement in a form that assumes there is no relationship between the variables or that a treatment has no effect.
Example: A researcher tests whether a new drug speeds recovery time from a certain disease. The average recovery time without treatment is 3 weeks.
- State the goal of the experiment: “I hope the average recovery time with the new drug will be less than 3 weeks.”
- Rephrase the hypothesis to assume the treatment has no effect: “If the drug doesn’t shorten recovery time, then the average time will be 3 weeks or longer.” Mathematically: H 0 : μ ≥ 3
This null hypothesis (inexact hypothesis) covers both the scenario in which the drug has no effect and the one in which the drugs makes the recovery time longer. The alternate hypothesis is that average recovery time will be less than three weeks:
H A : μ < 3
Of course, the researcher could test the no-effect hypothesis (exact null hypothesis): H 0 : μ = 3
The danger of testing this hypothesis is that rejecting it only implies the drug affected recovery time (not whether it made it better or worse). This is because the alternate hypothesis is:
H A : μ ≠ 3 (which includes μ <3 and μ >3)
Even though the no-effect null hypothesis yields less information, it’s used because it’s easier to test using statistics. Basically, testing whether something is unchanged/changed is easier than trying to quantify the nature of the change.
Remember, a researcher hopes to reject the null hypothesis because this supports the alternate hypothesis. Also, be sure the null and alternate hypothesis cover all outcomes. Finally, remember a simple true/false, equal/unequal, yes/no exact hypothesis is easier to test than a more complex inexact hypothesis.
- Adèr, H. J.; Mellenbergh, G. J. & Hand, D. J. (2007). Advising on Research Methods: A Consultant’s Companion . Huizen, The Netherlands: Johannes van Kessel Publishing. ISBN 978-90-79418-01-5 .
- Cox, D. R. (2006). Principles of Statistical Inference . Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-68567-2 .
- Everitt, Brian (1998). The Cambridge Dictionary of Statistics . Cambridge, UK New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521593465.
- Weiss, Neil A. (1999). Introductory Statistics (5th ed.). ISBN 9780201598773.
Related Posts
15 Null Hypothesis Examples
Chris Drew (PhD)
Dr. Chris Drew is the founder of the Helpful Professor. He holds a PhD in education and has published over 20 articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education. [Image Descriptor: Photo of Chris]
Learn about our Editorial Process
A null hypothesis is a general assertion or default position that there is no relationship or effect between two measured phenomena.
It’s a critical part of statistics, data analysis, and the scientific method . This concept forms the basis of testing statistical significance and allows researchers to be objective in their conclusions.
A null hypothesis helps to eliminate biases and ensures that the observed results are not due to chance. The rejection or failure to reject the null hypothesis helps in guiding the course of research.
Null Hypothesis Definition
The null hypothesis, often denoted as H 0 , is the hypothesis in a statistical test which proposes no statistical significance exists in a set of observed data.
It hypothesizes that any kind of difference or importance you see in a data set is due to chance.
Null hypotheses are typically proposed to be negated or disproved by statistical tests, paving way for the acceptance of an alternate hypothesis.
Importantly, a null hypothesis cannot be proven true; it can only be supported or rejected with confidence.
Should evidence – via statistical analysis – contradict the null hypothesis, it is rejected in favor of an alternative hypothesis. In essence, the null hypothesis is a tool to challenge and disprove that there is no effect or relationship between variables.
Video Explanation
I like to show this video to my students which outlines a null hypothesis really clearly and engagingly, using real life studies by research students! The into explains it really well:
“There’s an idea in science called the null hypothesis and it works like this: when you’re setting out to prove a theory, your default answer should be “it’s not going to work” and you have to convince the world otherwise through clear results”
Here’s the full video:
Null Hypothesis Examples
- Equality of Means: The null hypothesis posits that the average of group A does not differ from the average of group B. It suggests that any observed difference between the two group means is due to sampling or experimental error.
- No Correlation: The null hypothesis states there is no correlation between the variable X and variable Y in the population. It means that any correlation seen in sample data occurred by chance.
- Drug Effectiveness: The null hypothesis proposes that a new drug does not reduce the number of days to recover from a disease compared to a standard drug. Any observed difference is merely by chance and not due to the new drug.
- Classroom Teaching Method: The null hypothesis states that a new teaching method does not result in improved test scores compared to the traditional teaching method. Any improvement in scores can be attributed to chance or other unrelated factors.
- Smoking and Life Expectancy: The null hypothesis asserts that the average life expectancy of smokers is the same as that of non-smokers. Any perceived difference in life expectancy is due to random variation or other factors.
- Brand Preference: The null hypothesis suggests that the proportion of consumers preferring Brand A is the same as those preferring Brand B. Any observed preference in the sample is due to random selection.
- Vaccination Efficacy: The null hypothesis states that the efficacy of Vaccine A does not differ from that of Vaccine B. Any differences observed in a sample are due to chance or other confounding factors.
- Diet and Weight Loss: The null hypothesis proposes that following a specific diet does not result in more weight loss than not following the diet. Any weight loss observed among dieters is considered random or influenced by other factors.
- Exercise and Heart Rate: The null hypothesis states that regular exercise does not lower resting heart rate compared to no exercise. Any lower heart rates observed in exercisers could be due to chance or other unrelated factors.
- Climate Change: The null hypothesis asserts that the average global temperature this decade is not higher than the previous decade. Any observed temperature increase can be attributed to random variation or unaccounted factors.
- Gender Wage Gap: The null hypothesis posits that men and women earn the same average wage for the same job. Any observed wage disparity is due to chance or non-gender related factors.
- Psychotherapy Effectiveness: The null hypothesis states that patients undergoing psychotherapy do not show more improvement than those not undergoing therapy. Any improvement in the
- Energy Drink Consumption and Sleep: The null hypothesis proposes that consuming energy drinks does not affect the quantity of sleep. Any observed differences in sleep duration among energy drink consumers is due to random variation or other factors.
- Organic Food and Health: The null hypothesis asserts that consuming organic food does not lead to better health outcomes compared to consuming non-organic food. Any health differences observed in consumers of organic food are considered random or attributed to other confounding factors.
- Online Learning Effectiveness: The null hypothesis states that students learning online do not perform differently on exams than students learning in traditional classrooms. Any difference in performance can be attributed to chance or unrelated factors.
Null Hypothesis vs Alternative Hypothesis
An alternative hypothesis is the direct contrast to the null hypothesis. It posits that there is a statistically significant relationship or effect between the variables being observed.
If the null hypothesis is rejected based on the test data, the alternative hypothesis is accepted.
Importantly, while the null hypothesis is typically a statement of ‘no effect’ or ‘no difference,’ the alternative hypothesis states that there is an effect or difference.
Comprehension Checkpoint: How does the null hypothesis help to ensure that research is objective and unbiased?
Applications of the Null Hypothesis in Research
The null hypothesis plays a critical role in numerous research settings, promoting objectivity and ensuring findings aren’t due to random chance.
- Clinical Trials: Null hypothesis is used extensively in medical and pharmaceutical research. For example, when testing a new drug’s effectiveness, the null hypothesis might state that the drug has no effect on the disease. If data contradicts this, the null hypothesis is rejected, suggesting the drug might be effective.
- Business and Economics: Businesses use null hypotheses to make informed decisions. For instance, a company might use a null hypothesis to test if a new marketing strategy improves sales. If data suggests a significant increase in sales, the null hypothesis is rejected, and the new strategy may be implemented.
- Psychological Research: Psychologists use null hypotheses to test theories about behavior. For instance, a null hypothesis might state there’s no link between stress and sleep quality. Rejecting this hypothesis based on collected data could help establish a correlation between the two variables.
- Environmental Science: Null hypotheses are used to understand environmental changes. For instance, researchers might form a null hypothesis stating there is no significant difference in air quality before and after a policy change. If this hypothesis is rejected, it indicates the policy may have impacted air quality.
- Education: Educators and researchers often use null hypotheses to improve teaching methods. For example, a null hypothesis might propose a new teaching technique doesn’t enhance student performance. If data contradicts this, the technique may be beneficial.
In all these areas, the null hypothesis helps minimize bias, enabling researchers to support their findings with statistically significant data. It forms the backbone of many scientific research methodologies , promoting a disciplined approach to uncovering new knowledge.
See More Hypothesis Examples Here
The null hypothesis is a cornerstone of statistical analysis and empirical research. It serves as a starting point for investigations, providing a baseline premise that the observed effects are due to chance. By understanding and applying the concept of the null hypothesis, researchers can test the validity of their assumptions, making their findings more robust and reliable. In essence, the null hypothesis ensures that the scientific exploration remains objective, systematic, and free from unintended bias.
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 10 Reasons you’re Perpetually Single
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 20 Montessori Toddler Bedrooms (Design Inspiration)
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 21 Montessori Homeschool Setups
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 101 Hidden Talents Examples
Leave a Comment Cancel Reply
Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *
An official website of the United States government
Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.
- Publications
- Account settings
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
When are hypotheses useful in ecology and evolution?
Matthew g betts, adam s hadley, david w frey, sarah j k frey, dusty gannon, scott h harris, urs g kormann, kara leimberger, katie moriarty, joseph m northrup, josée s rousseau, thomas d stokely, jonathon j valente, diego zárrate‐charry.
- Author information
- Article notes
- Copyright and License information
Correspondence , Matthew G. Betts, Forest Biodiversity Research Network, Department of Forest Ecosystems and Society, Forest Biodiversity Research Network, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331, USA. Email: [email protected]
Corresponding author.
Revised 2021 Feb 4; Received 2021 Jan 12; Accepted 2021 Feb 8; Collection date 2021 Jun.
This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Research hypotheses have been a cornerstone of science since before Galileo. Many have argued that hypotheses (1) encourage discovery of mechanisms, and (2) reduce bias—both features that should increase transferability and reproducibility. However, we are entering a new era of big data and highly predictive models where some argue the hypothesis is outmoded. We hypothesized that hypothesis use has declined in ecology and evolution since the 1990s, given the substantial advancement of tools further facilitating descriptive, correlative research. Alternatively, hypothesis use may have become more frequent due to the strong recommendation by some journals and funding agencies that submissions have hypothesis statements. Using a detailed literature analysis ( N = 268 articles), we found prevalence of hypotheses in eco–evo research is very low (6.7%–26%) and static from 1990–2015, a pattern mirrored in an extensive literature search ( N = 302,558 articles). Our literature review also indicates that neither grant success nor citation rates were related to the inclusion of hypotheses, which may provide disincentive for hypothesis formulation. Here, we review common justifications for avoiding hypotheses and present new arguments based on benefits to the individual researcher. We argue that stating multiple alternative hypotheses increases research clarity and precision, and is more likely to address the mechanisms for observed patterns in nature. Although hypotheses are not always necessary, we expect their continued and increased use will help our fields move toward greater understanding, reproducibility, prediction, and effective conservation of nature.
Keywords: hypothesis, mechanisms, multiple working hypotheses, prediction, scientific method
We use a quantitative literature review to show that use of a priori hypotheses is still rare in the fields of ecology and evolution. We provide suggestions about the group and individual‐level benefits of hypothesis use.
1. INTRODUCTION
Why should ecologists have hypotheses? At the beginning of most science careers, there comes a time of “hypothesis angst” where students question the need for the hypothetico‐deductive approach their elders have deemed essential for good science. Why is it not sufficient to just have a research objective or question? Why can't we just collect observations and describe those in our research papers?
Research hypotheses are explanations for an observed phenomenon (Loehle, 1987 ; Wolff & Krebs, 2008 ) (see Box 1 ) and have been proposed as a central tool of science since Galileo and Francis Bacon in the mid‐1600s (Glass & Hall, 2008 ). Over the past century, there have been repeated calls for rigorous application of hypotheses in science, and arguments that hypothesis use is the cornerstone of the scientific method (Chamberlin, 1890 ; Popper, 1959 ; Romesburg, 1981 ). In a seminal paper in Science, Platt ( 1964 ) challenged all scientific fields to adopt and rigorously test multiple hypotheses (sensu Chamberlin, 1890 ), arguing that without such hypothesis tests, disciplines would be prone to “stamp collecting” (Landy, 1986 ). To constitute “strong inference,” Platt required the scientific method to be a three‐step process including (1) developing alternative hypotheses, (2) devising a set of “crucial” experiments to eliminate all but one hypothesis, and (3) performing the experiments (Elliott & Brook, 2007 ).
BOX 1. Definitions of hypotheses and associated terms.
Hypothesis : An explanation for an observed phenomenon.
Research Hypothesis: A statement about a phenomenon that also includes the potential mechanism or cause of that phenomenon. Though a research hypothesis doesn't need to adhere to this strict framework it is often best described as the “if” in an “if‐then” statement. In other words, “if X is true” (where X is the mechanism or cause for an observed phenomenon) “then Y” (where Y is the outcome of a crucial test that supports the hypothesis). These can also be thought of as “ mechanistic hypotheses ” since they link with a causal mechanism. For example, trees grow slowly at high elevation because of nutrient limitation (hypothesis); if this is the case, fertilizing trees should result in more rapid growth (prediction).
Prediction: The potential outcome of a test that would support a hypothesis. Most researchers call the second part of the if‐then statement a “prediction”.
Multiple alternative hypotheses: Multiple plausible explanations for the same phenomenon.
Descriptive Hypothesis: Descriptive statements or predictions with the word “hypothesis” in front of them. Typically researchers state their guess about the results they expect and call this the “hypothesis” (e.g., “I hypothesize trees at higher elevation will grow slowly”).
Statistical Hypothesis : A predicted pattern in data that should occur if a research hypothesis is true.
Null Hypothesis : A concise statement expressing the concept of “no difference” between a sample and the population mean.
The commonly touted strengths of hypotheses are two‐fold. First, by adopting multiple plausible explanations for a phenomenon (hereafter “ multiple alternative hypotheses ”; Box 1 ), a researcher reduces the chance that they will become attached to a single possibility, thereby biasing research in favor of this outcome (Chamberlin, 1890 ); this “confirmation bias” is a well‐known human trait (Loehle, 1987 ; Rosen, 2016 ) and likely decreases reproducibility (Munafò et al., 2017 ). Second, various authors have argued that the a priori hypothesis framework forces one to think in advance about—and then test—various causes for patterns in nature (Wolff & Krebs, 2008 ), rather than simply examining the patterns themselves and coming up with explanations after the fact (so called “inductive research;” Romesburg, 1981 ). By understanding and testing mechanisms, science becomes more reliable and transferable (Ayres & Lombardero, 2017 ; Houlahan et al., 2017 ; Sutherland et al., 2013 ) (Figure 1 ). Importantly, both of these strengths should have strong, positive impacts on reproducibility of ecological and evolutionary studies (see Discussion).
Understanding mechanisms often increases model transferability. Panels (a and b) show snowshoe hares in winter and summer coloration, respectively. If a correlative (i.e., nonmechanistic) model for hare survival as a function of color was trained only on hares during the winter and then extrapolated into the summer months, it would perform poorly (white hares would die disproportionately under no‐snow conditions). On the other hand, a researcher testing mechanisms for hare survival would (ideally via experimentation) arrive at the conclusion that it is not the whiteness of hares, but rather blending with the background that confers survival (the “camouflage” hypothesis). Understanding mechanism results in model predictions being robust to novel conditions. Panel (c) Shows x and y geographic locations of training (blue filled circles) and testing (blue open circles) locations for a hypothetical correlative model. Even if the model performs well on these independent test data (predicting open to closed circles), there is no guarantee that it will predict well outside of the spatial bounds of the existing data (red circles). Nonstationarity (in this case caused by a nonlinear relationship between predictor and response variable; panel d) could result in correlative relationships shifting substantially if extrapolated to new times or places. However, mechanistic hypotheses aimed at understanding the underlying factors driving the distribution of this species would be more likely to elucidate this nonlinear relationship. In both of these examples, understanding drivers behind ecological patterns—via testing mechanistic hypotheses—is likely to enhance model transferability
However, we are entering a new era of ecological and evolutionary science that is characterized by massive datasets on genomes, species distributions, climate, land cover, and other remotely sensed information (e.g., bioacoustics, camera traps; Pettorelli et al., 2017 ). Exceptional computing power and new statistical and machine‐learning algorithms now enable thousands of statistical models to be run in minutes. Such datasets and methods allow for pattern recognition at unprecedented spatial scales and for huge numbers of taxa and processes. Indeed, there have been recent arguments in both the scientific literature and popular press to do away with the traditional scientific method and a priori hypotheses (Glass & Hall, 2008 ; Golub, 2010 ). These arguments go something along the lines of “if we can get predictions right most of the time, why do we need to know the cause?”
In this paper, we sought to understand if hypothesis use in ecology and evolution has shifted in response to these pressures on the discipline. We, therefore, hypothesized that hypothesis use has declined in ecology and evolution since the 1990s, given the substantial advancement of tools further facilitating descriptive, correlative research (e.g., Cutler et al., 2007 ; Elith et al., 2008 ). We predicted that this decline should be particularly evident in the applied conservation literature—where the emergence of machine‐learning models has resulted in an explosion of conservation‐oriented species distribution models (Elith et al., 2006 ). Our alternative hypothesis was that hypothesis use has become more frequent. The mechanism for such increases is that higher‐profile journals (e.g., Functional Ecology , Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Ser. B ) and competitive granting agencies (e.g., the U.S. National Science Foundation) now require or strongly encourage hypothesis statements.
As noted above, many have argued that hypotheses are useful and important for overall progress in science, because they facilitate the discovery of mechanisms, reduce bias, and increase reproducibility (Platt, 1964 ). However, for hypothesis use to be propagated among scientists, one would also expect hypotheses to confer benefits to the individual. We, therefore, tested whether hypothesis use was associated with individual‐level incentives relevant to academic success: publications, citations, and grants (Weinberg, 2010 ). If hypothesis use confers individual‐level advantages, then hypothesis‐based research should be (1) published in more highly ranked journals, (2) have higher citation rates, and (3) be supported by highly competitive funding sources.
Finally, we also present some common justifications for absence of hypotheses and suggest potential counterpoints researchers should consider prior to dismissing hypothesis use, including potential benefits to the individual researcher. We hope this communication provides practical recommendations for improving hypothesis use in ecology and evolution—particularly for new practitioners in the field (Box 2 ).
BOX 2. Recommendations for improving hypotheses use in ecology and evolution.
Authors : Know that you are human and prone to confirmation bias and highly effective at false pattern recognition. Thus, inductive research and single working hypotheses should be rare in your research. Remember that if your work is to have a real “impact”, it needs to withstand multiple tests from other labs over the coming decades.
Editors and Reviewers : Reward research that is conducted using principles of sound scientific method. Be skeptical of research that smacks of data dredging, post hoc hypothesis development, and single hypotheses. If no hypotheses are stated in a paper and/or the paper is purely descriptive, ask whether the novelty of the system and question warrant this, or if the field would have been better served by a study with mechanistic hypotheses. If only single hypotheses are stated, ask whether appropriate precautions were taken for the researcher to avoid finding support for a pet idea (e.g., blinded experiments, randomized attribution of treatments, etc.). To paraphrase Platt ( 1964 ): beware of the person with only one method or one instrument, either experimental or theoretical.
Mentors : Encourage your advisees to think carefully about hypothesis use and teach them how to construct sound multiple, mechanistic hypotheses. Importantly, explain why hypotheses are important to the scientific method, the individual and group consequences of excluding them, and the rare instances where they may not be necessary.
Policymakers/media/educators/students/readers : Read scientific articles with skepticism; have a scrutinous eye out for single hypothesis studies and p‐hacking. Reward multi‐hypothesis, mechanistic, predictive science by giving it greater weight in policy decisions (Sutherland et al., 2013 ), more coverage in the media, greater leverage in education, and more citations in reports.
2.1. Literature analysis
To examine hypothesis use over time and test whether hypothesis presence was associated with research type (basic vs. applied), journal impact factor, citation rates, and grants, we sampled the ecology and evolution literature using a stratified random sample of ecology and evolution journals in existence before 1991. First, we randomly selected 19 journals across impact factor (IF) strata ranging from 0.5–10.0 in two bins (<3 IF and ≥3 IF; see Figure 3 for full journal list). We then added three multidisciplinary journals that regularly publish ecology and evolution articles ( Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Science, and Nature ). From this sample of 22 journals, we randomly selected ecology and evolution articles within 5‐year strata beginning in 1991 (3 articles/journal per 5‐year bin) to ensure the full date range was evenly sampled. We removed articles in the following categories: editorials, corrections, reviews, opinions, and methods papers. In multidisciplinary journals, we examined only ecology, evolution, and conservation biology articles, as indicated by section headers in each journal. Once selected, articles were randomly distributed to the authors of the current paper (hereafter “reviewers:” MGB, ASH, DF, SF, DG, SH, HK, UK, KL, KM, JN, BP, JSR, TSS, JV, DZC) for detailed examination. On rare occasions, an article was not found, or reviewers were not able to complete their review. Ultimately, our final sample comprised 268 articles.
Frequency distributions showing proportion of various hypotheses types across ecology and evolution journals included in our detailed literature search. Hypothesis use varied greatly across publication outlets. We considered J. Applied Ecology, J. Wildlife Management, J. Soil, and Water Cons., Ecological Applications, Conservation Biology, and Biological Conservation to be applied journals; both applied and basic journals varied greatly in the prevalence of hypotheses
Reviewers were given a maximum of 10 min to find research hypothesis statements within the abstract or introduction of articles. We chose 10 min to simulate the amount of time that a journal editor pressed for time might spend evaluating the introductory material in an article. After this initial 10 min period, we determined: (1) whether or not an article contained at least one hypothesis, (2) whether hypotheses were mechanistic or not (i.e., the authors claimed to examine the mechanism for an observed phenomenon), (3) whether multiple alternative hypotheses were considered (sensu Chamberlin, 1890 ), and (4) whether hypotheses were “descriptive” (that is, they did not explore a mechanism but simply stated the expected direction of an effect; we define this as a “prediction” [Box 1 ]). It is important to note that to be identified as having hypotheses, articles did not need to contain the actual term “hypothesis” under our protocol; we also included articles using phrases such as “If X is true, we expected …” or “ we anticipated, ” both of which reflect a priori expectations from the data. We categorized each article as either basic (fundamental research without applications as a focus) or applied (clear management or conservation focus to article). Finally, we also examined all articles for funding sources and noted the presence of a national or international‐level competitive grant (e.g., National Science Foundation, European Union, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council). We assumed that published articles would have fidelity to the hypotheses stated in original grant proposals that funded the research, therefore, the acknowledgment of a successful grant is an indicator of financial reward for including hypotheses in initial proposals. Journal impact factors and individual article citation rates were gleaned directly from Web of Science. We reasoned that many researchers seek out journals with higher impact factors for the first submission of their manuscripts (Paine & Fox, 2018 ). Our assumption was that studies with more careful experimental design—including hypotheses—should be published where initially submitted, whereas those without may be eventually published, on average, in lower impact journals (Opthof et al., 2000 ). Ideally, we could have included articles that were rejected and never published in our analysis, but such articles are notoriously difficult to track (Thornton & Lee, 2000 ).
To support our detailed literature analysis, we also tested for temporal trends in hypothesis use within a broader sample of the ecology and evolution literature. For the same set of 22 journals in our detailed sample, we conducted a Web of Science search for articles containing “hypoth*” in the title or abstract. To calculate the proportion of articles with hypotheses (from 1990–2018), we divided the number of articles with hypotheses by the total number of articles ( N = 302,558). Because our search method does not include the main text of articles and excludes more subtle ways of stating hypotheses (e.g., “We expected…,” “We predicted…”), we acknowledge that the proportion of papers identified is likely to be an underestimate of the true proportions. Nevertheless, we do not expect that the degree of underestimation would change over time, so temporal trends in the proportion of papers containing hypotheses should be unbiased.
2.2. Statistical analysis
We used generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) to test for change in the prevalance of various hypothesis types over time (descriptive, mechanistic, multiple, any hypothesis). Presence of a hypothesis was modeled as dichotomous (0,1) with binomial error structure, and “journal” was included as a random effect to account for potential lack of independence among articles published in the same outlet. The predictor variable (i.e., year) was scaled to enable convergence. Similarly, we tested for differences in hypothesis prevalence between basic and applied articles using GLMMs with “journal” as a random effect. Finally, we tested the hypothesis that hypothesis use might decline over time due to the emergence of machine‐learning in the applied conservation literature; specifically, we modeled “hypothesis presence” as a function of the statistical interaction between “year” and “basic versus applied” articles. We conducted this test for all hypothesis types. GLMMs were implemented in R (version 3.60) using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2018 ). In three of our models, the “journal” random effect standard deviation was estimated to be zero or nearly zero (i.e., 10 –8 ). In such cases, the model with the random effect is exceptionally difficult to estimate, and the random effect standard deviation being estimated as approximately zero indicates the random effect was likely not needed.
We tested whether the presence of hypotheses influenced the likelihood of publication in a high‐impact journal using generalized linear models with a Gaussian error structure. We used the log of journal impact factor (+0.5) as the response variable to improve normality of model residuals. We tested the association between major competitive grants and the presence of a hypotheses using generalized linear models (logistic regression) with “hypothesis presence” (0,1) as a predictor and presence of a grant (0,1) as a response.
Finally, we tested whether hypotheses increase citation rates using linear mixed effects models (LMMs); presence of various hypotheses (0,1) were predictors in univariate models and average citations per year (log‐transformed) was the response. “Journal” was treated as a random effect, which assumes that articles within a particular journal are unlikely to be independent in their citation rates. LMMs were implemented in R using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015 ).
3.1. Trends in hypothesis use in ecology and evolution
In the ecology and evolution articles we examined in detail, the prevalence of multiple alternative hypotheses (6.7%) and mechanistic hypotheses (26%) was very low and showed no temporal trend (GLMM: multiple alternative: β ^ = 0.098 [95% CI: −0.383, 0.595], z = 0.40, p = 0.69, mechanistic: β ^ = 0.131 [95% CI: −0.149, 0.418], z = 0.92, p = 0.36, Figure 2a,b ). Descriptive hypothesis use was also low (8.5%), and although we observed a slight tendency to increase over time, 95% confidence intervals overlapped zero (GLMM: β ^ = 0.351 [95% CI: −0.088, 0.819], z = 1.53, p = 0.13, Figure 2c ). Although the proportion of papers containing no hypotheses appears to have declined (Figure 2d ), this effect was not statistically significant (GLMM: β ^ = −0.201 [95% CI: −0.483, 0.074], z = −1.41, p = 0.15). This overall pattern is consistent with a Web of Science search ( N = 302,558 articles) for the term “hypoth*” in titles or abstracts that shows essentially no trend over the same time period (Figure 2e,f ).
Trends in hypothesis use from 1991–2015 from a sample of the ecological and evolutionary literature ( N = 268, (a) multiple alternative hypotheses, (b) mechanistic hypotheses, (c) descriptive hypotheses [predictions], and (d) no hypotheses present). We detected no temporal trend in any of these variables. Lines reflect LOESS smoothing with 95% confidence intervals. Dots show raw data with darker colors indicating overlapping data points. The total number of publications in ecology and evolution in selected journals has increased (e), but use of the term “hypoth*” in the title or abstracts of these 302,558 articles has remained flat, and at very low prevalence (f)
Counter to our hypothesis, applied and basic articles did not show a statistically significant difference in the prevalence of either mechanistic (GLMM: β ^ = 0.054 [95% CI: −0.620, 0.728], z = 0.16, p = 0.875) or multiple alternative hypotheses (GLMM: β ^ = 0.517 [95% CI: −0.582, 1.80], z = 0.88, p = 0.375). Although both basic and applied ecology and evolution articles containing hypotheses were similarly rare overall, there was a tendency for applied ecology articles to show increasing prevalence of mechanistic hypothesis use over time, whereas basic ecology articles have remained relatively unchanged (Table S1 , Figure S1 ). However, there was substantial variation across both basic and applied journals in the prevalence of hypotheses (Figure 3 ).
3.2. Do hypotheses “pay?”
We found little evidence that presence of hypotheses increased paper citation rates. Papers with mechanistic (LMM: β ^ = −0.109 [95% CI: −0.329, 0.115], t = 0.042, p = 0.97, Figure 4a , middle panel) or multiple alternative hypotheses (LMM: β ^ = −0.008 [95% CI: −0.369, 0.391], t = 0.042, p = 0.96, Figure 4a , bottom panel) did not have higher average annual citation rates, nor did papers with at least one hypothesis type (LMM: β ^ = −0.024 [95% CI: −0.239, 0.194], t = 0.218, p = 0.83, Figure 4a , top panel).
Results of our detailed literature search showing the relationship between having a hypothesis (or not) and three commonly sought after scientific rewards (Average times a paper is cited/year, Journal impact factor, and the likelihood of having a major national competitive grant). We found no statistically significant relationships between having a hypothesis and citation rates or grants, but articles with hypotheses tended to be published in higher impact journals
On the other hand, journal articles containing mechanistic hypotheses tended to be published in higher impact journals (GLM: β ^ = 0.290 [95% CI: 0.083, 0.497], t = 2.74, p = 0.006) but only slightly so (Figure 4b , middle panel). Including multiple alternative hypotheses in papers did not have a statistically significant effect (GLM: = 0.339 [95% CI: −0.029, 0.707], t = 1.80, p = 0.072, Figure 4b , bottom panel).
Finally, we found no association between obtaining a competitive national or international grant and the presence of a hypothesis (logistic regression: mechanistic: β ^ = −0.090 [95% CI: −0.637, 0.453], z = −0.36, p =0 .745; multiple alternative: β ^ = 0.080 [95% CI: −0.891, 1.052], z = 0.49, p = 0.870; any hypothesis: β ^ = −0.005 [95% CI: −0.536, 0.525], z = −0.02, p = 0.986, Figure 4c ).
4. DISCUSSION
Overall, the prevalence of hypothesis use in the ecological and evolutionary literature is strikingly low and has been so for the past 25 years despite repeated calls to reverse this pattern (Elliott & Brook, 2007 ; Peters, 1991 ; Rosen, 2016 ; Sells et al., 2018 ). Why is this the case?
Clearly, hypotheses are not always necessary and a portion of the sampled articles may represent situations where hypotheses are truly not useful (see Box 3 : “When Are Hypotheses Not Useful?”). Some authors (Wolff & Krebs, 2008 ) overlook knowledge gathering and descriptive research as a crucial first step for making observations about natural phenomena—from which hypotheses can be formulated. This descriptive work is an important part of ecological science (Tewksbury et al., 2014 ), but may not benefit from strict use of hypotheses. Similarly, some efforts are simply designed to be predictive, such as auto‐recognition of species via machine learning (Briggs et al., 2012 ) or for prioritizing conservation efforts (Wilson et al., 2006 ), where the primary concern is correct identification and prediction rather than the biological or computational reasons for correct predictions (Box 3 ). However, it would be surprising if 75% of ecology since 1990 has been purely descriptive work from little‐known systems or purely predictive in nature. Indeed, the majority of the articles we observed did not fall into these categories.
BOX 3. When are hypotheses not useful?
Of course, there are a number of instances where hypotheses might not be useful or needed. It is important to recognize these instances to prevent the pendulum from swinging in a direction where without hypotheses, research ceases to be considered science (Wolff & Krebs, 2008 ). Below are several important types of ecological research where formulating hypotheses may not always be beneficial.
When the goal is prediction rather than understanding. Examples of this exception include species distribution models (Elith et al., 2008 ) where the question is not why species are distributed as they are, but simply where species are predicted to be. Such results can be useful in conservation planning (Guisan et al., 2013 ; see below). Another example lies in auto‐recognition of species (Briggs et al., 2012 ) where the primary concern is getting identification right rather than the biological or computational reasons for correct predictions. In such instances, complex algorithms can be very effective at uncovering patterns (e.g., deep learning). A caveat and critical component of such efforts is to ensure that such models are tested on independent data. Further, if model predictions are made beyond the spatial or temporal bounds of training or test data, extreme caution should be applied (see Figure 4 ).
When the goal is description rather than understanding. In many applications, the objective is to simply quantify a pattern in nature; for example, where on Earth is forest loss most rapid (Hansen et al., 2013 )? Further, sometimes so little is known about a system or species that formulating hypotheses is impossible and more description is necessary. In rare instances, an ecological system may be so poorly known and different to other systems that generating testable hypotheses would be extremely challenging. Darwin's observations while traveling on the Beagle are some of the best examples of such “hypothesis generating” science; these initial observations resulted in the formulation of one of the most extensively tested hypotheses in biology. However, such novelty should be uncommon in ecological and evolutionary research where theoretical and empirical precedent abounds (Sells et al., 2018 ). In the field of biogeography, there is the commonly held view that researchers should first observe and analyze patterns, and only then might explanations emerge (“pattern before process”); however, it has frequently been demonstrated that mechanistic hypotheses are useful even in disciplines where manipulative experiments are impossible (Crisp et al., 2011 ).
When the objective is a practical planning outcome such as reserve design. In many conservation planning efforts, the goal is not to uncover mechanisms, but rather simply to predict efficient methods or contexts for conserving species (Myers et al., 2000 ; Wilson et al., 2006 ). Perhaps this is the reason for such low prevalence of hypotheses in conservation journals (e.g., Conservation Biology).
Alternatively, researchers may not include hypotheses because they see little individual‐level incentive for their inclusion. Our results suggest that currently there are relatively few measurable benefits to individuals. Articles with mechanistic hypotheses do tend to be published in higher impact factor journals, which, for better or worse, is one of the key predictors in obtaining an academic job (van Dijk et al., 2014 ). However, few of the other typical academic metrics (i.e., citations or grant funding) appear to reward this behavior. Although hypotheses might be “useful” for overall progress in science (Platt, 1964 ), for their use to be propagated in the population of scientists, one would also expect them to provide benefits to the individuals conducting the science. Interestingly, the few existing papers on hypotheses (Loehle, 1987 ; Romesburg, 1981 ; Sells et al., 2018 ) tended to explain the advantages in terms of benefits to the group by offering arguments such as “because hypotheses help the field move forward more rapidly”.
Here we address some common justifications for hypotheses being unnecessary and show how one's first instinct to avoid hypotheses may be mistaken. We also present four reasons that use of hypotheses may be of individual self‐interest.
5. RESPONSES TO COMMON JUSTIFICATIONS FOR THE ABSENCE OF HYPOTHESES
During our collective mentoring at graduate and undergraduate levels, as well as examination of the literature, we have heard a number of common justifications for why hypotheses are not included. We must admit that many of us have, on occasion, rationalized absence of hypotheses in our own work using the same logic! We understand that clearly formulating and testing hypotheses can often be challenging, but propose that the justifications for avoiding hypotheses should be carefully considered.
“ But I do have hypotheses ”. Simply using the word “hypothesis” does not a hypothesis make. A common pattern in the literature we reviewed was for researchers to state their guess about the results they expect and call this the “hypothesis” (e.g., “I hypothesize trees at higher elevation will grow slowly”). But these are usually predictions derived from an implicit theoretical model (Symes et al., 2015 ) or are simply descriptive statements with the word “hypothesis” in front of them (see Box 1 ). A research hypothesis must contain explanations for an observed phenomenon (Loehle, 1987 ; Wolff & Krebs, 2008 ). Such explanations are derived from existing or new theory (Symes et al., 2015 ). Making the link between the expected mechanism (hypothesis) and logical outcome if that mechanism were true (the prediction), is a key element of strong inference. Similarly, using “statistical hypotheses” and “null hypothesis testing” is not the same as developing mechanistic research hypotheses (Romesburg, 1981 ; Sells et al., 2018 ).
“ Not enough is known about my system to formulate hypotheses ”. This is perhaps the most common defense against needing hypotheses (Golub, 2010 ). The argument goes that due to lack of previous research no mature theory has developed, so formal tests are impossible. Such arguments may have basis in some truly novel contexts (e.g., exploratory research on genomes) (Golub, 2010 ). But on close inspection, similar work has often been conducted in other geographic regions, systems, or with different taxa. If the response by a researcher is “but we really need to know if X pattern also applies in this region” (e.g., does succession influence bird diversity in forests of Western North America the same way as it does in Eastern forests), this is fine and it is certainly useful to accumulate descriptive studies globally for future synthetic work. However, continued efforts at description alone constitute missed opportunities for understanding the mechanisms behind a pattern (e.g., why does bird diversity decline when the forest canopy closes?). Often with a little planning, both the initial descriptive local interest question (e.g., “is it?”) and the broader interest question (i.e., “why?”) can both be tackled with minimal additional effort.
“ What about Darwin? Many important discoveries have been made without hypotheses .” Several authors (and many students) have argued that many important and reliable patterns in nature have emerged outside of the hypothetico‐deductive (H‐D) method (Brush, 1974 ). For instance, Darwin's discovery of natural selection as a key force for evolution has been put forward as an example of how reliable ideas can emerge without the H‐D method (May, 1981 ; Milner, 2018 ). Examination of Darwin's notebooks has suggested that he did not propose explicit hypotheses and test them (Brush, 1974 ). However, Darwin himself wrote “all observation must be for or against some view if it is to be of any service!” (Ayala, 2009 ). In fact, Darwin actually put forward and empirically tested hypotheses in multiple fields, including geology, plant morphology and physiology, psychology, and evolution (Ayala, 2009 ). This debate suggests that, like Darwin, we should continue to value systematic observation and descriptive science (Tewksbury et al., 2014 ), but whenever possible, it should be with a view toward developing theory and testing hypotheses
The statement that “many important discoveries have been made without hypotheses” stems from a common misconception that somehow hypotheses spring fully formed into the mind, and that speculation, chance and induction play no role in the H‐D method. As noted by Loehle ( 1987 ; p. 402) “The H‐D method and strong inference, however, are valid no matter how theories are obtained. Dreams, crystal balls, or scribbled notebooks are all allowed. In fact, induction may be used to create empirical relations which then become candidates for hypothesis testing even though induction cannot be used to prove anything”. So, although induction has frequently been used to develop theory, it is an unreliable means to test theory (Popper, 1959 ). As is well‐known, Darwin's theory of natural selection was heavily debated in scientific circles at the time, and it is only through countless hypothesis tests that it remains the best explanation for evolution even today (Mayr, 2002 ).
“ Ecology is too complex for hypotheses ”. In one of the most forcefully presented arguments for the H‐D method, Karl Popper ( 1959 ) argued that science should be done through a process of falsification; that is, multiple hypotheses should be constructed and the researcher's role is to successively eliminate these one at a time via experimentation until a single plausible hypothesis remains. This approach has caused some consternation among ecologists because the idea of single causes to phenomena doesn't match most of our experiences (Quinn & Dunham, 1983 ); rather, multiple interacting processes often overlap to drive observed patterns. For example, Robert Paine found that the distribution of a common seaweed was best explained by competition, physical disturbance, and dispersal ability (Paine, 1966 ).
It would be interesting if Popperian logic has inoculated ecology and evolution against the frequent application of hypotheses in research. Perhaps because the bar of falsification and testable mutually exclusive hypotheses is so high, many have opted to ignore the need for hypotheses altogether. If this is the case, our response is that in ecology and evolution we must not let Popperian perfection be the enemy of strong inference. With sufficient knowledge of a system, formal a priori hypotheses can be formulated that directly address the possibility of nonlinear relationships and interactions among variables. An example from conservation biology is the well‐explored hypothesis that the effects of habitat fragmentation should be greatest when habitat amount is low due to dispersal limitation (i.e., there should be a statistical interaction between fragmentation and habitat loss (Andrén, 1994 )).
“ But I am not a physiologist .” A common misconception has to do with the hierarchical aspect of mechanisms (Figure 5 ). Many think that they are not testing the mechanism for a pattern because they have not managed to get to the bottom of a causal hierarchy (which reflects a sort of physics envy that commonly occurs in ecology and evolution (Egler, 1986 )). However, hierarchy theory (O'Neill et al., 1989 ), states that the cause of a given phenomenon usually occurs at the level of organization just below the observed phenomenon. So, for example, species distributions might be best understood by examining hypotheses about the spatial composition and configuration of landscapes (Fahrig, 2003 ), explanations for population regulation might be best explored through observing the reproductive success and survival of individual organisms (Lack, 1954 ), and to understand individual variation in fecundity, one might test hypotheses relating to individual behavior or physiology. Hypothesis generation is possible at all levels of organization (Figure 5 ). Support for a hypothesis at one level often generates a subsequent question and hypotheses at the next (e.g., Observation: variation in animal densities can best be explained by forest patch size; Question: why are densities lower in small patches? H 1 : small patches have more edge, and predation rates are higher at the edge). However, in a single research project it is not necessary to develop hypotheses that address mechanisms at all scales.
“ But my model predicts patterns well ”. An increasingly common justification for not presenting and testing research hypotheses seems to be the notion that if large datasets and complex modeling methods can predict outcomes effectively, what is the need for hypothesizing a mechanism (Glass & Hall, 2008 ; Golub, 2010 )? Indeed, some have argued that prediction is a gold standard in ecology and evolution (Houlahan et al., 2017 ). However, underlying such arguments is the critical assumption that the relationship between predictors (i.e., independent variables, 'x's) and responses ('y's) exhibit stationarity in time and space. Although this appears to be the case in cosmology (e.g., relativity is thought to apply wherever you are in the universe (Einstein, 1920 )), the assumption of stationarity has repeatedly been shown to be violated in ecological and evolutionary studies (Betts et al., 2006 ; Osborne et al., 2007 ; Thompson, 2005 ). Hence the well‐known maxim “correlation does not equal causation;” correlates of a phenomenon often shift, even if the underlying cause remains the same.
Hypothesis generation is possible at all levels of organization, and does not need to get to the bottom of a causal hierarchy to be useful. As illustrated in this case study (after Betts et al., 2015 ), using published work by the authors, support for a hypothesis at one level often generates a subsequent question and hypotheses at the next. After each new finding we had to return to the white board and draw out new alternative hypotheses as we progressed further down the hierarchy. Supported hypotheses are shown in black and the alternative hypotheses that were eliminated are in grey. A single study is not expected to tackle an entire mechanistic hierarchy. In fact, we still have yet to uncover the physiological mechanisms involved in this phenomenon
The advantage of understanding mechanism is that the relationship between cause and effect is less likely to shift in space and time than between the correlates of a phenomenon (Sells et al., 2018 ) (Figure 1 ). For instance, climate‐envelope models are still commonly used to predict future species distributions (Beale et al., 2008 ) despite the fact that links between correlates often fail (Gutiérrez et al., 2014 ) and climate per se may not be the direct driver of distributions. In an example from our own group, predictions that fit observed data well in the region where the model was built completely failed when predicted to a new region only 250 km away (Betts et al., 2006 ). Although it is true that mechanisms can also exhibit nonstationarity, at least in these instances logic can inform decisions about whether or not causal factors are likely to hold in a new place or time.
6. WHY SHOULD YOU HAVE HYPOTHESES? (A SELF‐INTERESTED PERSPECTIVE)
We have already described two arguments for hypothesis use, both of which should have positive influences on reproducibility and therefore progress in science: (1) multiple alternative hypotheses developed a priori prevent attachment to a single idea, and (2) hypotheses encourage exploration of mechanisms, which should increase the transferability of findings to new systems. Both these arguments have been made frequently in the eco‐evolutionary literature for decades (Elliott & Brook, 2007 ; Loehle, 1987 ; Rosen, 2016 ; Sells et al., 2018 ), but our results show that such arguments have been lost on the majority of researchers. One hypothesis recently proposed to explain why “poor methods persist [in science] despite perennial calls for improvements” is that such arguments have largely failed because they do not appeal to researcher self‐interest (Smaldino & McElreath, 2016 ). In periods of intense competition for grants and top‐tier publications, perhaps arguments that rely on altruism fall short. However, happily, there are at least four self‐interested reasons that students of ecological and evolutionary science should adopt the hypothetico‐deductive method.
Clarity and Precision in Research
First, and most apparent during our review of the literature, hypotheses force clarity and precision in thinking. We often found it difficult to determine the core purpose of papers that lacked clear hypotheses. One of the key goals of scientific writing is to communicate ideas efficiently (Schimel, 2011 ). Increased clarity through use of hypotheses could potentially even explain the pattern for manuscripts using hypotheses getting published in higher impact journals. Editors are increasingly pressed for time and forced to reject the majority of papers submitted to higher impact outlets prior to detailed review (AAAS, 2018 ). “Unclear message” and “lack of clear hypotheses” are top reasons a paper ends up in the editor's reject pile (Eassom, 2018 ; Elsevier, 2015 ). If editors have to struggle as often as we did to determine the purpose of a paper, this does not bode well for future publication. Clearly, communication through succinctly stated hypotheses is likely to enhance publication success.
Hypotheses also provide crucial direction during study design. Nothing is more frustrating than realizing that your hard‐earned data cannot actually address the key study objectives or rule out alternative explanations. Developing clear hypotheses and, in particular, multiple alternative hypotheses ensures that you actually design your study in a way that can answer the key questions of interest.
Personal Fulfillment
Second, science is more likely to be fulfilling and fun when the direction of research is clear, but perhaps more importantly, when questions are addressed with more than one plausible answer. Results are often disappointing or unfulfilling when the study starts out with a single biological hypothesis in mind (Symes et al., 2015 )—particularly if there is no support for this hypothesis. If multiple alternative hypotheses are well crafted, something interesting and rewarding will result regardless of the outcome. This results in a situation where researchers are much more likely to enjoy the process of science because the stress of wanting a particular end is removed. Subsequently, as Chamberlin ( 1890 ) proposed, “the dangers of parental affection for a favorite theory can be circumvented” which should reduce the risk of creeping bias. In our experience reviewing competitive grant proposals at the U.S. National Science Foundation, it is consistently the case that proposals testing several compelling hypotheses were more likely to be well received—presumably because reviewers are risk‐averse and understand that ultimately finding support for any of the outcomes will pay‐off. Why bet on just one horse when you can bet on them all?
Intrinsic Value to Mechanism
Mechanism seems to have intrinsic value for humans—regardless of the practical application. Humans tend to be interested in acquiring understanding rather than just accumulating facts. As a species, we crave answers to the question “why.” Indeed, it is partly this desire for mechanism that is driving a recent perceived “crisis” in machine learning, with the entire field being referred to as “alchemy” (Hutson, 2018 ); algorithms continue to increase in performance, but the mechanisms for such improvements are often a mystery—even to the researchers themselves. “Because our model predicts well” is the unsatisfying scientific equivalent to a parent answering a child's “why?” with “because that's just the way it is.” This problem is beginning to spawn a new field in artificial intelligence “AI neuroscience” which attempts to get into the “black‐box” of machine‐learning algorithms to understand how and why they are predictive (Voosen, 2017 ).
Even in some of our most applied research, we find that managers and policymakers when confronted with a result (e.g., thinning trees to 70% of initial densities reduced bird diversity) want to know why (e.g., thinning eliminated nesting substrate for 4 species); If the answer to this question is not available, policy is much less likely to change (Sells et al., 2018 ). So, formulating mechanistic hypotheses will not only be more personally satisfying, but we expect it may also be more likely to result in real‐world changes.
You Are More Likely To be Right
In a highly competitive era, it seems that in the quest for high publication rates and funding, researchers lose sight of the original aim of science: To discover a truth about nature that is transferable to other systems. In a recent poll conducted by Nature, more than 70% of researchers have tried and failed to reproduce another scientist's experiments (Baker, 2016 ). Ultimately, each researcher has a choice; put forward multiple explanations for a phenomenon on their own or risk “attachment” to a single hypothesis and run the risk of bias entering their work, rendering it irreproducible, and subsequently being found wrong by a future researcher. Imagine if Lamarck had not championed a single hypothesis for the mechanisms of evolution? Although Lamarck potentially had a vital impact as an early proponent of the idea that biological evolution occurred and proceeded in accordance with natural laws (Stafleu, 1971 ), unfortunately in the modern era he is largely remembered for his pet hypothesis. It may be a stretch to argue that he would have necessarily come up with natural selection, but if he had considered natural selection, the idea would have emerged 50 years earlier, substantially accelerating scientific progress and limiting his infamy as an early evolutionary biologist. An interesting contemporary example is provided by Prof. Amy Cuddy's research focused on “power posing” as a means to succeed. The work featured in one of the most viewed TED talks of all time but rather famously turned out to be irreproducible (Ranehill et al., 2015 ). When asked in a TED interview what she would do differently now, Prof. Cuddy noted that she would include a greater diversity of theory and multiple potential lines of evidence to “shed light on the psychological mechanisms” (Biello, 2017 ).
7. CONCLUSION
We acknowledge that formulating effective hypotheses can feel like a daunting hurdle for ecologists. However, we suggest that initial justifications for absence of hypotheses may often be unfounded. We argue that there are both selfish and altruistic reasons to include multiple alternative mechanistic hypotheses in your research: (1) testing multiple alternative hypotheses simultaneously makes for rapid and powerful progress which is to the benefit of all (Platt, 1964 ), (2) you lessen the chance that confirmation bias will result in you publishing an incorrect but provocative idea, (3) hypotheses provide clarity in design and writing, (4) research using hypotheses is more likely to be published in a high‐impact journal, and (5) you are able to provide satisfying answers to “why?” phenomena occur. However, few current academic metrics appear to reward use of hypotheses. Therefore, we propose that in order to promote hypothesis use we may need to provide additional incentives (Edwards & Roy, 2016 ; Smaldino & McElreath, 2016 ). We suggest editors reward research conducted using principles of sound scientific method and be skeptical of research that smacks of data dredging, post hoc hypothesis development, and single hypotheses. If no hypotheses are stated in a paper and/or the paper is purely descriptive, editors should ask whether the novelty of the system and question warrant this, or if the field would have been better served by a study with mechanistic hypotheses. Eleven of the top 20 ecology journals already indicate a desire for hypotheses in their instructions for authors—with some going as far as indicating “priority will be given” for manuscripts testing clearly stated hypotheses. Although hypotheses are not necessary in all instances, we expect that their continued and increased use will help our disciplines move toward greater understanding, higher reproducibility, better prediction, and more effective management and conservation of nature. We recommend authors, editors, and readers encourage their use (Box 2 ).
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors have no conflicts of interests to declare.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Matthew G. Betts: Conceptualization (lead); data curation (lead); formal analysis (lead); funding acquisition (lead); investigation (lead); methodology (equal); project administration (lead); resources (lead); supervision (lead); visualization (lead); writing‐original draft (lead); writing‐review & editing (lead). Adam S. Hadley: Conceptualization (lead); data curation (lead); funding acquisition (equal); investigation (equal); methodology (lead); project administration (equal); resources (supporting); software (supporting); supervision (lead); validation (lead); visualization (lead); writing‐original draft (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). David W. Frey: Conceptualization (supporting); data curation (supporting); formal analysis (supporting); funding acquisition (supporting); writing‐review & editing (supporting). Sarah J. K. Frey: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Dusty Gannon: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Scott H. Harris: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Hankyu Kim: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Kara Leimberger: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Katie Moriarty: Conceptualization (supporting); Investigation (equal); methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Joseph M. Northrup: Investigation (equal); methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Ben Phalan: Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Josée S. Rousseau: Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Thomas D. Stokely: Investigation (equal); methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Jonathon J. Valente: Investigation (equal); methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal). Urs G. Kormann: Methodology (supporting); resources (equal); writing‐review & editing (supporting). Chris Wolf: Formal analysis (supporting); writing‐review & editing (supporting). Diego Zárrate‐Charry: Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); writing‐review & editing (equal).
ETHICAL APPROVAL
The authors adhered to all standards for the ethical conduct of research.
Supporting information
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Funding from the National Science Foundation (NSF‐DEB‐1457837) to MGB and ASH supported this research. We thank Rob Fletcher, Craig Loehle and anonymous reviewers for thoughtful comments early versions of this manuscript, as well as Joe Nocera and his graduate student group at the University of New Brunswick for constructive comments on the penultimate version of the paper. The authors are also grateful for A. Dream for providing additional resources to enable the completion of this manuscript.
Betts MG, Hadley AS, Frey DW, et al. When are hypotheses useful in ecology and evolution?. Ecol Evol. 2021;11:5762–5776. 10.1002/ece3.7365
Matthew G. Betts and Adam S. Hadley contributed equally to this manuscript.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data for the analysis of hypothesis use in ecology and evolution publications is available at https://figshare.com/articles/dataset/Betts_et_al_2021_When_are_hypotheses_useful_in_ecology_and_evolution_Ecology_and_Evolution/14110289 .
- AAAS (2018). What percentage of submissions does Science accept?. AAAS Science Contributors. Retrieved from http://www.sciencemag.org/site/feature/contribinfo/faq/index.xhtml‐pct_faq [ Google Scholar ]
- Andrén, H. , & Andren, H. (1994). Effects of habitat fragmentation on birds and mammals in landscapes with different proportions of suitable habitat: A review. Oikos, 71, 355–366. 10.2307/3545823 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Ayala, F. J. (2009). Darwin and the scientific method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 10033–10039. 10.1073/pnas.0901404106 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Ayres, M. P. , & Lombardero, M. J. (2017). Forest pests and their management in the Anthropocene. Canadian Journal of Forest Research, 48, 292–301. 10.1139/cjfr-2017-0033 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Baker, M. (2016). 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature, 533, 452–454. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Bates, D. , Mächler, M. , Bolker, B. , & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bates, D. , Maechler, M. , & Bolker, B. (2018). ‘lme4’ Linear mixed‐effects models using S4 classes. R Core Team. Retrieved from https://cran.r‐project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
- Beale, C. M. , Lennon, J. J. , & Gimona, A. (2008). Opening the climate envelope reveals no macroscale associations with climate in European birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 14908–14912. 10.1073/pnas.0803506105 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Betts, M. , Diamond, T. , Forbes, G. J. , Villard, M.‐A. , & Gunn, J. (2006). The importance of spatial autocorrelation, extent and resolution in predicting forest bird occurrence. Ecological Modeling, 191, 197–224. [ Google Scholar ]
- Betts, M. G. , Hadley, A. S. , & Kress, J. (2015). Pollinator recognition in a keystone tropical plant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 3433–3438. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Biello, D. (2017). Inside the debate about power posing: A Q & A with Amy Cuddy. Ideas.TED.com. Retrieved from https://ideas.ted.com/inside‐the‐debate‐about‐power‐posing‐a‐q‐a‐with‐amy‐cuddy/ [ Google Scholar ]
- Briggs, F. , Lakshminarayanan, B. , Neal, L. , Fern, X. Z. , Raich, R. , Hadley, S. J. K. , Hadley, A. S. , & Betts, M. G. (2012). Acoustic classification of multiple simultaneous bird species: A multi‐instance multi‐label approach. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131, 4640–4650. 10.1121/1.4707424 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Brush, S. G. (1974). Should the history of science be rated X? Science, 183, 1164–1172. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Chamberlin, T. C. (1890). The method of multiple working hypotheses. Science, 15, 92–96. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Crisp, M. D. , Trewick, S. A. , & Cook, L. G. (2011). Hypothesis testing in biogeography. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 26, 66–72. 10.1016/j.tree.2010.11.005 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Cutler, D. R. , Edwards, T. C. , Beard, K. H. , Cutler, A. , Hess, K. T. , Gibson, J. , & Lawler, J. J. (2007). Random forests for classification in ecology. Ecology, 88, 2783–2792. 10.1890/07-0539.1 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Eassom, H. (2018). 9 common reasons for rejection. Wiley: Discover the Future of Research. Retrieved from https://hub.wiley.com/community/exchanges/discover/blog/2018/2001/2031/2019‐common‐reasons‐for‐rejection [ Google Scholar ]
- Edwards, M. A. , & Roy, S. (2016). Academic research in the 21st century: Maintaining scientific integrity in a climate of perverse incentives and hypercompetition. Environmental Engineering Science, 34, 51–61. 10.1089/ees.2016.0223 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Egler, F. E. (1986). “Physics envy” in ecology. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America, 67, 233–235. [ Google Scholar ]
- Einstein, A. (1920). Relativity: The special and general theory (78 pp.). Henry Holt and Company. [ Google Scholar ]
- Elith, J. , Graham, C. H. , Anderson, R. P. , Dudík, M. , Ferrier, S. , Guisan, A. , Hijmans, R. J. , Huettmann, F. , Leathwick, J. R. , Lehmann, A. , Li, J. , Lohmann, L. G. , Loiselle, B. A. , Manion, G. , Moritz, C. , Nakamura, M. , Nakazawa, Y. , Overton, J. M. M. , Townsend Peterson, A. , … Zimmermann, N. E. (2006). Novel methods improve prediction of species' distributions from occurrence data. Ecography, 29, 129–151. [ Google Scholar ]
- Elith, J. , Leathwick, J. R. , & Hastie, T. (2008). A working guide to boosted regression trees. Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 802–813. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01390.x [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Elliott, L. P. , & Brook, B. W. (2007). Revisiting Chamberlin: Multiple working hypotheses for the 21st century. BioScience, 57, 608–614. 10.1641/B570708 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Elsevier (2015). 5 ways you can ensure your manuscript avoids the desk reject pile. Elsevier Connect: https://www.elsevier.com/authors‐update/story/publishing‐tips/5‐ways‐you‐can‐ensure‐your‐manuscript‐avoids‐the‐desk‐reject‐pile [ Google Scholar ]
- Fahrig, L. (2003). Effects of habitat fragmentation on biodiversity. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 34, 487–515. [ Google Scholar ]
- Glass, D. J. , & Hall, N. (2008). A brief history of the hypothesis. Cell, 134, 378–381. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.07.033 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Golub, T. (2010). Counterpoint: Data first. Nature, 464, 679. 10.1038/464679a [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Guisan, A. , Tingley, R. , Baumgartner, J. B. , Naujokaitis‐Lewis, I. , Sutcliffe, P. R. , Tulloch, A. I. T. , Regan, T. J. , Brotons, L. , McDonald‐Madden, E. , Mantyka‐Pringle, C. , Martin, T. G. , Rhodes, J. R. , Maggini, R. , Setterfield, S. A. , Elith, J. , Schwartz, M. W. , Wintle, B. A. , Broennimann, O. , Austin, M. , … Buckley, Y. M. (2013). Predicting species distributions for conservation decisions. Ecology Letters, 16, 1424–1435. 10.1111/ele.12189 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hansen, M. C. , Potapov, P. V. , Moore, R. , Hancher, M. , Turubanova, S. A. , Tyukavina, A. , Thau, D. , Stehman, S. V. , Goetz, S. J. , Loveland, T. R. , Kommareddy, A. , Egorov, A. , Chini, L. , Justice, C. O. , & Townshend, J. R. G. (2013). High‐resolution global maps of 21st‐century forest cover change. Science, 342, 850–853. 10.1126/science.1244693 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Houlahan, J. E. , McKinney, S. T. , Anderson, T. M. , & McGill, B. J. (2017). The priority of prediction in ecological understanding. Oikos, 126, 1–7. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hutson, M. (2018). AI researchers allege that machine learning is alchemy. Science Posted in: Technology May 3, 2018. 10.1126/science.aau0577 [ DOI ]
- Illán, J. G. , Thomas, C. D. , Jones, J. A. , Wong, W.‐K. , Shirley, S. M. , & Betts, M. G. (2014). Precipitation and winter temperature predict long‐term range‐scale abundance changes in Western North American birds. Global Change Biology, 20, 3351–3364. 10.1111/gcb.12642 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lack, D. (1954). The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon Press. [ Google Scholar ]
- Landy, F. (1986). Stamp collecting versus science ‐ Validation as hypothesis testing. American Psychologist, 41, 1183–1192. [ Google Scholar ]
- Loehle, C. (1987). Hypothesis testing in ecology: Psychological aspects and the importance of theory maturation. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 62, 397–409. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- May, R. M. (1981). The role of theory in ecology. American Zoologist, 21, 903–910. [ Google Scholar ]
- Mayr, E. (2002). What evolution is. Basic Books. [ Google Scholar ]
- Milner, S. (2018). Newton didn't frame hypotheses. Why should we? Real Clear Science: Posted in: Physics Today April 25, 2018. 10.1063/PT.2016.2013.20180424a [ DOI ]
- Munafò, M. , Nosek, B. , Bishop, D. , Button, K. S. , Chambers, C. D. , du Sert, N. P. , Simonsohn, U. , Wagenmakers, E.‐J. , Ware, J. J. , & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). A manifesto for reproducible science. Nature Human Behavior, 1, 0021. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Myers, N. , Mittermeier, R. A. , Mittermeier, C. G. , da Fonseca, G. A. B. , & Kent, J. (2000). Biodiversity hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature, 403, 853. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- O'Neill, R. V. , Johnson, A. R. , & King, A. W. (1989). A hierarchical framework for the analysis of scale. Landscape Ecology, 3, 193–205. 10.1007/BF00131538 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Opthof, T. , Furstner, F. , van Geer, M. , & Coronel, R. (2000). Regrets or no regrets? No regrets! The fate of rejected manuscripts. Cardiovascular Research, 45, 255–258. 10.1016/S0008-6363(99)00339-9 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Osborne Patrick, E. , Foody Giles, M. , & Suárez‐Seoane, S. (2007). Non‐stationarity and local approaches to modelling the distributions of wildlife. Diversity and Distributions, 13, 313–323. 10.1111/j.1472-4642.2007.00344.x [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Paine, C. E. T. , & Fox, C. W. (2018). The effectiveness of journals as arbiters of scientific impact. Ecology and Evolution, 8, 9666–9685. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Paine, R. T. (1966). Food web complexity and species diversity. American Naturalist, 100, 65–75. 10.1086/282400 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Peters, R. H. (1991). A critique for ecology. Nature. Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
- Pettorelli, N. , Nagendra, H. , Rocchini, D. , Rowcliffe, M. , Williams, R. , Ahumada, J. , de Angelo, C. , Atzberger, C. , Boyd, D. , Buchanan, G. , Chauvenet, A. , Disney, M. , Duncan, C. , Fatoyinbo, T. , Fernandez, N. , Haklay, M. , He, K. , Horning, N. , Kelly, N. , … Wegmann, M. (2017). Remote sensing in ecology and conservation: Three years on. Remote Sensing in Ecology and Conservation, 3, 53–56. [ Google Scholar ]
- Platt, J. R. (1964). Strong inference. Science, 146, 347–353. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. Basic Books. [ Google Scholar ]
- Quinn, J. F. , & Dunham, A. E. (1983). On hypothesis testing in ecology and evolution. The American Naturalist, 122, 602–617. 10.1086/284161 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Ranehill, E. , Dreber, A. , Johannesson, M. , Leiberg, S. , Sul, S. , & Weber, R. A. (2015). Assessing the robustness of power posing: No effect on hormones and risk tolerance in a large sample of men and women. Psychological Science, 26, 653–656. 10.1177/0956797614553946 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Romesburg, H. C. (1981). Wildlife science: Gaining reliable knowledge. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 45, 293–313. 10.2307/3807913 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Rosen, J. (2016). Research protocols: A forest of hypotheses. Nature, 536, 239–241. 10.1038/nj7615-239a [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Schimel, J. (2011). Writing science: How to write papers that get cited and proposals that get funded. Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
- Sells, S. N. , Bassing, S. B. , Barker, K. J. , Forshee, S. C. , Keever, A. C. , Goerz, J. W. , & Mitchell, M. S. (2018). Increased scientific rigor will improve reliability of research and effectiveness of management. Journal of Wildlife Management, 82, 485–494. 10.1002/jwmg.21413 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Smaldino, P. E. , & McElreath, R. (2016). The natural selection of bad science. Royal Society Open Science, 3(9), 160384. 10.1098/rsos.160384 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Stafleu, F. (1971). Lamarck: The birth of biology. Taxon, 20, 397–442. 10.2307/1218244 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Sutherland, W. J. , Spiegelhalter, D. , & Burgman, M. A. (2013). Twenty tips for interpreting scientific claims. Nature, 503, 335–337. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Symes, L. B. , Serrell, N. , & Ayres, M. P. (2015). A pactical guide for mentoring scientific inquiry. The Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America, 96, 352–367. [ Google Scholar ]
- Tewksbury, J. J. , Anderson, J. G. T. , Bakker, J. D. , Billo, T. J. , Dunwiddie, P. W. , Groom, M. J. , Hampton, S. E. , Herman, S. G. , Levey, D. J. , Machnicki, N. J. , del Rio, C. M. , Power, M. E. , Rowell, K. , Salomon, A. K. , Stacey, L. , Trombulak, S. C. , & Wheeler, T. A. (2014). Natural history's place in science and society. BioScience, 64, 300–310. 10.1093/biosci/biu032 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Thompson, J. N. (2005). The geographic mosaic of coevolution. University of Chicago Press. [ Google Scholar ]
- Thornton, A. , & Lee, P. (2000). Publication bias in meta‐analysis: Its causes and consequences. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 53, 207–216. 10.1016/S0895-4356(99)00161-4 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- van Dijk, D. , Manor, O. , & Carey, L. B. (2014). Publication metrics and success on the academic job market. Current Biology, 24, R516–R517. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.04.039 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Voosen, P. (2017). The AI detectives. Science, 357, 22–27. 10.1126/science.357.6346.22 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Weinberg, R. (2010). Point: Hypotheses first. Nature, 464, 678. 10.1038/464678a [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Wilson, K. A. , McBride, M. F. , Bode, M. , & Possingham, H. P. (2006). Prioritizing global conservation efforts. Nature, 440, 337. 10.1038/nature04366 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Wolff, J. O. , & Krebs, C. J. (2008). Hypothesis testing and the scientific method revisited. Acta Zoologica Sinica, 54, 383–386. [ Google Scholar ]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data availability statement.
- View on publisher site
- PDF (2.1 MB)
- Collections
Similar articles
Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.
- Download .nbib .nbib
- Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Add to Collections
Null Hypotheses in Ecology
Cite this chapter.
- Donald R. Strong Jr. 2
261 Accesses
8 Citations
Null hypotheses entertain the possibility that nothing has happened, that a process has not occurred, or that change has not been produced by a cause of interest. Null hypotheses are reference points against which alternatives should be contrasted. They are used not only in statistics but in all sciences. “This hypothesis…is… characteristic of all experimentation” (Fisher 1935). In physics for example, an important null hypothesis of the post-Newtonian era was that time is a variable which is independent of all other factors. Modern physics is based upon the alternative hypothesis that time can be a function of space and relative velocities. Another famous null hypothesis, that the speed of light is independent of its direction, inspired the Michelson-Morley experiments, which failed to disprove it. An example in chemistry is that there is no molecular property unique to life, that any synthesis by protoplasm can be repeated in the test tube. Modern biochemistry has failed to disprove this null hypothesis. But the term null hypothesis sounds odd in reference to much of physics and chemistry. It is not found in textbooks nor is it used frequently in conversation about these disciplines. Though all sciences use null hypotheses in principle, the ‘atomistic’ 1 sciences of physics and chemistry often use them implicitly. In atomistic sciences, fundamental units are simple and quite similar to one another, and effects of phenomena are commonly so distinct that the null state of no effect does not need special recognition.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this chapter
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
- Available as PDF
- Read on any device
- Instant download
- Own it forever
- Available as EPUB and PDF
- Compact, lightweight edition
- Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
- Free shipping worldwide - see info
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Institutional subscriptions
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
Similar content being viewed by others
Not null enough: pseudo-null hypotheses in community ecology and comparative psychology
The use and limitations of null-model-based hypothesis testing
One-Group t-Test for the Mean
Abele, L. G. and K. Walters: 1979, ‘The stability-time hypothesis: reevaluation of the data’, Amer. Natur . 114 , 559.
Article Google Scholar
Abbot, I., Abbott, L. K., and P. R. Grant: 1977, ‘Comparative ecology of the Galapágos ground finches’, Ecol. Monog . 47 , 151–184.
Anderson, S.: 1974, ‘Patterns of faunal evolution’, The Quart. Rev. Biol . 49 , 311–332.
Auerbach, M. A.: 1979, ‘Some real communities are unstable’, Nature 279 , 821–822.
Beddington, J. R., Free, C. A., and J. H. Lawton: 1978, ‘Characteristics of successful natural enemies in models of biological control of insect pests’, Nature 273 , 513–519.
Bowman, R. I.: 1961, ‘Morphological variation and adaptation in the Galapágos finches’, Univ. Calif. Pub. Zool . 59 , 1–302
Google Scholar
Brown, W. H., and E. O. Wilson: 1956, ‘Character displacement’, Syst. Zool . 5 , 48–64.
Connell, J.: 1961, ‘The influence of interspecific competition and other factors on the distribution of the barnacle Chthamalus stellatusEcology 42 , 710–723.
Connor, E. F. and D. S. Simberloff: 1979, ‘The assembly of species communities: chance or competition? Ecology 60 , 1132–1141.
Cook, R. E.: 1969, ‘Variation in species density of North American birds’, Syst. Zool . 18 , 63–84.
Darwin, C.: 1859, The Origin of Species , Murray, London.
Dayton, P. K.: 1973, ‘Two cases of resource partitioning in an intertidal community: making the right predictions for the wrong reasons’, The American Naturalist 107 , 662–670.
Diamond, J. M.: 1975, ‘Assemble rules of species communities’, pp. 342–344 in M. Cody and J. Diamond (eds.), Ecology and Evolution of Communities , Harvard Univ. Press., Cambridge Mass.
Diamond, J. M.: 1978, ‘Niche-shifts and the rediscovery of interspecific competition’, American Scientist 66 , 322–331.
Elton, C. S.: 1946, Journ. Anim. Ecol . 15 , 54–68.
Emlen, J. M.: 1966, ‘The role of time and energy in food preference’, Amer. Natur . 107 , 580–584.
Fenchel, T.: 1975, ‘Character displacement and coexistence in mud snails’, Oecologia 20 , 19–32.
Fisher, R. A.: 1935, The Design of Experiments ii , Oliver and Boyd, London.
Gause, G. F.: 1934, The Struggle for Existence , Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore.
Grant, P. R.: 1966, ‘Ecological compatibility of bird species on islands’, Amer. Natur . 100 , 451–462.
Grant, P. R.: 1972, ‘Convergent and divergent character displacement’, Biol. Journ. Linn. Soc . 4 , 39–68.
Greenwood, J. J. D.: 1968, ‘Coexistence of avian congeners on islands’, Amer. Natur . 102 , 591–592.
Heatwole, H. and R. Levins: 1972, ‘The trophic structure, stability and faunal change during recolonization’, Ecology 53 , 531–534.
Heck, K. L.: 1976, ‘Some critical considerations of the theory of species packing’, Evol. Theory 1 , 247–258.
Heck, K. L., Van Belle, G., and D. Simberloff: 1975, ‘Explicit calculation of the rarefaction diversity measurement and the determination of sufficient sample size’, Ecology 56 , 1451–1461.
Huey, R. B., and E. R. Pianka: 1974, ‘Ecological character displacement in a lizard’, Amer. Zool . 14 , 1127–1136.
Hutchinson, G. E.: 1959, ‘Homage to Santa Rosalia, or why are there so many kinds of animals? Amer. Natur . 93 , 145–159.
Janzen, D. H.: 1966, ‘Coevolution of mutualism between ants and acacias in Central America’, Evolution 20 , 249–275.
Kuhn, T. S.: 1962, The Structure of Scientific Revolutions , 2nd ed., Univ. Chicago Press.
Lack, D.: 1947, The Galapágos Finches , Cambridge Univ. Press.
Lawlor, L. R.: 1978, ‘A comment on randomly constructed model ecosystems’, The American Naturalist 112 , 445–447.
Levin, S. A.: 1974, ‘Dispersion and population interactions’, The Amer. Natur . 108 , 207–228.
MacArthur, R. H. and E. O. Wilson: 1967, The theory of island biogeography , Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, N.J.
MacFadyen, A.: 1975, ‘Some thoughts on the behaviour of ecologists’, Journ. Anim. Ecol ., 351–363.
May, R. M.: 1974, Stability and Complexity in Model Ecosystems , 2nd ed., Princeton Univ. Press. Princeton, N.J.
May, R. M.: 1978, ‘The evolution of ecological systems’, Scientific American 239 , 160–175.
Moreau, R. E.: 1948, ‘Ecological isolation in a rich tropical avifauna’, Journ. Anim. Ecol . 17 , 113–126.
Moreau, R. E.: 1966, The Bird Faunas of Africa and Its Islands , Academic Press, N.Y.
Murdoch, W. W.: 1966, ‘Community structure, population control, and competition — a critique’, The Amer. Natur . 100 , 219–226.
Paine, R. T.: 1966, ‘Food web complexity and species diversity’, Amer. Natur . 100 , 65–75.
Peters, R. H.: 1977, ‘The unpredictable problems of tropho-dynamics’, Env. Biol. Fish . 2 , 97–101.
Pimm, S. L., and J. H. Lawton: 1977, ‘Number of trophic levels in ecological communities’, Nature 268 , 329–331.
Popper, K. R.: 1968, The Logic of Scientific Discovery , Hutchinson, London.
Schoener, T. W.: 1965, ‘The evolution of bill size differences among sympatric congeneric species of birds’, Evolution 19 , 189–213.
Simberloff, D. S.: 1970, ‘The taxonomic diversity of island biotas’, Evolution 24 , 22–47.
Simberloff, D. S.: 1976, ‘Trophic structure determination and equilibrium in an arthropod community’, Ecology 57 , 395–398.
Stearns, S. C.: 1978, ‘Life–history tactics: a review of ideas’, T he Quarterly Review of Biology , 51 , 3–47.
Strong, D. R., Szyska, L. A., and D. S. Simberloff: 1979, Tests of community-wide character displacement against null hypotheses’, Evolution , in press.
Terborgh, J., Faaborg, K. and H. J. Brockmann: 1978, ‘Island colonization by lesser Antillean birds’, The Auk 95 , 59–72.
Williams, C. B.: 1947, ‘The generic relations of species in small ecological communities’, J. Anim. Ecol . 16 : 11–18.
Williams, C. B.: 1951, ‘Intra-generic competition as illustrated by Moreau’s records of East African birds’, Journ. Anim. Ecol . 20 , 246–253.
Williams, C. B.: 1964, Patterns in the Balance of Nature , Academic Press, Lond.
Wilson, E. O.: 1975, Sociobiology , Harvard.
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Department of Biological Sciences, Florida State University, Tallahassee, USA
Donald R. Strong Jr.
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Editor information
Editors and affiliations.
Dept. of Philosophy, University of Helsinki, Finland
Esa Saarinen
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
Copyright information
© 1982 D. Reidel Publishing Company, Dordrecht, Holland
About this chapter
Strong, D.R. (1982). Null Hypotheses in Ecology. In: Saarinen, E. (eds) Conceptual Issues in Ecology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-7796-9_10
Download citation
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-7796-9_10
Publisher Name : Springer, Dordrecht
Print ISBN : 978-90-277-1391-9
Online ISBN : 978-94-009-7796-9
eBook Packages : Springer Book Archive
Share this chapter
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Publish with us
Policies and ethics
- Find a journal
- Track your research
All Subjects
AP Psychology
Study guides for every class, that actually explain what's on your next test, null hypothesis, from class:.
The null hypothesis is a statement that assumes there is no significant relationship or difference between variables being studied. It represents what we expect if there is no effect or relationship in reality.
Related terms
The opposite of the null hypothesis, suggesting that there is a significant relationship or difference between variables.
Type I Error : Rejecting the null hypothesis when it is actually true. It's like convicting an innocent person in court.
Type II Error : Failing to reject the null hypothesis when it is actually false. It's like letting a guilty person go free due to lack of evidence.
" Null Hypothesis " also found in:
Subjects ( 79 ).
- AP Statistics
- Advanced Communication Research Methods
- Advanced Design Strategy and Software
- Advanced quantitative methods
- Analytical Chemistry
- Bayesian Statistics
- Big Data Analytics and Visualization
- Biostatistics
- Business Analytics
- Business Anthropology
- Business Decision Making
- Business Forecasting
- Causal Inference
- College Introductory Statistics
- Communication Research Methods
- Communication in Healthcare
- Computational Chemistry
- Covering Politics
- Data Journalism
- Data Visualization for Business
- Data, Inference, and Decisions
- Documentary Forms
- Engineering Applications of Statistics
- Engineering Probability
- Epidemiology
- Experimental Design
- Financial Mathematics
- Foundations of Data Science
- Fundamentals of Ecology
- Fundamentals of Mechanical Prototyping
- Healthcare Quality and Outcomes
- History of Mathematics
- Honors Statistics
- Intro to Business Statistics
- Introduction to Advanced Programming in R
- Introduction to Biostatistics
- Introduction to Business Analytics
- Introduction to Econometrics
- Introduction to Environmental Science
- Introduction to Mathematical Economics
- Introduction to Political Research
- Introduction to Probabilistic Methods in Mathematics and the Sciences
- Introduction to Probability
- Introduction to Programming in R
- Introduction to Public Health
- Introduction to Time Series
- Introductory Probability and Statistics for Business
- Linear Modeling: Theory and Applications
- Machine Learning Engineering
- Market Research: Tools and Techniques for Data Collection and Analysis
- Marketing Research
- Marketing Strategy
- Mathematical Modeling
- Mathematical Probability Theory
- Mathematical and Computational Methods in Molecular Biology
- Media Effects
- Methods for Public Health Practice
- Methods of Mathematics: Calculus, Statistics, and Combinatorics
- Modern Statistical Prediction and Machine Learning
- Philosophy of Science
- Predictive Analytics in Business
- Preparatory Statistics
- Principles & Techniques of Data Science
- Principles of Finance
- Probabilistic & Statistical Decision-Making for Management
- Probability and Mathematical Statistics in Data Science
- Probability and Statistics
- Professionalism and Research in Nursing
- Public Health Policy and Administration
- Public Policy Analysis
- Radiochemistry
- Reproducible and Collaborative Statistical Data Science
- Risk Management and Insurance
- Sampling Surveys
- Statistical Inference
- Statistical Methods for Data Science
- Structural Health Monitoring
- Theoretical Statistics
- Thinking Like a Mathematician
Practice Questions ( 1 )
- In terms of statistics and probability, what is meant by "null hypothesis"?
© 2024 Fiveable Inc. All rights reserved.
Ap® and sat® are trademarks registered by the college board, which is not affiliated with, and does not endorse this website..
Best Practices in Science
The Null Hypothesis
Show Topics
- Publications
Journals and Blogs
The null hypothesis, as described by Anthony Greenwald in ‘Consequences of Prejudice Against the Null Hypothesis,’ is the hypothesis of no difference between treatment effects or of no association between variables. Unfortunately in academia, the ‘null’ is often associated with ‘insignificant,’ ‘no value,’ or ‘invalid.’ This association is due to the bias against papers that accept the null hypothesis by journals. This prejudice by journals to only accept papers that show ‘significant’ results (aka rejecting this ‘null hypothesis’) puts added pressure on those working in academia, especially with their relevance and salaries often depend on publications. This pressure may also be correlated with increased scientific misconduct, which you can also read more about on this website by clicking here . If you would like to read publication, journal articles, and blogs about the null hypothesis, views on rejecting and accepting the null, and journal bias against the null hypothesis, please see the resources we have linked below.
Most scientific journals are prejudiced against papers that demonstrate support for null hypotheses and are unlikely to publish such papers and articles. This phenomenon leads to selective publishing of papers and ensures that the portion of articles that do get published is unrepresentative of the total research in the field.
Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis
Journal of Negative Results, Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
Journal of Negative Results in BioMedicine
Positively Negative: A New PLOS ONE Collection focusing on Negative, Null and Inconclusive Results
F1000Research
Journal of Interesting Negative Results in Natural Language Processing and Machine Learning
The All Results Journals
Journal of Contradicting Results in Science
Journal of Pharmaceutical Negative Results
Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
- View all journals
- Explore content
- About the journal
- Publish with us
- Sign up for alerts
- Published: 30 October 2024
The oldest tadpole reveals evolutionary stability of the anuran life cycle
- Mariana Chuliver ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-6717-0459 1 ,
- Federico L. Agnolín 1 , 2 ,
- Agustín Scanferla 1 , 3 ,
- Mauro Aranciaga Rolando 2 ,
- Martín D. Ezcurra ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-6000-6450 4 , 5 ,
- Fernando E. Novas ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-6901-8677 2 &
- Xing Xu ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4786-9948 6
Nature ( 2024 ) Cite this article
Metrics details
- Herpetology
- Palaeontology
Anurans are characterized by a biphasic life cycle, with an aquatic larval (tadpole) stage followed by an adult (frog) stage, both connected through the metamorphic period in which drastic morphological and physiological changes occur 1 . Extant tadpoles exhibit great morphological diversity and ecological relevance 2 , but their absence in the pre-Cretaceous fossil record (older than 145 million years) makes their origins and early evolution enigmatic. This contrasts with the postmetamorphic anuran fossil record that dates back to the Early Jurassic and with closely related species in the Late Triassic (around 217–213 million years ago (Ma)) 3 . Here we report a late-stage tadpole of the stem-anuran Notobatrachus degiustoi from the Middle Jurassic of Patagonia (around 168–161 Ma). This finding has dual importance because it represents the oldest-known tadpole and, to our knowledge, the first stem-anuran larva. Its exquisite preservation, including soft tissues, shows features associated with the filter-feeding mechanism characteristic of extant tadpoles 4 , 5 . Notably, both N. degiustoi tadpole and adult reached a large size, demonstrating that tadpole gigantism occurred among stem-anurans. This new discovery reveals that a biphasic life cycle, with filter-feeding tadpoles inhabiting aquatic ephemeral environments, was already present in the early evolutionary history of stem-anurans and has remained stable for at least 161 million years.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
24,99 € / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
185,98 € per year
only 3,65 € per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during the study are included as part of this Article and its Supplementary Information files. The datasets for phylogenetic, disparity and morphometric analyses are available at Figshare ( https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25339195 ) 63 .
Code availability
The data matrix used for phylogenetic analyses is available at Figshare ( https://figshare.com/s/4fc207d07da2b8ff13cd?file=44846395 ) 64 and can be accessed from the project entitled ‘ Notobatrachus tadpole’. The R codes used for phylogenetic, disparity and morphometric analyses are also available at Figshare ( https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25339195 ) 63 .
Handrigan, G. R. & Wassersug, R. J. The anuran Bauplan: a review of the adaptive, developmental, and genetic underpinnings of frog and tadpole morphology. Biol. Rev. 82 , 1–25 (2007).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
McDiarmid, R. W. & Altig, R. Tadpoles: the Biology of Anuran Larvae (Univ. of Chicago Press, 1999).
Stocker, M. R. et al. The earliest equatorial record of frogs from the Late Triassic of Arizona. Biol. Lett. 15 , 20180922 (2019).
Kenny, J. S. Feeding mechanisms in anuran larvae. J. Zool. (1987) 157 , 225–246 (1969).
Article Google Scholar
Wassersug, R. J. & Hoff, K. A comparative study of the buccal pumping mechanism of tadpoles. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 12 , 225–259 (1979).
Schoch, R. R. & Witzmann, F. The evolution of larvae in temnospondyls and the stepwise origin of amphibian metamorphosis. Biol. Rev . https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.13084 (2024).
Wassersug, R. J. The adaptive significance of the tadpole stage with comments on the maintenance of complex life cycles in anurans. Amer. Zool. 15 , 405–417 (1975).
Roelants, K., Haas, A. & Bossuyt, F. Anuran radiations and the evolution of tadpole morphospace. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108 , 8731–8736 (2011).
Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Gardner, J. D. The fossil record of tadpoles. Fossil Imprint 72 , 17–44 (2016).
Reig, O. A. Noticia sobre un nuevo anuro fósil del Jurásico de Santa Cruz (Patagonia). Ameghiniana 2 , 73–78 (1961).
Google Scholar
Shubin, N. H. & Jenkins, F. A. An Early Jurassic jumping frog. Nature 377 , 49–52 (1995).
Article ADS CAS Google Scholar
Báez, A. M. & Basso, N. G. The earliest known frogs of the Jurassic of South America: review and cladistic appraisal of their relationship. Münchner Geowiss. Abh. (A) 30 , 131–158 (1996).
Báez, A. M. & Nicoli, L. A new look at an old frog: the Jurassic Notobatrachus Reig from Patagonia. Ameghiniana 41 , 257–270 (2004).
Gao, K. Q. & Chen, S. A new frog (Amphibia: Anura) from the Lower Cretaceous of western Liaoning, China. Cretac. Res. 25 , 761–769 (2004).
Stipanicic, P. N. & Reig, O. A. El “Complejo porfírico de la Patagonia extraandina” y su fauna de anuros. Acta Geol. Lilloana 1 , 185–298 (1957).
De Barrio, R. E., Panza, J. L. & Nullo, F. E. in Geología Argentina (ed. Caminos, R.) 511–527 (Instituto de Geología y Recursos Minerales, 1999).
Rees, P. M. et al . in Warm Climates in Earth History (eds Huber, B. T. et al.) 297–318 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2000).
Gallego, O. F. Conchóstracos (Cyzicidae) del Jurásico de Santa Cruz y Chubut, Argentina. Ameghiniana 30 , 333–345 (1994).
Zamuner, A. B. & Falaschi, P. Agathoxylon matildense n. sp., leño araucariaceo del Bosque Petrificado del cerro Madre e Hija, Formación La Matilde (Jurásico medio), provincia de Santa Cruz, Argentina. Ameghiniana 42 , 339–346 (2005).
Gnaedinger, S. Podocarpaceae woods (Coniferales) from Middle Jurassic La Matilde formation, Santa Cruz Province, Argentina. Rev. Palaeobot. Palyno. 147 , 77–93 (2007).
Trueb, L. in The Skull, Vol. 2. Patterns of Structural and Systematic Diversity (eds Hanken, J. & Hall, B. K.) 255–343 (Univ. of Chicago Press, 1993).
Ročková, H. & Roček, Z. Development of the pelvis and posterior part of the vertebral column in the Anura. J. Anat. 206 , 17–35 (2005).
Havelková, P. & Roček, Z. Transformation of the pectoral girdle in the evolutionary origin of frogs: insights from the primitive anuran Discoglossus . J. Anat. 209 , 1–11 (2006).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Gosner, K. L. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16 , 183–190 (1960).
Gao, K.-Q. & Wang, Y. Mesozoic anurans from Liaoning Province, China, and phylogenetic relationships of archaeobatrachian anuran clades. J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 21 , 460–476 (2001).
Marjanović, D. & Laurin, M. An updated paleontological timetree of lissamphibians, with comments on the anatomy of Jurassic crown-group salamanders (Urodela). Hist. Biol. 26 , 535–550 (2014).
Sherratt, E., Vidal-García, M., Anstis, M. & Keogh, J. S. Adult frogs and tadpoles have different macroevolutionary patterns across the Australian continent. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1 , 1385–1391 (2017).
Wollenberg Valero, K. et al. Transcriptomic and macroevolutionary evidence for phenotypic uncoupling between frog life history phases. Nat. Commun. 8 , 15213 (2017).
Wassersug, R. J. The mechanism of ultraplanktonic entrapment in anuran larvae. J. Morphol. 137 , 279–288 (1972).
Haas, A. The larval hyobranchial apparatus of discoglossoid frogs: its structure and bearing on the systematics of the Anura (Amphibia: Anura). J. Zool. Syst. Evol. Res. 35 , 179–197 (1997).
Cannatella, D. in Tadpoles: the Biology of Anuran Larvae (eds McDiarmid, R. W. & Altig, R.) 52–91 (Univ. of Chicago Press, 1999).
Emerson, S. The giant tadpole of Pseudis paradoxa . Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 34 , 93–104 (1988).
Fabrezi, M., Quinzio, S. I. & Goldberg, J. The giant tadpole and delayed metamorphosis of Pseudis platensis Gallardo, 1961 (Anura: Hylidae). J. Herpetol. 43 , 228–243 (2009).
Womack, M. C. & Bell, R. C. Two-hundred million years of anuran body-size evolution in relation to geography, ecology and life history. J. Evolution. Biol. 33 , 1417–1432 (2020).
Roček, Z., Böttcher, R. & Wassersug, R. J. Gigantism in the tadpoles of the Neogene frog Paleobatrachus . Paleobiology 32 , 666–675 (2006).
Fabrezi, M. Heterochrony in growth and development in anurans from the Chaco of South America. Evol. Biol. 38 , 390–411 (2011).
Skelly, D. K. Tadpole communities. Am. Sci. 85 , 36–45 (1997).
ADS Google Scholar
Orton, G. L. The systematics of vertebrate larvae. Syst. Zool . 2 , 63–75 (1953).
Duellman, W. E. & Trueb, L. Biology of Amphibians (McGraw-Hill, 1986).
Hanken, J. in The Origin and Evolution of Larval Forms (eds Hall, B. K. & Wake, M. H.) 61–108 (Academic, 1999).
Trueb, L. in Evolutionary Biology of the Anurans, Contemporary Research on Major Problems (ed. Vial, J. L.) 65–132 (Univ. of Missouri Press, 1973).
Pugener, L. A. & Maglia, A. M. Developmental evolution of the anuran sacro-urostylic complex. S. Am. J. Herpetol. 4 , 193–209 (2009).
Wassersug, R. J. Oral morphology of anuran larvae: terminology and general description. Occas. Pap. Mus. Nat. Hist. Univ. Kansas 48 , 1–23 (1976).
Schlosser, G. & Roth, G. Distribution of cranial and rostral spinal nerves in tadpoles of the frog Discoglossus pictus (Discoglossidae). J. Morphol. 226 , 189–212 (1995).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Haas, A. Phylogeny of frogs as inferred from primarily larval characters (Amphibia: Anura). Cladistics 19 , 23–89 (2003).
PubMed Google Scholar
Ronquist, F. et al. MrBayes 3.2: efficient Bayesian phylogenetic inference and model choice across a large model space. Syst. Biol. 61 , 539–542 (2012).
Báez, A. M. Anurans from the Early Cretaceous Lagerstätte of Las Hoyas, Spain: new evidence on the Mesozoic diversification of crown-clade Anura. Cretac. Res. 41 , 90–106 (2013).
Article ADS Google Scholar
Carvalho, I. S. et al. A new genus of pipimorph frog (Anura) from the early Cretaceous Crato formation (Aptian) and the evolution of South American tongueless frogs. J. S. Am. Earth Sci . 92 , 222–233 (2019).
Henrici, A. C., Báez, A. M. & Grande, L. Aerugoamnis paulus , new genus and new species (Anura: Anomocoela): first reported Anuran from the Early Eocene (Wasatchian) fossil Butte member of the Green River formation, Wyoming. Ann. Carnegie Mus. 81 , 295–309 (2013).
Lemierre, A., Folie, A., Bailon, S., Robin, N. & Laurin, M. From toad to frog, a CT-based reconsideration of Bufo servatus , an Eocene anuran mummy from Quercy (France). J. Vertebr. Paleontol . https://hal.science/hal-03501090 (2021).
Benton, M. J. et al. Constraints on the timescale of animal evolutionary history. Palaeontol. Electron. 18.1.1FC , 1–106 (2015).
Rambaut, A., Drummond, A. J., Xie, D., Baele, G. & Suchard, M. A. Posterior summarization in Bayesian phylogenetics using Tracer 1.7. Syst. Biol. 67 , 901–904 (2018).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Jetz, W. & Pyron, R. A. The interplay of past diversification and evolutionary isolation with present imperilment across the amphibian tree of life. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 21 , 850–858 (2018).
Ezcurra, M. D. Exploring the effects of weighting against homoplasy in genealogies of palaeontological phylogenetic matrices. Cladistics 40 , 242–281 (2024).
Goloboff, P. & Morales, M. TNT version 1.6, with a graphical interface for MacOs and Linux, including new routines in parallel. Cladistics 39 , 144–153 (2023).
Lloyd, G. T. Estimating morphological diversity and tempo with discrete character-taxon matrices: implementation, challenges, progress, and future directions. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 118 , 131–151 (2016).
Ezcurra, M. D. & Butler, R. J. The rise of the ruling reptiles and ecosystem recovery from the Permo-Triassic mass extinction. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 285 , 20180361 (2018).
Flannery Sutherland, J. T., Moon, B. C., Stubbs, T. L. & Benton, M. J. Does exceptional preservation distort our view of disparity in the fossil record? Proc. Biol. Sci. 286 , 20190091 (2019).
PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Lehmann, O. E., Ezcurra, M. D., Butler, R. J. & Lloyd, G. T. Biases with the Generalized Euclidean Distance measure in disparity analyses with high levels of missing data. Palaeontology 62 , 837–849 (2019).
Revell, L. J. Phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3 , 217–223 (2012).
Akima, H., Gebhardt, A., Petzold, T. & Maechler, M. akima: Interpolation of irregularly and regularly spaced data. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/akima/index.html (2021).
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2023).
Chuliver, M. Datasets used for phylogenetic analyses of the manuscript entitled ‘The oldest tadpole reveals evolutionary stability of the anuran life cycle’. Figshare https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25339195.v1 (2024).
Chuliver, M. Data matrix used for phylogenetic analyses of the manuscript entitled ‘The oldest tadpole reveals evolutionary stability of the anuran life cycle’. Figshare https://figshare.com/s/4fc207d07da2b8ff13cd (2024).
Download references
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by Agencia Nacional de Promoción Científica y Técnica (no. PICT 2020-02443) to A.S., and by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant No. 42288201) and Yunnan Revitalization Talent Support Program (no. 202305AB350006) to X.X. We also thank N. Vega (LAHN – CNEA) for microcomputed tomography facilities, V. D’Acurso (CITEDEF) for technical assistance, A. Haas (LIB) for providing files of phylogenetic matrices, and M. Isasi (MACN – CONICET) for technical preparation of the fossil specimen. We thank A. Di Federico and M. A. Queizán (Advanced Machine Systems) for 3D scanning, high-resolution tomography (Zeiss Metrotom), and post-processing of images. This study used computational resources from Universidad Nacional de Córdoba ( https://ccad.unc.edu.ar/ ), which are part of SNCAD – MinCyT, Argentina. We also thank the Willi Hennig Society for supporting the free use of TNT software. Special thanks go to the team that excavated at the Estancia La Matilde locality in 2018.
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Fundación de Historia Natural “Félix de Azara”, Centro de Ciencias Naturales, Ambientales y Antropológicas, Universidad Maimónides, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Mariana Chuliver, Federico L. Agnolín & Agustín Scanferla
Laboratorio de Anatomía Comparada y Evolución de los Vertebrados, CONICET–Museo Argentino de Ciencias Naturales “Bernardino Rivadavia”, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Federico L. Agnolín, Mauro Aranciaga Rolando & Fernando E. Novas
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Agustín Scanferla
Sección Paleontología de Vertebrados, CONICET–Museo Argentino de Ciencias Naturales “Bernardino Rivadavia”, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Martín D. Ezcurra
School of Geography, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK
Key Laboratory of Vertebrate Evolution and Human Origins, Institute of Vertebrate Paleontology and Paleoanthropology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Contributions
M.C., F.L.A. and A.S. contributed equally to project conceptualization, analysis and writing of the original draft. M.C. and A.S. scored phylogenetic matrices, gathered analytic data and performed statistical analyses. M.A.R. conducted specimen curation and created the figures with input from M.C. and A.S. M.D.E. conducted methodological development and phylogenetic and morphological disparity analyses and their visualization. F.L.A., F.E.N. and X.X. contributed material, field trip logistics and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to writing the manuscript, discussion and conclusions.
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Mariana Chuliver .
Ethics declarations
Competing interests.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information.
Nature thanks Liping Dong, Nadia Fröbisch, James Gardner and Zbynĕk Roček for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data figures and tables
Extended data fig. 1 geographic location and fossils found at the la matilde locality..
a , b , Geographic location of the Estancia La Matilde locality (type locality of Notobatrachus degiustoi ), in Santa Cruz Province, Argentina. c , d , Several specimens of the branchiopod Eosolimnadiopsis? santacrucensis and plant debris frequently found in the same levels as N. degiustoi specimens in the Estancia La Matilde locality. Abbreviations: Ea, Estancia.
Extended Data Fig. 2 Specimen in high resolution.
High-resolution photograph of MPM-PV 23540 taken under long exposure time and sub-vertical artificial white light.
Extended Data Fig. 3 Specimen in high resolution.
High-resolution photograph of MPM-PV 23540 taken under short exposure time and low-angle artificial white light coming from the upper left corner.
Extended Data Fig. 4 Photographs of the latex cast of MPM-PV 23540 taken under low-angle light.
a , Cast of the complete specimen showing the preserved vertebral elements; b , close-up of the skull and first vertebrae; c , close-up of the last vertebrae and appendicular elements. Abbreviations: cl, cleithrum; cn, chondrified neurocranium, f, femur; fo, fenestra ovalis; fp, frontoparietal; il, ilium; n, nasal; ps, parasphenoid; I–IX, presacral vertebrae; X, sacrum; 1–3 postsacral neural arches.
Extended Data Fig. 5 Majority rule consensus tree recovered from the unconstrained Bayesian phylogenetic analysis depicting the position of the Notobatrachus degiustoi tadpole.
Numbers at nodes indicate posterior probabilities and dotted grey vertical lines indicate the boundaries between the Permian and Triassic and Cretaceous and Palaeogene geological periods, respectively.
Extended Data Fig. 6 Majority rule consensus tree recovered from the constrained Bayesian phylogenetic analysis using a molecular backbone, depicting the position of the Notobatrachus degiustoi tadpole.
Extended data fig. 7 global strict consensus tree recovered from the unconstrained parsimony phylogenetic analysis depicting the position of the notobatrachus degiustoi tadpole..
Numbers above branches indicate absolute (left) and GC (group present/contradicted) (right) no-zero weight symmetric resampling frequencies (values between squared brackets are negative).
Extended Data Fig. 8 Global strict consensus tree recovered from the constrained parsimony phylogenetic analysis using a molecular backbone, depicting the position of the Notobatrachus degiustoi tadpole.
Numbers above branches indicate absolute (left) and GC (group present/contradicted) (right) no-zero weight symmetric resampling frequencies.
Extended Data Fig. 9 Gigantism in anurans.
a , Plot depicting the total length of tadpoles across 187 species with the inclusion of the Notobatrachus degiustoi tadpole. The red line denotes the mean of the surveyed values (50.94 mm), whereas the blue line denotes twice the mean (101.8 mm). Labelled species are those with gigantic tadpoles. b , Optimization of the residuals of the linear regression between the tadpole total length and adult snout-vent length in the Bayesian tip-dating tree (using mean ages), showing the independent acquisition of gigantism in Notobatrachus degiustoi and some crown-anuran species.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information.
This file contains Supplementary data, methods and references.
Reporting Summary
Peer review file, supplementary table 1.
Measurements of adults of the described Mesozoic anuran species.
Supplementary Table 2
Measurements of tadpoles and adults of anuran species, and measurements of larva and adults of extant urodeles species used as an outgroup.
Supplementary Table 3
Body length and total length of extant tadpoles (full references for ‘Source’ column are provided in Supplementary Table 2).
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Cite this article.
Chuliver, M., Agnolín, F.L., Scanferla, A. et al. The oldest tadpole reveals evolutionary stability of the anuran life cycle. Nature (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-08055-y
Download citation
Received : 05 March 2024
Accepted : 16 September 2024
Published : 30 October 2024
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-08055-y
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
Quick links
- Explore articles by subject
- Guide to authors
- Editorial policies
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Null Hypothesis Examples. "Hyperactivity is unrelated to eating sugar " is an example of a null hypothesis. If the hypothesis is tested and found to be false, using statistics, then a connection between hyperactivity and sugar ingestion may be indicated.
Null hypothesis, often denoted as H0, is a foundational concept in statistical hypothesis testing. ... Environmental Science: Null Hypothesis: "There's no substantial difference in pollution levels before and after a water treatment plant's establishment." Principle of Null Hypothesis. ... Alternative Hypothesis: Definition, Types and Examples.
Null hypothesis In Intro to Environmental Science. Definition. The null hypothesis is a statement in statistical hypothesis testing that asserts there is no effect or no difference between groups or variables being studied. It serves as a default position that researchers aim to test against, helping to determine if any observed effect is ...
Biology definition: A null hypothesis is an assumption or proposition where an observed difference between two samples of a statistical population is purely accidental and not due to systematic causes. It is the hypothesis to be investigated through statistical hypothesis testing so that when refuted indicates that the alternative hypothesis is true. . Thus, a null hypothesis is a hypothesis ...
An example of the null hypothesis is that light color has no effect on plant growth. The null hypothesis (H 0) is the hypothesis that states there is no statistical difference between two sample sets. In other words, it assumes the independent variable does not have an effect on the dependent variable in a scientific experiment.
Null Hypothesis Definition. The null hypothesis, often denoted as H 0, is the hypothesis in a statistical test which proposes no statistical significance exists in a set of observed data. ... Environmental Science: Null hypotheses are used to understand environmental changes. For instance, researchers might form a null hypothesis stating there ...
The null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis are types of conjectures used in statistical tests to make statistical inferences, which are formal methods of reaching conclusions and separating scientific claims from statistical noise.. The statement being tested in a test of statistical significance is called the null hypothesis. The test of significance is designed to assess the strength ...
The null hypothesis is a prediction that states there is no significant difference between variables or treatments, or the relationship is random. The alternative hypothesis is a prediction that ...
The null hypothesis (H0) answers "No, there's no effect in the population.". The alternative hypothesis (Ha) answers "Yes, there is an effect in the population.". The null and alternative are always claims about the population. That's because the goal of hypothesis testing is to make inferences about a population based on a sample.
The null hypothesis is a statement in statistical testing that suggests there is no significant effect or relationship between variables. It serves as a baseline or default position that assumes any observed differences are due to random chance rather than a specific cause. Understanding this concept is essential for designing experiments and interpreting results in ecological studies.
When your sample contains sufficient evidence, you can reject the null and conclude that the effect is statistically significant. Statisticians often denote the null hypothesis as H 0 or H A.. Null Hypothesis H 0: No effect exists in the population.; Alternative Hypothesis H A: The effect exists in the population.; In every study or experiment, researchers assess an effect or relationship.
Research hypotheses have been a cornerstone of science since before Galileo. Many have argued that hypotheses (1) encourage discovery of mechanisms, and (2) reduce bias—both features that should increase transferability and reproducibility. However, we are entering a new era of big data and highly predictive models where some argue the ...
Definition. In formal hypothesis testing, the null hypothesis (H0) is the hypothesis assumed to be true in the population and which gives rise to the sampling distribution of the test statistic in question (Hays 1994). The critical feature of the null hypothesis across hypothesis testing frameworks is that it is stated with enough precision ...
Description. Applications of Hypothesis Testing for Environmental Science presents the theory and application of hypothesis testing in environmental science, allowing researchers to carry out suitable tests for decision-making on a variety of issues. This book works as a step-by-step resource to provide understanding of the concepts and ...
Null hypotheses entertain the possibility that nothing has happened, that a process has not occurred, or that change has not been produced by a cause of interest. Null hypotheses are reference points against which alternatives should be contrasted. They are used not only in statistics but in all sciences.
Definition. The null hypothesis is a statement that assumes there is no significant relationship or difference between variables being studied. It represents what we expect if there is no effect or relationship in reality. ... Introduction to Environmental Science; Introduction to Mathematical Economics; Introduction to Political Research;
The null hypothesis, as described by Anthony Greenwald in 'Consequences of Prejudice Against the Null Hypothesis,' is the hypothesis of no difference between treatment effects or of no association between variables. Unfortunately in academia, the 'null' is often associated with 'insignificant,' 'no value,' or 'invalid.'.
The null hypothesis, as described by Anthony Greenwald in 'Consequences of Prejudice Against the Null Hypothesis,' is the hypothesis of no difference between treatment effects or of no association between variables. Unfortunately in academia, the 'null' is often associated with 'insignificant,' 'no value,' or 'invalid.'.
The motivational and verificationist realities of normal science have fundamental implications for how the "null hypothesis" is interpreted, and whether or not it is "accepted." In a normal-science setting, multiple explanatory paradigms may offer competing accounts of demonstrated effects and differing predictions for what may be ...
The null hypothesis is rejected. Student environmental awareness deficiency vs. the inclusion of projects dealing with environmental sustainability: A significant positive correlation was found (t = 3.577, p < .001), indicating that the inclusion of environmental sustainability projects in the curriculum is associated with higher environmental ...
Hypothesis: An explanation for an observed phenomenon. Research Hypothesis: A statement about a phenomenon that also includes the potential mechanism or cause of that phenomenon. Though a research hypothesis doesn't need to adhere to this strict framework it is often best described as the "if" in an "if-then" statement.
This depends on ecological factors such as environmental ... distributed under the null hypothesis that the distribution was normal. Because we found no agreement for the definition of a gigantic ...