thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  • français
  •   BAHÁNDÌAN Home
  • College of Nursing
  • Master's Special Papers
  • Master in Nursing

Alcohol use in the family and other factors, their relationship to alcohol consumption among the junior and senior student nurses of the College of St. John-Roxas

Thumbnail

Thesis Adviser

Defense panel chair, defense panel member, share , description, suggested citation, shelf location, physical description, collections.

  • Master in Nursing [37]

Related items

Showing items related by title, author, creator and subject.

Thumbnail

Psychosocial factors associated with alcohol use among high school students in a public school in Iloilo City 

Thumbnail

Critical analysis of Republic Act No. 10351 also known as “Sin Tax Reform Law of 2012”, an act restructuring the excise tax on alcohol and tobacco products and its proposed amendments and revisions. 

Thumbnail

Factors associated with degree of alcoholism in the households of Barangay East Valencia, Buenavista, Guimaras 

CPU Henry Luce III Library

EXTERNAL LINKS DISCLAIMER

This link is being provided as a convenience and for informational purposes only. Central Philippine University bears no responsibility for the accuracy, legality or content of the external site or for that of subsequent links. Contact the external site for answers to questions regarding its content.

If you come across any external links that don't work, we would be grateful if you could report them to the repository administrators .

Click DOWNLOAD to open/view the file. Chat Bertha to inform us in case the link we provided don't work.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

Theoretical Frameworks and Mechanistic Aspects of Alcohol Addiction: Alcohol Addiction as a Reward Deficit Disorder

George f. koob.

Committee on the Neurobiology of Addictive Disorders, The Scripps Research Institute, 10550 North Torrey Pines Road, SP30-2400, La Jolla, CA 92037, USA

Alcoholism can be defined by a compulsion to seek and take drug, loss of control in limiting intake, and the emergence of a negative emotional state when access to the drug is prevented. Alcoholism impacts multiple motivational mechanisms and can be conceptualized as a disorder that includes a progression from impulsivity (positive reinforcement) to compulsivity (negative reinforcement). The compulsive drug seeking associated with alcoholism can be derived from multiple neuroadaptations, but the thesis argued here is that a key component involves the construct of negative reinforcement. Negative reinforcement is defined as drug taking that alleviates a negative emotional state. The negative emotional state that drives such negative reinforcement is hypothesized to derive from dysregulation of specific neurochemical elements involved in reward and stress within the basal forebrain structures involving the ventral striatum and extended amygdala, respectively. Specific neurochemical elements in these structures include not only decreases in reward neurotransmission, such as decreased dopamine and γ -aminobutyric acid function in the ventral striatum, but also recruitment of brain stress systems, such as corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), in the extended amygdala. Acute withdrawal from chronic alcohol, sufficient to produce dependence, increases reward thresholds, increases anxiety-like responses, decreases dopamine system function, and increases extracellular levels of CRF in the central nucleus of the amygdala. CRF receptor antagonists also block excessive drug intake produced by dependence. A brain stress response system is hypothesized to be activated by acute excessive drug intake, to be sensitized during repeated withdrawal, to persist into protracted abstinence, and to contribute to the compulsivity of alcoholism. Other components of brain stress systems in the extended amygdala that interact with CRF and that may contribute to the negative motivational state of withdrawal include norepinephrine, dynorphin, and neuropeptide Y. The combination of loss of reward function and recruitment of brain stress systems provides a powerful neurochemical basis for a negative emotional state that is responsible for the negative reinforcement driving, at least partially, the compulsivity of alcoholism.

1 Definitions and Conceptual Framework for Reward Deficit in Alcoholism

Alcoholism has many definitions that vary from social frameworks to a psychiatric framework embedded in the diagnosis of Substance Dependence on Alcohol defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association, 4th edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association 1994 ). Alcoholism, and more generically drug addiction, can be defined as a chronically relapsing disorder characterized by (i) compulsion to seek and take the drug (alcohol), (ii) loss of control in limiting (alcohol) intake, and (iii) emergence of a negative emotional state (e.g., dysphoria, anxiety and irritability) reflecting a motivational withdrawal syndrome when access to the drug (alcohol) is prevented (defined here as dependence: Koob and Le Moal 1997 ). Clinically and in animal models, the occasional but limited use of alcohol with the potential for abuse or dependence is distinct from escalated alcohol intake and the emergence of a chronic alcohol-dependent state. The thesis argued in the present synthesis is that alcoholism, similar to drug addiction, is a reward deficit disorder, and the “emergence of a negative emotional state” plays an important role in defining and perpetuating alcoholism. Alcoholism also involves substantial neuroadaptations that persist beyond acute withdrawal and trigger relapse and deficits in cognitive function that can also fuel compulsive drinking. However, the argument here is that the core deficit that sets up vulnerability to relapse in alcoholism, and possibly even deficits in cognitive function, is in fact decreased reward function.

To support this hypothesis, a holistic view of alcoholism will be presented with the following arguments. A negative emotional state is a common presentation in most alcoholics during withdrawal and protracted abstinence. Compulsivity observed in alcoholism has an important negative reinforcement component that perpetuates alcoholism. Such negative emotional states become sensitized over time and set up an allostatic state that perpetuates dependence. Negative emotional states set up a powerful motivational state for relapse. Finally, the neurobiological substrates underlying the motivation to seek alcohol will be reviewed, and an argument will be presented that it is loss of reward function and gain of brain stress function that mediate the negative emotional state outlined as key to alcoholism.

Drug addiction has generally been conceptualized as a disorder that involves elements of both impulsivity and compulsivity, in which impulsivity can be defined behaviorally as “a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal and external stimuli without regard for the negative consequences of these reactions to themselves or others” ( Moeller et al. 2001 ). Impulsivity is measured in two domains: the choice of a smaller, immediate reward over a larger, delayed reward ( Rachlin and Green 1972 ) or the inability to inhibit behavior by changing the course of action or to stop a response once it is initiated ( Logan et al. 1997 ). Impulsivity is a core deficit in substance abuse disorders ( Allen et al. 1998 ) and neuropsychiatric disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Operationally, delay-to-gratification tasks (delayed discounting tasks; impulsive choice) and the stop-signal or go/no-go task (behavioral impulsivity) have been used as measures of impulsivity ( Fillmore and Rush 2002 ; Green et al. 1994 ). Compulsivity can be defined as elements of behavior that result in perseveration of responding in the face of adverse consequences or perseveration in the face of incorrect responses in choice situations (e.g., operationally, responding for a drug or alcohol in the face of adverse consequences ( Wolffgramm and Heyne 1995 ) or responding for a drug or alcohol on a progressive-ratio schedule of reinforcement ( Walker et al. 2008 )). Compulsivity is analogous to the symptoms of Substance Dependence outlined by the American Psychiatric Association: continued substance use despite knowledge of having had a persistent or recurrent physical or psychological problem and a great deal of time spent in activities necessary to obtain the substance ( American Psychiatric Association 2000 ).

Collapsing the cycles of impulsivity and compulsivity yields a composite addiction cycle comprising three stages— preoccupation/anticipation, binge/intoxication, and withdrawal/negative affect —in which impulsivity often dominates at the early stages and compulsivity dominates at terminal stages ( Fig. 1 ). As an individual moves from impulsivity to compulsivity, a shift occurs from positive reinforcement driving the motivated behavior to negative reinforcement driving the motivated behavior ( Koob 2004 ). Negative reinforcement can be defined as the process by which removal of an aversive stimulus (e.g., negative emotional state of drug withdrawal) increases the probability of a response (e.g., dependence-induced drug intake to relieve the negative emotional state). Note that negative reinforcement is not punishment, although both involve an aversive stimulus. In punishment, the aversive stimulus suppresses behavior, including drug taking (e.g., disulfiram [Antabuse]). Negative reinforcement can be perhaps described in lay terms as reward via relief (i.e., relief reward), such as removal of pain or in the case of alcoholism removal of the negative emotional state of acute withdrawal or protracted abstinence. The three stages are conceptualized as interacting with each other, becoming more intense, and ultimately leading to the pathological state known as addiction ( Koob and Le Moal 1997 ) ( Fig. 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms403706f1.jpg

(Top left) Diagram showing the stages of impulse control disorder and compulsive disorder cycles related to the sources of reinforcement. In impulse control disorders, an increasing tension and arousal occurs before the impulsive act, with pleasure, gratification, or relief during the act. Following the act, there may or may not be regret or guilt. In compulsive disorders, there are recurrent and persistent thoughts (obsessions) that cause marked anxiety and stress followed by repetitive behaviors (compulsions) that are aimed at preventing or reducing distress ( American Psychiatric Association 1994 ). Positive reinforcement (pleasure/gratification) is more closely associated with impulse control disorders. Negative reinforcement (relief of anxiety or relief of stress) is more closely associated with compulsive disorders. (Top right) Collapsing the cycles of impulsivity and compulsivity results in the addiction cycle, conceptualized as three major components: preoccupation/anticipation, binge/intoxication, and withdrawal/negative affect (Taken with permission from Koob 2008b .) (Bottom) Change in the relative contribution of positive and negative reinforcement constructs during the development of substance dependence on alcohol

In alcohol addiction, or alcoholism, a pattern of oral drug taking evolves that is often characterized by binges of alcohol intake that can be daily episodes or prolonged days of heavy drinking and is characterized by a severe emotional and somatic withdrawal syndrome. Many alcoholics continue with such a binge/withdrawal pattern for extended periods of time, but some individuals can evolve into an opioid-like situation in which they must have alcohol available at all times to avoid the negative consequences of abstinence. Here, intense preoccupation with obtaining alcohol (craving) develops that is linked not only to stimuli associated with obtaining the drug but also to stimuli associated with withdrawal and the aversive motivational state. A pattern develops in which the drug must be obtained to avoid the severe dysphoria and discomfort of abstinence.

The pattern of alcohol addiction, related to reward dysfunction, can be amply illustrated by excerpts from two case histories from Knapp (1996) and Goodwin (1981) . In the first representative case history, an individual progresses from the state where they stated, “I drank when I was happy and I drank when I was anxious and I drank when I was bored and I drank when I was depressed, which was often,” to, “I loved the way drink made me feel, and I loved its special power of deflection, its ability to shift my focus away from my own awareness of self and onto something else, something less painful than my own feelings,” and, “There’s a sense of deep need, and the response is a grabbiness, a compulsion to latch on to something outside yourself in order to assuage some deep discomfort” ( Knapp 1996 ). Similarly, in a second representative case history, “Alcohol seemed to satisfy some specific need I had, which I can’t describe,” and, “There were always reasons to drink. I was low, tense, tired, mad, happy,” and, “The goal, always, was to maintain a glow, not enough, I hoped, that people would notice, but a glow,” and, “By now I was hooked and knew it, but desperately did not want others to know it. I had been sneaking drinks for years—slipping out to the kitchen during parties and such—but now I began hiding alcohol, in my desk, bedroom, car glove compartment, so it would never be far away, ever. I grew panicky even thinking I might not have alcohol when I needed it, which was just about always, “ and, “ I loathed myself. I was waking early and thinking what a mess I was, how I had hurt so many others and myself. The words ‘guilty’ and ‘depression’ sound superficial in trying to describe how I felt. The loathing was almost physical—a dead weight that could be lifted in only one way, and that was by having a drink” ( Goodwin 1981 ; see Koob and Le Moal 2006 , Appendix, for full quotations).

These case histories illustrate numerous key points regarding the present treatise, but the main point to be further discussed below is the transition from drinking to feel good to drinking to avoid feeling bad. To some extent, this transition is facilitated by personality differences, presumably shaped not only by genetics but also by developmental and even social factors. As Khantzian (1997) cogently argued, addiction can be considered a type of chronic emotional distress syndrome that varies with the individual from physical and emotional pain to chronic dysphoria to stress and anxiety to interpersonal difficulties for which drugs can be argued to be sources of self-medication for such negative emotional states. Additionally, he argued that self-medication may be drug-specific—patients may have a preferential use of drugs that fits with the nature of the painful feeling states that they are self-medicating (e.g., opiates to counter intense anger and rage, stimulants as augmenting agents for high-energy individuals, energizing agents for low-energy individuals, and depressants [e.g., alcohol] for individuals who are tense and anxious). The common element argued by Khantzian is that each class of drugs serves as antidotes or correctives to dysphoric states and acts as a “replacement for a defect in the psychological structure” ( Kohut 1971 , p. 46) of such individuals ( Khantzian 2003 ).

1.1 Theoretical Framework: Motivation, Withdrawal, and Opponent Process

Motivation is a state that can be defined as a “tendency of the whole animal to produce organized activity” ( Hebb 1972 ), and such motivational states are not constant but rather vary over time. Early work by Wikler stressed the role of changes in drive states associated with dependence. Subjects described changes in withdrawal as a “hunger” or primary need and the effects of morphine on such a state as “satiation” or gratification of the primary need ( Wikler 1952 ). Although Wikler argued that positive reinforcement was retained even in heavily dependent subjects (thrill of the intravenous opioid injection), dependence produced a new source of gratification, that of negative reinforcement (see above).

The concept of motivation in addiction was inextricably linked with hedonic, affective, or emotional states in the context of temporal dynamics by Solomon’s opponent process theory of motivation. Solomon and Corbit (1974) postulated that hedonic, affective, or emotional states, once initiated by drugs, are automatically modulated by the central nervous system with mechanisms that reduce the intensity of hedonic feelings. The a-process includes affective or hedonic habituation (or tolerance), and the b-process includes affective or hedonic withdrawal (abstinence). The a-process in drug use consists of positive hedonic responses, occurs shortly after presentation of a stimulus, correlates closely with the intensity, quality, and duration of the reinforcer, and shows tolerance. In contrast, the b-process in drug use appears after the a-process has terminated, consists of negative hedonic responses, and is sluggish in onset, slow to build up to an asymptote, slow to decay, and gets larger with repeated exposure. The thesis here is that opponent processes begin early in drug taking, reflect changes in the brain reward and stress systems, and later form one of the major motivations for compulsivity in drug taking.

Thus, dependence or manifestation of a withdrawal syndrome after removal of chronic drug administration is defined in terms of motivational aspects of dependence, such as emergence of a negative emotional state (e.g., dysphoria, anxiety and irritability) when access to the drug is prevented ( Koob and Le Moal 2001 ), rather than on the physical , signs of dependence. Indeed, some have argued that the development of such a negative affective state can define dependence as it relates to addiction:

The notion of dependence on a drug, object, role, activity or any other stimulus-source requires the crucial feature of negative affect experienced in its absence. The degree of dependence can be equated with the amount of this negative affect, which may range from mild discomfort to extreme distress, or it may be equated with the amount of difficulty or effort required to do without the drug, object, etc ( Russell 1976 ).

Alcoholics show dramatic evidence of dysphoric states during acute withdrawal that persist into protracted abstinence. Alcohol withdrawal in humans produces well documented physical (somatic) symptoms, such as tremor, autonomic hyperactivity, nausea, vomiting, and seizures, but more importantly produces significant affective symptoms of anxiety, dysphoria, and depression-like symptoms. Acute withdrawal (i.e., the first week post-alcohol) is characterized by Beck Depression Inventory scores of approximately 20, which is categorized within the range of moderate depression ( Potokar et al. 21997 ; 15–30), and Hamilton Depression Scores of 18, which is close to 20 (the cutoff for antidepressant medication in affective disorder; Brown and Schuckit 1988 ). Depression scores decline during subsequent weeks of treatment but remain at close to 10 for Hamilton Depression Scores for up to 4 weeks of an inpatient treatment program ( Brown and Schuckit 1988 ). In another study of inpatient alcoholics during withdrawal, the Beck Depression Inventory score was at 15 at withdrawal and remained at 12.8 two days into withdrawal and at 9.4 two weeks post-withdrawal ( de Timary et al. 2008 ). Similar results were obtained for anxiety measures ( Potokar et al. 1997 ; de Timary et al. 2008 ). In another study with a long-term follow-up of 6 months after a 4-week inpatient detoxification. Beck Depression Inventory scores remained at approximately 6, and trait anxiety scores (STAI-X2) remained above 33 even in subjects without comorbid anxiety or depression ( Driessen et al. 2001 ). Independent of comorbidity status, individuals who relapsed had higher trait anxiety scores than those who abstained ( Driessen et al. 2001 ). Thus, although alcoholics show significant decreases in measures of depression and anxiety during withdrawal, there is a measurable level of depression-like symptoms that persist long after acute withdrawal into protracted abstinence that may be clinically (treatment) relevant.

More compelling for the present thesis, during a 2-week inpatient withdrawal study, alexithymia (defined as a state of deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing emotions: from the Greek a for “lack.” lexis for “word,” and thymos for “emotion”; Sifneos 1973 ; Taylor and Bagby 2000 ), which results in poor emotional regulation and stress management abilities, remained high and stable during the 2-week period ( de Timary et al. 2008 ). Alexithymia scores did not decline between the 0 and 2 day time-points but remained high at a score of 57 and declined only to 53 at the 3-week time-point ( de Timary et al. 2008 ). The authors argued that alexithymia is a stable personality trait in alcoholics rather than a state-dependent phenomenon, providing support for the self-medication hypothesis outlined above.

Animal models can also be used to test the hypothesis that there are opponent process-like motivational changes associated with the development of alcohol dependence. Electrical brain stimulation reward or intracranial self-stimulation has a long history as a measure of activity of the brain reward system and of the acute reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse. All drugs of abuse, when administered acutely, decrease brain stimulation reward thresholds ( Kornetsky and Esposito 1979 ) and when administered chronically increase reward thresholds during withdrawal (see above). Brain stimulation reward involves widespread neurocircuitry in the brain, but the most sensitive sites defined by the lowest thresholds involve the trajectory of the medial forebrain bundle that connects the ventral tegmental area with the basal forebrain ( Olds and Milner 1954 ; Koob et al. 1977 ). Although much emphasis was focussed initially on the role of the ascending monoamine systems in the medial forebrain bundle in brain stimulation reward, other nondopaminergic systems in the medial forebrain bundle clearly play a key role ( Hernandez et al. 2006 ).

Rats made dependent using chronic ethanol vapor exposure at blood alcohol levels sufficient to drive excessive drinking showed an increase in brain reward thresholds during withdrawal that lasted up to 3 days post-withdrawal ( Schulteis et al. 1995 ). However, data suggest that, similar to other drugs of abuse, such opponent-like processes can begin with a single dosing ( Fig. 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms403706f2.jpg

a Withdrawal from a single boul of acute ethanol intoxication (week 1) resulted in a significant but transient increase in brain reward threshold only with the highest dose of ethanol tested (2.0 g/kg: a P < 0.05, compared with vehicle controls at given time-point post-injection). The effect was significant at 6 hours, a time when blood alcohol levels had declined to virtually undetectable levels following this dose of ethanol. Repeated treatment with this dose for two additional weeks resulted in a progressive broadening of the duration of significant threshold elevations. By comparison, treatment with 1.5 g/kg ethanol resulted in significant but transient elevations only after three repeated bouts of intoxication/withdrawal, and no statistically reliable changes were seen after one or two treatments (data not shown). Treatment with 1.0 g/kg did not produce any statistically reliable threshold changes regardless of treatment week (data not shown). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM percentage of baseline threshold, n = 8–10 per dose group. [Taken with permission from Schulteis and Liu 2006 .] b Time-dependent elevation of intracranial self-stimulation thresholds during ethanol withdrawal. Mean blood alcohol levels were 197.29 mg%, Data are expressed as mean ± SEM percentage of baseline threshold, a p < 0.05, thresholds that were significantly elevated above control levels at 2–48 hours post-ethanol. Open circles indicate the control condition. Closed circles indicate the ethanol withdrawal condition. [Taken with permission from Schulteis et al. 1995 .]

An acute elevation in brain reward thresholds was observed during repeated acute withdrawal from ethanol, bearing a striking resemblance to human subjective reports ( Schulteis and Liu 2006 ) ( Fig. 2 ). These results demonstrate that the elevation in brain reward thresholds following prolonged access to alcohol may fail to return to baseline levels between repeated and prolonged exposure to alcohol self-administration (i.e., a residual reward deficit), thus creating the greater elevation in reward thresholds observed during withdrawal from chronic ethanol. Rapid acute tolerance and opponent process-like effects in response to the hedonic effects of alcohol have been reported in human studies using the alcohol clamp procedure ( Morzorati et al. 2002 ). These data provide compelling evidence for brain reward dysfunction with chronic alcohol, which provides strong support for a hedonic allostasis model of alcoholism ( Koob 2003 ).

The dysregulation of brain reward function associated with withdrawal from chronic administration of drugs of abuse is a common element of all drugs of abuse. Withdrawal from chronic cocaine ( Markou and Koob 1991 ), amphetamine ( Paterson et al. 2000 ), opioids ( Schulteis et al. 1994 ), cannabinoids ( Gardner and Vorel 1998 ), nicotine ( Epping-Jordan et al. 1998 ), and ethanol ( Schulteis et al. 1995 ) leads to increases in reward thresholds during acute abstinence, and some of these elevations in threshold can last for up to 1 week. These observations lend credence to the hypothesis that opponent processes can set the stage for one aspect of compulsivity in which negative reinforcement mechanisms are engaged.

More recently, the opponent process theory has been expanded into the domains of the neurobiology of drug addiction from a neurocircuitry perspective. An allostatic model of the brain motivational systems has been proposed to explain the persistent changes in motivation that are associated with dependence in addiction ( Koob and Le Moal 2001 , 2008 ). In this formulation, addiction is conceptualized as a cycle of increasing dysregulation of brain reward/anti-reward mechanisms that results in a negative emotional state contributing to the compulsive use of drugs. Counteradaptive processes that are part of the normal homeostatic limitation of reward function fail to return within the normal homeostatic range. These counteradaptive processes are hypothesized to be mediated by two mechanisms: within-system neuroadaptations and between-system neuroadaptations ( Koob and Bloom 1988 ).

In a within-system neuroadaptation. “the primary cellular response element to the drug would itself adapt to neutralize the drug’s effects: persistence of the opposing effects after the drug disappears would produce the withdrawal response” ( Koob and Bloom 1988 ). Thus, a within-system neuroadaptation is a molecular or cellular change within a given reward circuit to accommodate overactivity of hedonic processing associated with addiction resulting in a decrease in reward function.

The emotional dysregulation associated with the withdrawal/negative affect stage may also involve between-system neuroadaptations in which neurochemical systems other than those involved in the positive rewarding effects of drugs of abuse are recruited or dysregulated by chronic activation of the reward system. “In the between-systems opposing process, a different cellular system and separable molecular apparatus would be triggered by the changes in the primary drug response neurons and would produce the adaptation and tolerance” ( Koob and Bloom 1988 ). Thus, a between-system neuroadaptation is a circuitry change in which another different circuit (anti-reward circuit) is activated by the reward circuit and has opposing actions, again limiting reward function. The remainder of this review explores the neuroadaptational changes that occur in the brain emotional systems to account for the neurocircuitry changes that produce opponent processes and are hypothesized to play a key role in the compulsivity of addiction.

2 Animal Models for Compulsive Alcohol Seeking

Methods of inducing binge-like drinking with alcohol range from having animals drink alcohol solutions that are made more palatable with the addition of a sweetener ( Ji et al. 2008 ) to restricting intake to specific periods of the dark cycle (drinking in the dark; Rhodes et al. 2005 ) to models involving alcohol dependence in animals such as alcohol vapor inhalation, intragastric alcohol infusion, and alcohol-liquid diet. The compulsive use of alcohol derives from multiple sources of reinforcement, and animal models have been developed not only for the acute positive reinforcing effects of ethanol, but also for the negative reinforcing effects associated with removal of the aversive effects of ethanol withdrawal or an existing aversive state (i.e., self-medication of the aversive effects of abstinence from chronic ethanol or self-medication of a pre-existing negative affective state; Koob and Le Moal 1997 ). A major early breakthrough was the development of a training procedure involving access to a sweetened solution and a subsequent fading in of ethanol to avoid the aversiveness of the ethanol taste (for review, see Samson 1987 ). Subsequent work extended these procedures to measures of self-administration in dependent rats and post-dependent rats ( Roberts et al. 1996 ; O’Dell et al. 2004 ).

High doses of alcohol solution will be self-administered intragastrically after animals are made dependent via passive intragastric infusion, and rats will self-infuse 4–7 g/kg per day of ethanol ( Fidler et al. 2006 ). Here, blood alcohol levels average 0.12 g%, measured 30 min after the start of a bout in which rats infuse 1.5 g/kg per 30 min.

In an alcohol-liquid diet procedure, the diet is typically the sole source of calories available to rats (for example, see Moy et al. 1997 ), thereby forcing rats to consume the alcohol. Typically, rats are provided a palatable liquid diet containing 5–8.7% v/v ethanol as their sole source of calories sufficient to produce dependence and maintain blood alcohol levels of 100–130 mg% during the dark (active drinking) cycle ( Schulteis et al. 1996 ; Brown et al. 1998 ; Valdez et al. 2004). High responders during withdrawal from liquid diet will reach blood alcohol levels of approximately 80–100 mg% ( Schulteis et al. 1996 ; Gilpin et al. 2009 ).

Reliable self-administration of ethanol in dependent animals using ethanol vapor exposure has been extensively characterized in rats, in which animals obtain blood alcohol levels in the 100–150 mg% range ( Roberts et al. 1999 , 2000 ). Similarly, rats with a history of alcohol dependence show increased self-administration of ethanol, even weeks after acute withdrawal ( Roberts et al. 2000 ). In a variant of alcohol vapor exposure with more face validity, intermittent exposure to chronic ethanol using alcohol vapor chambers (14 h on/10 h off) produces more rapid escalation to increased ethanol intake and higher amounts of intake ( O’Dell et al. 2004 ; Rimondini et al. 2002 ), and blood alcohol levels are reliably above 140 mg% after a 30 min session of self-administration in dependent animals ( Richardson et al. 2008 ). In both the liquid diet and ethanol vapor procedures, alcohol intake is directly related to the blood alcohol range and the pattern of intermittent high-dose alcohol exposure ( Gilpin et al. 2009 ). Although the alcohol vapor model may have limited face validity, considering that alcohol is passively administered to animals, numerous studies demonstrated that it also has robust predictive validity for alcohol addiction ( Heilig and Koob 2007 ; Koob et al. 2009 ).

A similar procedure has been developed for mice and produces reliable increases in ethanol self-administration during withdrawal. Now termed chronic intermittent exposure (CIE), C57BL/6 mice are exposed to intermittent durations of ethanol vapor (three cycles of 16 h of vapor and 8 h of air) and then tested in a 2 h limited access ethanol preference drinking test during the circadian dark period ( Becker and Lopez 2004 ; Lopez and Becker 2005 ; Finn et al. 2007 ). Intermittent ethanol vapor exposure significantly increased 15% (v/v) ethanol intake by 30–50% in the post-vapor period, usually after multiple cycles and usually after 24 h of withdrawal ( Finn et al. 2007 ). Similar results have been reported using an operant response in mice in 60 min test sessions for 10% (w/v) ethanol with intermittent vapor exposure of 14 h on/10 h off ( Chu et al. 2007 ).

3 Neural Substrates for the Negative Emotional State Associated with Alcoholism

3.1 within-system neuroadaptations that contribute to the compulsivity associated with the dark side of alcoholism.

Within-system neuroadaptations to chronic drug exposure include decreases in function of the same neurotransmitter systems in the same neurocircuits implicated in the acute reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse. One prominent hypothesis is that dopamine systems are compromised in crucial phases of the addiction cycle, such as withdrawal and protracted abstinence. This decrease in dopamine function is hypothesized to lead to decreased motivation for non-drug-related stimuli and increased sensitivity to the abused drug ( Melis et al. 2005 ). Activation of the mesolimbic dopamine system has long been known to be critical for the acute rewarding properties of psychostimulant drugs and to be associated with the acute reinforcing effects of alcohol ( Koob 1992 ; McBride and Li 1998 ; Nestler 2005 ). However, the magnitude of the increase in dopaminergic activity produced by alcohol pales in comparison to that of psychostimulant “intoxication.” For example, intravenous cocaine self-administration produces a 200% increase in extracellular dopamine ( Weiss et al. 1992b ) compared with ethanol which produces a 20% increase in extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens ( Doyon et al. 2003 ) and heroin (which does not increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens) ( Table 1 ). Such a relationship changes with the development of dependence and may change with genetic background (see Ramachandani et al. 2010 , who demonstrated a nearly 200% increase with alcohol in animals that carried the OPRM1 118G variant).

Effects of intravenous self-administration of D-amphetamine, cocaine, and heroin and oral self-administration of alcohol on extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens using in vivo microdialysis

More compelling in the mesolimbic dopamine domain are the decreases in activity of the mesolimbic dopamine system and decreases in serotonergic neurotransmission in the nucleus accumbens that occur during alcohol withdrawal in animal studies ( Rossetti et al. 1992 ; Weiss et al. 1992a , 1996 ). In dependent male Wistar rats trained to self-administer ethanol during withdrawal, the release of dopamine and serotonin was monitored by microdialysis in the nucleus accumbens at the end of a 3–5 week ethanol (8.7% w/v) liquid diet regimen, during 8 h of withdrawal, and during renewed availability of ethanol involving the opportunity to operantly self-administer ethanol (10% w/v) for 60 min, followed by unlimited access to the ethanol liquid diet. In nondependent rats, operant ethanol self-administration increased both dopamine and serotonin release in the nucleus accumbens. Withdrawal from the chronic ethanol diet produced a progressive suppression in the release of these transmitters over the 8 h withdrawal period. Self-administration of ethanol reinstated and maintained dopamine release at pre-withdrawal levels but failed to completely restore serotonin efflux. These findings suggested that deficits in nucleus accumbens monoamine release may contribute to the negative affective consequences of ethanol withdrawal and thereby motivate ethanol-seeking behavior in dependent subjects ( Weiss et al. 1996 ). Similar dramatic decreases in extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens, measured by microdialysis, were found in a study in which animals were tested for 8 h into ethanol withdrawal produced by chronic repeated ethanol injections of up to 5 g/kg every 6 h for six consecutive days using the Majchrowicz procedure ( Majchrowicz 1975 ; Rossetti et al. 1999 ). Thus, as a result, ethanol-dependent animals may show a much greater percentage increase in dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens during ethanol self-administration during withdrawal because baseline levels of dopamine are so low during withdrawal ( Weiss et al. 1996 ).

Imaging studies in drug-addicted humans have consistently shown long-lasting decreases in the numbers of dopamine D 2 receptors in alcoholics compared with controls ( Volkow et al. 2002 ). Additionally, alcohol-dependent subjects had dramatically reduced dopamine release in the striatum response to a pharmacological challenge with the stimulant drug methylphenidate ( Volkow et al. 2007 ). Decreases in the number of dopamine D 2 receptors, coupled with the decrease in dopaminergic activity, in cocaine, nicotine, and alcohol abusers are hypothesized to produce a decreased sensitivity of reward circuits to stimulation by natural rein-forcers ( Martin-Solch et al. 2001 ; Volkow and Fowler 2000 ). These findings suggest an overall reduction in the sensitivity of the dopamine component of reward circuitry to natural reinforcers and other drugs in drug-addicted individuals ( Table 2 ).

Role of corticotropin-releasing factor in dependence

nt, not tested

CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala

Other within-system neuroadaptations under this conceptual framework could include increased sensitivity of receptor transduction mechanisms in the nucleus accumbens. Drugs of abuse have acute receptor actions that are linked to intracellular signaling pathways that may undergo adaptations with chronic treatment. In the context of chronic alcohol administration, multiple molecular mechanisms have been hypothesized to counteract the acute effects of ethanol that could be considered within-system neuroadaptations. For example, chronic ethanol decreases γ -aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptor function, possibly through downregulation of the α 1 subunit ( Mhatre et al. 1993 ; Devaud et al. 1997 ). Chronic ethanol also decreases the acute inhibition of adenosine reuptake (i.e., tolerance develops to the inhibition of adenosine by ethanol; Sapru et al. 1994 ). Perhaps more relevant to the present treatise, whereas acute ethanol activates adenylate cyclase, withdrawal from chronic ethanol decreases CREB phosphorylation in the amygdala and is linked to decrease in function of neuropeptide Y (NPY) and to the anxiety-like responses observed during acute ethanol withdrawal ( Chance et al. 2000 ; Pandey 2004 ).

3.2 Between-System Neuroadaptations that Contribute to Compulsivity Associated with the Dark Side of Alcoholism

Brain neurochemical systems involved in arousal-stress modulation may also be engaged within the neurocircuitry of the brain stress systems in an attempt to overcome the chronic presence of the perturbing drug (alcohol) and to restore normal function despite the presence of drug. The neuroanatomical entity termed the extended amygdala ( Heimer and Alheid 1991 ) may represent a common anatomical substrate integrating brain arousal-stress systems with hedonic processing systems to produce some of the between-system opponent process elaborated above. The extended amygdala is composed of the central nucleus of the amygdala, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and a transition zone in the medial (shell) subregion of the nucleus accumbens. Each of these regions has cytoarchitectural and circuitry similarities ( Heimer and Alheid 1991 ). The extended amygdala receives numerous afferents from limbic structures, such as the basolateral amygdala and hippocampus, and sends efferents to the medial part of the ventral pallidum and a large projection to the lateral hypothalamus, thus further defining the specific brain areas that interface classical limbic (emotional) structures with the extrapyramidal motor system ( Alheid et al. 1995 ). The extended amygdala has long been hypothesized to play a key role not only in fear conditioning ( Le Doux 2000 ) but also in the emotional component of pain processing ( Neugebauer et al. 2004 ).

The brain stress system mediated by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) systems in both the extended amygdala and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis are dysregulated by chronic administration of all major drugs with dependence or abuse potential, with a common response of elevated adrenocorticotropic hormone, corticosterone, and extended amygdala CRF during acute withdrawal from chronic drug administration ( Rivier et al. 1984 ; Merlo-Pich et al. 1995 ; Koob et al 1994 ; Rasmussen et al. 2000 ; Olive et al. 2002 ; Delfs et al. 2000 ; Koob 2008a ).

More specifically, alcohol withdrawal reliably produces anxiety-like responses in animal models that can be reversed by CRF receptor antagonists ( Koob 2008a ). Ethanol withdrawal produces anxiety-like behavior that is reversed by intracerebroventricular administration of CRF 1 /CRF 2 peptidergic antagonists ( Baldwin et al. 1991 ), small-molecule CRF 1 antagonists ( Knapp et al. 2004 ; Overstreet et al. 2004 ; Funk et al. 2007 ), and intracerebral administration of a peptidergic CRF 1 /CRF 2 antagonist into the amygdala ( Rassnick et al. 1993 ). CRF antagonists injected intracerebroventricularly or systemically also block the potentiated anxiety-like responses to stressors observed during protracted abstinence from chronic ethanol ( Breese et al. 2005 ; Valdez et al. 2003 ; Sommer et al. 2008 ).

Perhaps more relevant to the present thesis are studies showing that intermittent alcohol exposure sensitizes withdrawal of anxiety-like responses and that administration of drug treatments during withdrawal from the first and second alcohol cycles blocked this sensitization of withdrawal ( Knapp et al. 2004 ). Diazepam, flumazenil (a GABA A receptor partial agonist), and baclofen (a GABA B receptor agonist) blocked the sensitization of withdrawal, consistent with a within-system neuroadaptation ( Knapp et al. 2004 , 2005 , 2007 ; see above). However, a CRF 1 , antagonist also prevented the sensitization of withdrawal-induced anxiety ( Overstreet et al. 2004a . 2005 ). These results are consistent with a prolonged history of alcohol exposure producing persistent upregulation of both CRF and CRF 1 , receptors in the brain ( Roberto et al. 2010 ; Sommer et al. 2008 ; Zorrilla et al. 2001 ).

The ability of CRF antagonists to block the anxiogenic-like and aversive-like motivational effects of drug withdrawal would predict motivational effects of CRF antagonists in animal models of extended access to drugs. A particularly dramatic example of the motivational effects of CRF in dependence can be observed in animal models of ethanol self-administration in dependent animals. During ethanol withdrawal, extrahypothalamic CRF systems become hyperactive, with an increase in extracellular CRF within the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in dependent rats ( Funk et al. 2006 ; Merlo-Pich et al. 1995 ; Olive et al. 2002 ). The dysregulation of brain CRF systems is hypothesized to underlie not only the enhanced anxiety-like behaviors but also the enhanced ethanol self-administration associated with ethanol withdrawal. Supporting this hypothesis, the subtype nonselective CRF receptor antagonists α -helical CRF 9–41 and D-Phe CRF 12–41 (intracerebroventricular administration) reduced ethanol self-administration in dependent animals during acute withdrawal and during protracted abstinence ( Valdez et al. 2002 ). When administered directly into the central nucleus of the amygdala, a CRF 1 /CRF 2 antagonist blocked ethanol self-administration in ethanol-dependent rats ( Funk et al. 2006 ). Systemic injections of small-molecule CRF 1 , antagonists also blocked the increased ethanol intake associated with acute withdrawal and protracted abstinence ( Gehlert et al. 2007 ; Funk et al. 2007 ). These data suggest an important role for CRF, primarily within the central nucleus of the amygdala, in mediating the increased self-administration associated with dependence. Consistent with the sensitization of the withdrawal response associated with repeated alcohol exposure, a CRF antagonist administered during repeated withdrawal also blocked the development of excessive drinking during withdrawal ( Roberto et al. 2010 ).

Although less well developed, evidence supports a role of norepinephrine systems in the extended amygdala in the negative motivational state and increased self-administration associated with dependence. Substantial evidence has accumulated suggesting that in animals and humans, central noradrenergic systems are activated during acute withdrawal from ethanol. Alcohol withdrawal in humans is associated with activation of noradrenergic function, and the signs and symptoms of alcohol withdrawal in humans are blocked by postsynaptic β -adrenergic blockade ( Romach and Sellers 1991 ). Alcohol withdrawal signs are also blocked in animals by administration of α 1 antagonists and β -adrenergic antagonists and selective blockade of norepinephrine synthesis ( Trzaskowska and Kostowski 1983 ). In dependent rats, the α 1 , antagonist prazosin selectively blocked the increased drinking associated with acute withdrawal ( Walker et al. 2008 ). Thus, converging data suggest that noradrenergic neurotransmission is enhanced during ethanol withdrawal and that noradrenergic functional antagonists can block aspects of ethanol withdrawal.

Dynorphin, an opioid peptide that binds to κ opioid receptors, has long been known to show activation with chronic administration of psychostimulants and opioids ( Nestler 2004 ; Koob 2008a ), and κ opioid receptor agonists produce aversive effects in animals and humans ( Mucha and Herz 1985 ; Pfeiffer et al. 1986 ). Although κ agonists suppress nondependent drinking, possibly via aversive stimulus effects ( Wee and Koob 2010 ), κ opioid antagonists block the excessive drinking associated with ethanol withdrawal and dependence ( Holter et al. 2000 ; Walker and Koob 2008 ). Recently, some have argued that the effects of CRF in producing negative emotional states are mediated by activation of κ opioid systems ( Land et al. 2008 ). However, κ receptor activation can activate CRF systems in the spinal cord ( Song and Takemori 1992 ), and there is pharmacological evidence that dynorphin systems can also activate the CRF system. A CRF 1 , antagonist blocked κ agonist-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in squirrel monkeys ( Valdez et al. 2007 ).

The dynamic nature of the brain stress system response to challenge is illustrated by the pronounced interaction of central nervous system CRF systems and central nervous system norepinephrine systems. Conceptualized as a feed-forward system at multiple levels of the pons and basal forebrain. CRF activates norepinephrine, and norepinephrine in turn activates CRF ( Koob 1999 ). Much pharmacologic, physiologic, and anatomic evidence supports an important role for a CRF-norepinephrine interaction in the region of the locus coeruleus in response to stressors ( Valentino et al. 1991 , 1993 ; Van Bockstaele et al. 1998 ). However, norepinephrine also stimulates CRF release in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus ( Alonso et al. 1986 ), bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and central nucleus of the amygdala. Such feed-forward systems were further hypothesized to have powerful functional significance for mobilizing an organism’s response to environmental challenge, but such a mechanism may be particularly vulnerable to pathology ( Koob 1999 ).

Neuropeptide Y is a neuropeptide with dramatic anxiolytic-like properties localized to the amygdala and has been hypothesized to have effects opposite to CRF in the negative motivational state of withdrawal from drugs of abuse ( Heilig and Koob 2007 ). Significant evidence suggests that activation of NPY in the central nucleus of the amygdala can block the motivational aspects of dependence associated with chronic ethanol administration. Neuropeptide Y administered intracerebroventricularly blocked the increased drug intake associated with ethanol dependence ( Thorsell et al. 2005a , b ). Injection of NPY directly into the central nucleus of the amygdala ( Gilpin et al. 2008 ) and viral vector-enhanced expression of NPY in the central nucleus of the amygdala also blocked the increased drug intake associated with ethanol dependence ( Thorsell et al. 2007 ).

Thus, acute withdrawal from drugs increases CRF in the central nucleus of the amygdala, which has motivational significance for the anxiety-like effects of acute withdrawal from alcohol and the increased drug intake associated with dependence. Acute withdrawal may also increase the release of norepinephrine in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and dynorphin in the nucleus accumbens, both of which may contribute to the negative emotional state associated with dependence. Decreased activity of NPY in the central nucleus of the amygdala may contribute to the anxiety-like state associated with ethanol dependence. Activation of brain stress systems (CRF, norepinephrine and dynorphin) combined with inactivation of brain anti-stress systems (NPY) elicits powerful emotional dysregulation in the extended amygdala. Such dysregulation of emotional processing may be a significant contribution to the between-system opponent processes that help maintain dependence and also set the stage for more prolonged state changes in emotionality such as in protracted abstinence.

4 Compulsivity in Alcoholism: an Allostatic View

Compulsivity in alcoholism can derive from multiple sources, including enhanced incentive salience, engagement of habit function, and impairment in executive function. However, underlying each of these sources is a negative emotional state that may strongly impact on compulsivity. The development of the negative emotional state that drives the negative reinforcement of addiction has been defined as the “dark side” of addiction ( Koob and Le Moal 2005 , 2008 ) and is hypothesized to be the b-process of the hedonic dynamic known as opponent process when the a-process is euphoria. The negative emotional state that comprises the withdrawal/negative affect stage consists of key motivational elements, such as chronic irritability, emotional pain, malaise, dysphoria, alexithymia, and loss of motivation for natural rewards, and is characterized in animals by increase in reward thresholds during withdrawal from all major drugs of abuse. Two processes are hypothesized to form the neurobiological basis for the b-process: loss of function in the reward systems (within-system neuroadaplation) and recruitment of the brain stress or anti-reward systems (between-system neuroadaptation; Koob and Bloom 1988 ; Koob and Le Moal 1997 ). Anti-reward is a construct based on the hypothesis that brain systems are in place to limit reward ( Koob and Le Moal 2008 ). As dependence and withdrawal develop, brain stress systems, such as CRF, norepinephrine, and dynorphin, are recruited, producing aversive or stress-like states ( Koob 2003 ; Nestler 2001 ; Aston-Jones et al. 1999 ). At the same time, within the motivational circuits of the ventral striatum-extended amygdala, reward function decreases. The combination of decreases in reward neurotransmitter function and recruitment of anti-reward systems provides a powerful source of negative reinforcement that contributes to compulsive drug-seeking behavior and addiction ( Fig. 3 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms403706f3.jpg

Pathways for key elements of addiction circuitry implicated in negative emotional states. Addiction circuitry is composed of structures involved in the three stages of the addiction cycle: binge/intoxication (ventral striatum, dorsal striatum and thalamus), withdrawal/negative affect (ventral striatum, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and central nucleus of the amygdala), preoccupation/anticipation (prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and hippocampus). Highlighted here for the withdrawal/negative affect stage is increased activity in the extended amygdala and decreased activity in the reward system, illustrated with the use of imaging colors (i.e., red for high activity and blue for low activity). Modified with permission from Blackburn-Munro and Blackburn-Munro (2003) and Koob et al. (2008) . AMG, amygdala; BNST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; DS, dorsal striatum; GP, globus pallidus; Hippo, hippocampus; Hyp, hypothalamus; Insula, insular cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; Thal, thalamus; VS, ventral striatum; and VTA, ventral tegmental area. [Modified with permission from Zald and Kim 2001 ]

An overall conceptual theme argued here is that drug addiction represents a break with homeostatic brain regulatory mechanisms that regulate the emotional state of the animal. The dysregulation of emotion begins with the binge and subsequent acute withdrawal, but leaves a residual neuroadaptive trace that allows rapid “re-addiction” even months and years after detoxification and abstinence. Thus, the emotional dysregulation of alcohol addiction represents more than simply a homeostatic dysregulation of hedonic function—it also represents a dynamic break with homeostasis of this system that has been termed allostasis ( Koob 2003 ).

Allostasis, originally conceptualized to explain persistent morbidity of arousal and autonomic function, can be defined simply as “stability through change” ( Sterling and Eyer 1988 ). Allostasis is different from homeostasis. Allostasis involves a feed-forward mechanism rather than the negative feedback mechanisms of homeostasis. Allostasis involves a changed set point with continuous re-evaluation of need and continuous readjustment of all parameters toward new set points. The set point in question here is emotional state. An allostatic state can be defined as a state of chronic deviation of the reward system from its normal (homeostatic) operating level. Allostatic load has been defined as the “long-term cost of allostasis that accumulates over time and reflects the accumulation of damage that can lead to pathological states” ( McEwen 2000 ). Although the concept of allostatic state has not received much attention, the argument here is that alcoholism reflects largely a movement to an allostatic state, often before sufficient pathology has ensued to produced allostatic load sufficient for physical pathology ( Koob and Le Moal 2001 ).

Allostatic mechanisms have been hypothesized to be involved in maintaining a functioning brain reward system that has relevance for the pathology of addiction ( Koob and Le Moal 2001 ). Two components are hypothesized to adjust to challenges of the brain produced by drugs of abuse: underactivation of brain reward transmitters and circuits and recruitment of the brain anti-reward or brain stress systems ( Fig. 4 ). Thus, the very physiological mechanism that allows rapid responses to environmental challenge becomes the source of pathology if adequate time or resources are not available to shut off the response (one example is the interaction between CRF and norepinephrine in the brainstem and basal forebrain that could lead to pathological anxiety; Koob 1999 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms403706f4.jpg

a Schematic of the progression of alcohol dependence over time, illustrating the shift in underlying motivational mechanisms. From initial, positive reinforcing, pleasurable alcohol effects, the addictive process progresses over time to being maintained by negative reinforcing relief from a negative emotional state. Data presented in this paper suggest that neuroadaptations encompassing the recruitment of extrahypothalamic CRF systems are key to this shift. (Taken with permission from Heilig and Koob 2007 .) b The a-process represents a positive hedonic or positive mood state, and the b-process represents the negative hedonic or negative mood state. The affective stimulus (state) has been argued to be the sum of both the a-process and b-process. An individual who experiences a positive hedonic mood state from a drug of abuse with sufficient time between re-administering the drug is hypothesized to retain the a-process. An appropriate counteradaptive opponent process (b-process) that balances the activational process (a-process) does not lead to an allostatic state. The changes in the affective stimulus (state) in an individual with repeated frequent drug use may represent a transition to an allostatic state in the brain systems and, by extrapolation, a transition to addiction ( see text). Notice that the apparent b-process never returns to the original homeostatic level before drug taking begins again, thus creating a greater and greater allostatic state in the brain emotional systems. The counteradaptive opponent-process (b-process) does not balance the activational process (a-process) but in fact shows a residual hysteresis. Although these changes illustrated in the figure are exaggerated and condensed over time, the hypothesis is that even during post-detoxification (a period of “protracted abstinence”), the brain emotional systems still bear allostatic changes ( see text). The following definitions apply: alkalosis, the process of achieving stability through change; allostatic state, a state of chronic deviation of the regulatory system from its normal (homeostatic) operating level: allostatic load, the cost to the brain and body of the deviation, accumulating over time, and reflecting in many cases pathological states and accumulation of damage. [Modified with permission from Koob and Le Moal 2001 .]

Repeated challenges, such as with repeated alcohol binges, lead to attempts of the brain via molecular, cellular, and neurocircuitry changes to maintain stability but at a cost. For the alcoholism framework elaborated here, the residual deviation from a normal emotional state is termed the allostatic state. This state represents a combination of chronic elevation of the reward set point fueled by decreased function of reward circuits and recruitment of anti-reward systems, both of which lead to the compulsivity of alcohol-seeking and alcohol taking. How these systems are modulated by other known brain emotional systems localized to the basal forebrain, where the ventral striatum and extended amygdala project to convey emotional valence, how the dysregulation of brain emotional systems impacts on the cognitive domain linked to impairments in executive function, and how individuals differ at the molecular-genetic level of analysis to convey loading on these circuits remain challenges for future research ( George and Koob 2010 ).

As such, the present thesis does not preclude a key role for other systems associated with the addiction process, including the mesolimbic dopamine system involved in incentive salience, the dorsal striatum involved in habit formation, the parabrachial amygdala and spinothalamocortical systems involved in pain, and the prefrontal cortex involved in decision-making ( Koob and Volkow 2010 ; George and Koob 2010 ). Such modules are driven by bottom-up signals from both the external world and interoceptive signals and by top-down signals from higher-order systems mediating cognitive control. Indeed, the failure of a specific module may differ from one individual to another and may represent a neuropsychobiological mechanism underlying individual differences in the vulnerability to drug addiction. For example, we have hypothesized that individual differences in the function of the incentive salience mesolimbic dopamine system and the habit/striatum modules may be particularly important for craving-type 1 (or reward craving), defined as craving for the rewarding effects of alcohol and usually induced by stimuli that have been paired with alcohol self-administration, such as environmental cues. Additionally, hypoactivity of the decision-making/prefrontal cortex module may lead to a loss of control over drug intake despite negative consequence because of impaired inhibitory control and decision-making leading to choices of immediate rewards over delayed rewards ( Goldstein and Volkow 2002 ).

Nevertheless the hypothesis outlined here is that a core component of alcoholism involves hyperactivity of the negative emotional state/extended amygdala system that is associated with increased emotional pain and stress and might be a risk factor for drug use as self-medication for emotional pain, dysphoria, and stress ( Khantzian 1997 ). A subhypothesis is that vulnerability in the emotional pain parabrachial-amygdala system ( Besson 1999 ; Shurman et al. 2010 ) may lead to increased emotional pain during withdrawal and intense craving-type 2 (or withdrawal relief craving), which is conceptualized as an excessive motivation for the drug to obtain relief from a state change characterized by anxiety and dysphoria after protracted abstinence ( Heinz et al. 2003 ), thus contributing to the preponderant role of the withdrawal/negative affect stage that characterizes alcoholism. Increased reactivity of the stress/hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis module may be critical in the initiation of alcohol intake and for the maintenance of drug intake which have little initial rewarding value, such as nicotine. Activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis can potentiate the reinforcing effects of drugs ( Piazza and Le Moal 1998 ). However, this activation can in turn drive amygdala CRF. further exacerbating the development of negative emotional states ( Koob and Kreek 2007 ). Although the initial deficit in a specific functional circuit that drives excessive drinking might be specific to one stage of the addiction cycle, as the transition to addiction progresses, an individual is ultimately likely to show a progressive and generalized loss of control over many, if not all, systems. However, the thesis argued here is that as excessive alcohol intake progresses to Substance Dependence on Alcohol (Alcoholism), a common dysregulated functional element is a reward system deficit.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Michael Arends for assistance with manuscript preparation and editing. Research was supported by National Institutes of Health grants AA06420 and AA08459 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and the Pearson Center for Alcoholism and Addiction Research. This is publication number 20763 from The Scripps Research Institute.

  • Alheid GF, De Olmos JS, Beltramino CA. Amygdala and extended amygdala. In: Paxinos G, editor. The rat nervous system. Academic Press; San Diego: 1995. pp. 495–578. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allen TJ, Moeller FG, Rhoades HM, Cherek DR. Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998; 50 :137–145. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Alonso G, Szafarczyk A, Balmefrezol M, Assenmacher I. Immunocytochemical evidence for stimulatory control by the ventral noradrenergic bundle of parvocellular neurons of the paraventricular nucleus secreting corticotropin-releasing hormone and vasopressin in rats. Brain Res. 1986; 397 :297–307. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. American Psychiatric Press; Washington. DC: 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. American Psychiatric Press; Washington, DC: 2000. text revision. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aston-Jones G, Delfs JM, Druhan J, Zhu Y. The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: a target site for noradrenergic actions in opiate withdrawal. In: McGinty JF, editor. Advancing from the ventral striatum to the extended amygdala: implications for neuropsychiatry and drug abuse (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Vol. 877. New York Academy of Sciences; New York: 1999. pp. 486–498. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baldwin HA, Rassnick S, Rivier J, Koob GF, Britton KT. CRF antagonist reverses the “anxiogenic” response to ethanol withdrawal in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1991; 103 :227–232. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker HC, Lopez MF. Increased ethanol drinking after repeated chronic ethanol exposure and withdrawal experience in C57BL/6 mice. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2004; 28 :1829–1838. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Besson JM. The neurobiology of pain. Lancet. 1999; 353 :1610–1615. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blackburn-Munro G, Jensen BS. The anticonvulsant retigabine attenuates nociceptive behaviours in rat models of persistent and neuropathic pain. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003; 460 :109–116. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breese GR, Overstreet DH, Knapp DJ, Navarro M. Prior multiple ethanol withdrawals enhance stress-induced anxiety-like behavior; inhibition by CRF 1 - and benzodiazepine-receptor antagonists and a 5-HT 1a -receptor agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005; 30 :1662–1669. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown SA, Schuckit MA. Changes in depression among abstinent alcoholics. J Stud Alcohol. 1988; 49 :412–417. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown G, Jackson A, Stephens DN. Effects of repeated withdrawal from chronic ethanol on oral self-administration of ethanol on a progressive ratio schedule. Behav Pharmacol. 1998; 9 :149–161. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chance WT, Sheriff S, Peng F, Balasubramaniam A. Antagonism of NPY-induced feeding by pretreatment with cyclic AMP response element binding protein antisense oligonucleotide. Neuropeptides. 2000; 34 :167–172. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chu K, Koob GF, Cole M, Zorrilla EP, Roberts AJ. Dependence-induced increases in ethanol self-administration in mice are blocked by the CRF 1 receptor antagonist antalarmin and by CRF 1 , receptor knockout. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007; 86 :813–821. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Timary P, Luts A, Hers D, Luminel O. Absolute and relative stability of alexithymia in alcoholic inpatients undergoing alcohol withdrawal: relationship to depression and anxiety. Psychiatry Res. 2008; 157 :105–113. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Delfs JM, Zhu Y, Druhan JP, Aston-Jones G. Noradrenaline in the ventral forebrain is critical for opiate withdrawal-induced aversion. Nature. 2000; 403 :430–434. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Devaud LL, Fritschy JM, Sieghart W, Morrow AL. Bidirectional alterations of GABA A receptor subunit pepetide levels in rat cortex during chronic ethanol consumption and withdrawal. J Neurochem. 1997; 69 :126–130. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Di Ciano P, Coury A, Depoortere RY, Egilmez Y, Lane JD, Emmett-Oglesby MW, Lepiane FG, Phillips AG, Blaha CD. Comparison of changes in extracellular dopamine concentrations in the nucleus accumbens during intravenous self-administration of cocaine or d-amphetamine. Behav Pharmacol. 1995; 6 :311–322. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doyon WM, York JL, Diaz LM, Samson HH, Czachowski CL, Gonzales RA. Dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens during consummatory phases of oral ethanol self-administration. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2003; 27 :1573–1582. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Driessen M, Meier S, Hill A, Wetterling T, Langs W, Junghanns K. The course of anxiety, depression and drinking behaviours after completed detoxification in alcoholics with and without comorbid anxiety and depressive disorders. Alcohol Alcoholism. 2001; 36 :249–255. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Epping-Jordan MP, Watkins SS, Koob GF, Markou A. Dramatic decreases in brain reward function during nicotine withdrawal. Nature. 1998; 393 :76–79. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fidler TL, Clews TW, Cunningham CL. Reestablishing an intragastric ethanol self-infusion model in rats. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2006; 30 :414–428. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fillmore MT, Rush CR. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002; 66 :265–273. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Finn DA, Snelling C, Fretwell AM, Tanchuck MA, Underwood L, Cole M, Crabbe JC, Roberts AJ. Increased drinking during withdrawal from intermittent ethanol exposure is blocked by the CRF receptor antagonist D-Phe-CRF(12–41) Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2007; 31 :939–949. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funk CK, O’Dell LE, Crawford EF, Koob GF. Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats. J Neurosci. 2006; 26 :11324–11332. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funk CK, Zorrilla EP, Lee M-J, Rice KC, Koob GF. Corticotropin-releasing factor 1 antagonists selectively reduce ethanol self-administration in ethanol-dependent rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 61 :78–86. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gardner EL, Vorel SR. Cannabinoid transmission and reward-related events. Neurobiol Dis. 1998; 5 :502–533. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gehlert DR, Cippitelli A, Thorsell A, Le AD, Hipskind PA, Hamdouchi C, Lu J, Hembre EJ, Cramer J, Song M, McKinzie D, Morin M, Ciccocioppo R, Heilig M. 3-(4-Chloro-2-morpholin-4-ylthiazol-5-yl)-8-(1-ethylpropyl)-2,6-dimethyl-imidazo[1,2-b]pyridazine: a novel brain penetrant, orally available corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 antagonist with efficacy in animal models of alcoholism. J Neurosci. 2007; 27 :2718–2726. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • George O, Koob GF. Individual differences in prefrontal cortex function and the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010 (in press) [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gilpin NW, Misra K, Koob GF. Neuropeptide Y in the central nucleus of the amygdala suppresses dependence-induced increases in alcohol drinking. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008; 90 :475–480. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gilpin NW, Smith AD, Cole M, Weiss F, Koob GF, Richardson HN. Operant behavior and alcohol levels in blood and brain of alcohol-dependent rats. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2009; 33 :2113–2123. [erratum: 34: 382] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002; 159 :1642–1652. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodwin DW. Alcoholism: the facts. Oxford University Press; New York: 1981. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Green L, Fristoe N, Myerson J. Temporal discounting and preference reversals in choice between delayed outcomes. Psychon Bull Rev. 1994; 1 :383–389. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hebb DO. Textbook of psychology. 3. W.B. Saunders; Philadelphia: 1972. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heilig M, Koob GF. A key role for corticotropin-releasing factor in alcohol dependence. Trends Neurosci. 2007; 30 :399–406. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heimer L, Alheid G. Piecing together the puzzle of basal forebrain anatomy. In: Napier TC, Kalivas PW, Hanin I, editors. The basal forebrain: Anatomy to function (series title: advances in experimental medicine and biology) Vol. 295. Plenum Press; New York: 1991. pp. 1–42. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heinz A, Lober S, Georgi A, Wrase J, Hermann D, Rey ER, Wellek S, Mann K. Reward craving and withdrawal relief craving: assessment of different motivational pathways to alcohol intake. Alcohol Alcoholism. 2003; 38 :35–39. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hemby SE, Martin TJ, Co C, Dworkin SI, Smith JE. The effects of intravenous heroin administration on extracellular nucleus accumbens dopamine concentrations as determined by in vivo microdialysis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1995; 273 :591–598. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hernandez G, Hamdani S, Rajabi H, Conover K, Stewart J, Arvanitogiannis A, Shizgal P. Prolonged rewarding stimulation of the rat medial forebrain bundle: neurochemical and behavioral consequences. Behav Neurosci. 2006; 120 :888–904. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holter SM, Henniger MS, Lipkowski AW, Spanagel R. Kappa-opioid receptors and relapse-like drinking in long-term ethanol-experienced rats. Psychopharmacology. 2000; 153 :93–102. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ji D, Gilpin NW, Richardson HN, Rivier CL, Koob GF. Effects of naltrexone, duloxetine, and a corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 receptor antagonist on binge-like alcohol drinking in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2008; 19 :1–12. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1997; 4 :231–244. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Khantzian EJ. Understanding addictive vulnerability: an evolving psychodynamic perspective. Neuro-Psychoanalysis. 2003; 5 :5–7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knapp C. Drinking: a love story. Dial Press; New York: 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knapp DJ, Overstreet DH, Moy SS, Breese GR. SB242084, flumazenil, and CRA1000 block ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety in rats. Alcohol. 2004; 32 :101–111. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knapp DJ, Overstreet DH, Breese GR. Modulation of ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior during later withdrawals by treatment of early withdrawals with benzodiazepine/ γ -aminobutyric acid ligands. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2005; 29 :553–563. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knapp DJ, Overstreet DH, Breese GR. Baclofen blocks expression and sensitization of anxiety-like behavior in an animal model of repeated stress and ethanol withdrawal. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2007; 31 :582–595. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kohut H. The analysis of the self (series title: the psychoanalytic study of the child) Vol. 4. International Universities Press; New York: 1971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. Drugs of abuse: anatomy, pharmacology, and function of reward pathways. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1992; 13 :177–184. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. Corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine and stress. Biol Psychiatry. 1999; 46 :1167–1180. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2003; 27 :232–243. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. Allostatic view of motivation: implications for psychopathology. In: Bevins RA, Bardo MT, editors. Motivational factors in the etiology of drug abuse (series title: nebraska symposium on motivation) Vol. 50. University of Nebraska Press; Lincoln NE: 2004. pp. 1–18. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron. 2008a; 59 :11–34. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF. Neurobiology of addiction. In: Galanter M, Kleber HD, editors. Textbook of substance abuse treatment. 4. American Psychiatric Publishing; Washington DC: 2008b. pp. 3–16. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Boutrel B, Chen SA, Kenny PJ, Markou A, O’Dell LE, Parsons LH, Sanna PP. Neurobiological mechanisms in the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1994; 27 :739–749. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Bloom FE. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science. 1988; 242 :715–723. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Reward, motivation, and addiction. In: Squire LG, Berg D, Bloom FE, Du Lac S, Ghosh A, Spitzer N, editors. Fundamental Neuroscience. 3. Academic Press; Amsterdam: 2008. pp. 987–1016. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Kreek MJ. Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2007; 164 :1149–1159. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997; 278 :52–58. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001; 24 :97–129. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the “dark side” of drug addiction. Nature Neurosci. 2005; 8 :1442–1444. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Neurobiology of addiction. Academic Press; London: 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008; 59 :29–53. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol Rev. 2010; 35 :217–238. [erratum:35:1051] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Winger GD, Meyerhoff JL, Annau Z. Effects of D-amphetamine on concurrent self-stimulation of forebrain and brain stem loci. Brain Res. 1977; 137 :109–126. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koob GF, Lloyd GK, Mason BJ. Development of pharmacotherapies for drug addiction: a Rosetta stone approach. Nature Rev Drug Discov. 2009; 8 :500–515. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kornelsky C, Esposito RU. Euphorigenic drugs: effects on the reward pathways of the brain. Fed Proc. 1979; 38 :2473–7476. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Land BB, Bruchas MR, Lemos JC, Xu M, Melief EJ, Chavkin C. The dysphoric component of stress is encoded by activation of the dynorphin κ -opioid system. J Neurosci. 2008; 28 :407–414. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Le Doux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000; 23 :155–184. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Logan GD, Schachar RJ, Tannock R. Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol Sci. 1997; 8 :60–64. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lopez MF, Becker HC. Effect of pattern and number of chronic ethanol exposures on subsequent voluntary ethanol intake in C57BL/6J mice. Psychopharmacology. 2005; 181 :688–696. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Majchrowicz E. Induction of physical dependence upon ethanol and the associated behavioral changes in rats. Psychopharmacologia. 1975; 43 :245–254. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Markou A, Koob GF. Post-cocaine anhedonia: an animal model of cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1991; 4 :17–26. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin-Solch C, Magyar S, Kunig G, Missimer J, Schultz W, Leenders KL. Changes in brain activation associated with reward processing in smokers and nonsmokers: a positron emission tomography study. Exp Brain Res. 2001; 139 :278–286. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McBride WJ, Li TK. Animal models of alcoholism: neurobiology of high alcohol-drinking behavior in rodents. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 1998; 12 :339–369. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000; 22 :108–124. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Melis M, Spiga S, Diana M. The dopamine hypothesis of drug addiction: hypodopaminergic state. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2005; 63 :101–154. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Merlo-Pich E, Lorang M, Yeganeh M, Rodriguez de Fonseca F, Raber J, Koob GF, Weiss F. Increase of extracellular corticotropin-releasing factor-like immunoreactivity levels in the amygdala of awake rats during restraint stress and ethanol withdrawal as measured by microdialysis. J Neurosci. 1995; 15 :5439–5447. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mhatre MC, Pena G, Sieghart W, Ticku MK. Antibodies specific for GABAA receptor alpha subunits reveal that chronic alcohol treatment down-regulates α -subunit expression in rat brain regions. J Neurochem. 1993; 61 :1620–1625. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001; 158 :1783–1793. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morzorati SL, Ramchandani VA, Flury L, Li TK, O’Connor S. Self-reported subjective perception of intoxication reflects family history of alcoholism when breath alcohol levels are constant. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2002; 26 :1299–1306. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moy SS, Knapp DJ, Criswell HE, Breese GR. Flumazenil blockade of anxiety following ethanol withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1997; 131 :354–360. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mucha RF, Herz A. Motivational properties of kappa and mu opioid receptor agonists studied with place and taste preference conditioning. Psychopharmacology. 1985; 86 :274–280. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nestler EJ. Molecular basis of long-term plasticity underlying addiction. Nature Rev Neurosci. 2001; 2 :119–128. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nestler EJ. Historical review: molecular and cellular mechanisms of opiate and cocaine addiction. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2004; 25 :210–218. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nestler EJ. Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction? Nature Neurosci. 2005; 8 :1445–1449. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Neugebauer V, Li W, Bird GC, Han JS. The amygdala and persistent pain. Neuroscientist. 2004; 10 :221–234. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Dell LE, Roberts AJ, Smith RT, Koob GF. Enhanced alcohol self-administration after intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2004; 28 :1676–1682. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olds J, Milner P. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954; 47 :419–427. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olive MF, Koenig HN, Nannini MA, Hodge CW. Elevated extracellular CRF levels in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis during ethanol withdrawal and reduction by subsequent ethanol intake. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002; 72 :213–220. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Overstreet DH, Knapp DJ, Breese GR. Modulation of multiple ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior by CRF and CRF 1 , receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004; 77 :405–413. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Overstreet DH, Knapp DJ, Breese GR. Pharmacological modulation of repeated ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior differs in alcohol-preferring P and Sprague-Dawley rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005; 81 :122–130. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pandey SC. The gene transcription factor cyclic AMP-responsive element binding protein: role in positive and negative affective states of alcohol addiction. Pharmacol Ther. 2004; 104 :47–58. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paterson NE, Myers C, Markou A. Effects of repeated withdrawal from continuous amphetamine administration on brain reward function in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2000; 152 :440–446. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pfeiffer A, Brantl V, Herz A, Emrich HM. Psychotomimesis mediated by κ opiate receptors. Science. 1986; 233 :774–776. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Piazza PV, Le Moal M. The role of stress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1998; 19 :67–74. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Potokar J, Coupland N, Glue P, Groves S, Malizia A, Bailey J, Wilson S, Nutt D. Flumazenil in alcohol withdrawal: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Alcohol Alcoholism. 1997; 32 :605–611. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rachlin H, Green L. Commitment, choice and self-control. J Exp Anal Behav. 1972; 17 :15–22. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ramchandani VA, Umhau J, Pavon FJ, Ruiz-Velasco V, Margas W, Sun H, Damadzic R, Eskay R, Schoor M, Thorsell A, Schwandt ML, Sommer WH, George DT, Parsons LH, Herscovitch P, Hommer D, Heilig M. A genetic determinant of the striatal dopamine response to alcohol in men. Mol Psychiatry. 2010 (in press) [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rasmussen DD, Boldt BM, Bryant CA, Mitton DR, Larsen SA, Wilkinson CW. Chronic daily ethanol and withdrawal: 1. Long-term changes in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2000; 24 :1836–1849. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rassnick S, Heinrichs SC, Britton KT, Koob GF. Microinjection of a corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist into the central nucleus of the amygdala reverses anxiogenic-like effects of ethanol withdrawal. Brain Res. 1993; 605 :25–32. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rhodes JS, Best K, Belknap JK, Finn DA, Crabbe JC. Evaluation of a simple model of ethanol drinking to intoxication in C57BL/6J mice. Physiol Behav. 2005; 84 :53–63. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richardson HN, Zhao Y, Fekete EM, Funk CK, Wirsching P, Janda KD, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. MPZP: a novel small molecule corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 receptor (CRF 1 ) antagonist. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008; 88 :497–510. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rimondini R, Arlinde C, Sommer W, Heilig M. Long-lasting increase in voluntary ethanol consumption and transcriptional regulation in the rat brain after intermittent exposure to alcohol. FASEB J. 2002; 16 :27–35. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rivier C, Bruhn T, Vale W. Effect of ethanol on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the rat: role of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1984; 229 :127–131. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberto M, Cruz MT, Gilpin NW, Sabino V, Schweitzer P, Bajo M, Cottone P, Madamba SG, Stouffer DG, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF, Siggins GR, Parsons LH. Corticotropin releasing factor-induced amygdala gamma-aminobutyric acid release plays a key role in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 67 :831–839. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts AJ, Cole M, Koob GF. Intra-amygdala muscimol decreases operant ethanol self-administration in dependent rats. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 1996; 20 :1289–1298. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Koob GF. Operant self-administration of sweetened versus unsweetened ethanol: effects on blood alcohol levels. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 1999; 23 :1151–1157. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF. Excessive ethanol drinking following a history of dependence: animal model of allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000; 22 :581–594. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Romach MK, Sellers EM. Management of the alcohol withdrawal syndrome. Annu Rev Med. 1991; 42 :323–340. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rossetti ZL, Hmaidan Y, Gessa GL. Marked inhibition of mesolimbic dopamine release: a common feature of ethanol, morphine, cocaine and amphetamine abstinence in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1992; 221 :227–234. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rossetti ZL, Isola D, De Vry J, Fadda F. Effects of nimodipine on extracellular dopamine levels in the rat nucleus accumbens in ethanol withdrawal. Neuropharmacology. 1999; 38 :1361–1369. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Russell MAH. What is dependence? In: Edwards G, editor. Drugs and drug dependence. Lexington Books; Lexington MA: 1976. pp. 182–187. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Samson HH. Initiation of ethanol-maintained behavior: a comparison of animal models and their implication to human drinking. In: Thompson T, Dews PB, Barrett JE, editors. Neurobehavioral Pharmacology (series title: Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology) Vol. 6. Lawrence Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1987. pp. 221–248. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sapru MK, Diamond I, Gordon AS. Adenosine receptors mediate cellular adaptation to ethanol in NG108–15 cells. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994; 271 :542–548. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schulteis G, Liu J. Brain reward deficits accompany withdrawal (hangover) from acute ethanol in rats. Alcohol. 2006; 39 :21–28. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schulteis G, Markou A, Gold LH, Stinus L, Koob GF. Relative sensitivity to naloxone of multiple indices of opiate withdrawal: a quantitative dose-response analysis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994; 271 :1391–1398. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schulteis G, Markou A, Cole M, Koob G. Decreased brain reward produced by ethanol withdrawal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1995; 92 :5880–5884. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schulteis G, Hyytia P, Heinrichs SC, Koob GF. Effects of chronic ethanol exposure on oral self-administration of ethanol or saccharin by Wistar rats. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 1996; 20 :164–171. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shurnan J, Koob GF, Gutstein H. Hyperalgesia in pain treatment: an allostatic perspective. Pain Med 2010 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973; 22 :255–262. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychol Rev. 1974; 81 :119–145. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sommer WH, Rimondini R, Hansson AC, Hipskind PA, Gehlert DR, Barr CS, Heilig MA. Upregulation of voluntary alcohol intake, behavioral sensitivity to stress, and amygdala crhrl expression following a history of dependence. Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 63 :139–145. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Song ZH, Takemori AE. Stimulation by corticotropin-releasing factor of the release of immunoreactive dynorphin A from mouse spinal cords in vitro. Eur J Pharmacol. 1992; 222 :27–32. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sterling P, Eyer J. Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In: Fisher S, Reason J, editors. Handbook of life stress. Cognition and health. Wiley; Chichester: 1988. pp. 629–649. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. An overview of the alexithymia construct. In: Bar-On R, Parker JDA, editors. The handbook of emotional intelligence: theory, development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace. Jossey-Bass; San Francisco: 2000. pp. 40–67. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y and corticotropin-releasing factor on ethanol intake in Wistar rats: interaction with chronic ethanol exposure. Behav Brain Res. 2005a; 161 :133–140. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y on appetitive and consummatory behaviors associated with alcohol drinking in wistar rats with a history of ethanol exposure. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2005b; 29 :584–590. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thorsell A, Rapunte-Canonigo V, O’Dell L, Chen SA, King A, Lekic D, Koob GF, Sanna PP. Viral vector-induced amygdala NPY overexpression reverses increased alcohol intake caused by repeated deprivations in Wistar rats. Brain. 2007; 130 :1330–1337. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trzaskowska E, Kostowski W. Further studies on the role of noradrenergic mechanisms in ethanol withdrawal syndrome in rats. Pol J Pharmacol. 1983; 35 :351–358. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valdez CR, Zorrilla EP, Roberts AJ, Koob GF. Antagonism of corticotropin-releasing factor attenuates the enhanced responsiveness to stress observed during protracted ethanol abstinence. Alcohol. 2003; 29 :55–60. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valdez GR, Sabino V, Koob GF. Increased anxiety-like behavior and ethanol self-administration in dependent rats: reversal via corticotropin-releasing factor-2 receptor activation. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res. 2002; 26 :1494–1501. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valdez GR, Platt DM, Rowlett JK, Rüedi-Bettschen D, Spealman RD. κ Agonist-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in squirrel monkeys: a role for opioid and stress-related mechanisms. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2007; 323 :525–533. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valentino RJ, Page ME, Curtis AL. Activation of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons by hemodynamic stress is due to local release of corticotropin-releasing factor. Brain Res. 1991; 555 :25–34. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valentino RJ, Foote SL, Page ME. The locus coeruleus as a site for integrating corticotropin-releasing factor and noradrenergic mediation of stress responses. In: Tache Y, Rivier C, editors. Corticotropin-releasing factor and cytokines: role in the stress response (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Vol. 697. New York Academy of Sciences; New York: 1993. pp. 173–188. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Bockstaele EJ, Colago EE, Valentino RJ. Amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing factor targets locus coeruleus dendrites: substrate for the co-ordination of emotional and cognitive limbs of the stress response. J Neuroendocrinol. 1998; 10 :743–757. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS. Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000; 10 :318–325. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. Role of dopamine in drug reinforcement and addiction in humans: results from imaging studies. Behav Pharmacol. 2002; 13 :355–366. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, Ma Y, Pradhan K, Wong C. Profound decreases in dopamine release in striatum in detoxified alcoholics: possible orbitofrontal involvement. J Neurosci. 2007; 27 :12700–12706. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walker BM, Koob GF. Pharmacological evidence for a motivational role of κ -Opioid systems in ethanol dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008; 33 :643–652. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walker BM, Rasmussen DD, Raskind MA, Koob GF. α 1 -Noradrenergic receptor antagonism blocks dependence-induced increases in responding for ethanol. Alcohol. 2008; 42 :91–97. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wee S, Koob GF. The role of the dynorphin-kappa opioid system in the reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse. Psychopharmacology. 2010; 210 :121–135. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiss F, Hurd YL, Ungerstedt U, Markou A, Plotsky PM, Koob GF. Neurochemical correlates of cocaine and ethanol self-administration. In: Kalivas PW, Samson HH, editors. The neurobiology of drug and alcohol addiction (series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences) Vol. 654. New York Academy of Sciences; New York: 1992a. pp. 220–241. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiss F, Markou A, Lorang MT, Koob GF. Basal extracellular dopamine levels in the nucleus accumbens are decreased during cocaine withdrawal after unlimited-access self-administration. Brain Res. 1992b; 593 :314–318. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiss F, Parsons LH, Schulteis G, Hyytia P, Lorang MT, Bloom FE, Koob GF. Ethanol self-administration restores withdrawal-associated deficiencies in accumbal dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine release in dependent rats. J Neurosci. 1996; 16 :3474–3485. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wikler A. A psychodynamic study of a patient during experimental self-regulated re-addiction to morphine. Psychiatric Q. 1952; 26 :270–293. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wolffgramm J, Heyne A. From controlled drug intake to loss of control: the irreversible development of drug addiction in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 1995; 70 :77–94. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zald DH, Kim SW. The orbitofrontal cortex. In: Salloway SP, Malloy PF, Duffy JD, editors. The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness. American Psychiatric Press; Washington. DC: 2001. pp. 33–70. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zorrilla EP, Valdez GR, Weiss F. Changes in levels of regional CRF-like-immunoreactivity and plasma corticosterone during protracted drug withdrawal in dependent rats. Psychopharmacology. 2001; 158 :374–381. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Alcohol Consumption and Crime Incidence in the Philippines

Profile image of Jennifer Hinlo

Most police reports about crime incidence are attributed to alcohol drinking. With this conviction, it is imperative to investigate the determinants of alcohol consumption and examine how alcohol consumption affects crime incidence in the Philippines. The study employed cross-sectional data and panel data in the estimation. There is an upward trend in the consumption of alcohol from 2004-2012. Results in the estimation showed that among the determinants of alcohol consumption, food expenditure, wear expenditure, educational expenditure, income and age, significantly explain the changes in alcohol consumption in the Philippines. On the other hand, alcohol consumption significantly affects the changes in murder and physical injury. Although there is a close relationship between alcohol and crime, the impact of alcohol as a cause of crime should be seen in the context of other causal factors.

Related Papers

thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

Miguel Gillado

Journal of Advances in Humanities Research

Manilyn Barreda

The low crime rate has been identified as a predictor of a calm, orderly, and secure neighborhood. Furthermore, a safe and secure environment is essential in encouraging investment and economic progress in a country. Crime statistics can help criminal justice professionals predict increased crime risk. This study, therefore, aims to compare the crime rate of two municipalities in the province of Sorsogon: Bulan Sorsogon and Irosin Sorsogon from the calendar year 2018 to 2020. This study used document analysis to analyze and compare the crime data in the Municipality of Bulan and Irosin Sorsogon. Data reflected that in the comparison of the two municipalities in the province, the Municipality of Bulan has the highest crime rate (61%) compared to the Municipality of Irosin which has a (39%) total crime rate. Data also revealed that the crime under special law was the most numbered crime committed with (253) total in the five parameters of crime in both municipalities. Interestingly cr...

Acta Medica Philippina

Teodoro Herbosa

Introduction. The ever-increasing global road traffic accidents is caused by several risk factors: human factors, vehicle factors, and road environmental factors. In the Philippines, 12,000 road users die annually due to road crashes and almost one-third are alcohol-related crashes.Objectives. This study aims to investigate the effects of suspected alcohol intoxication on the severity of injuries among drivers and passengers in a major urban region in the Philippines using the Metro Manila Accident Recording and Analysis System (MMARAS) of the Metropolitan Manila Development Authority from 2005 to 2020.Methods. The Metro Manila Accident Recording and Analysis System (MMARAS) was used to determine the data on trends and association for alcohol-related road crashes, while descriptive and inferential statistics such as logistic and multinomial regression were used to determine trends and associations.Results. Suspected alcohol intoxication road crash cases in Metro Manila have decrease...

Management Science Letters

Seyed Nouraddin Mousavinasab

BMC Public Health

Surasak Chaiyasong

Hernan Malaga , Carlos Huaco

Background: La Victoria was considered, the most violent area in Lima City, the local government enforced a public policy regarding number of hours for selling of alcoholic beverages in January 2007. The study was designed to compare its results in Violence between one district with the law and other without the law. Methods: This retrospective cross-sectional was an ecological study with a chronological and geographical comparison between La Victoria, with the restriction and Cercado de Lima without the ban. The participants in the study were patients from a local National Hospital, with aggres-sions from fighting, or were wounded in traffic accidents, and violent death bodies at National Institute of Legal Medicine. Data were analyzed, using clinical histories (2006 vs. 2007-8) and ne-cropsies (2005-6 vs. 2007-8) Results: The reduction of aggression rates at La Victoria in 2007 and 2008 in comparison to 2006, were 40.7% and 36.4% respectively (P< 0.05). It was related to the number of hours of liquor authorized selling Y=-11.25+27.32 X (P<0.05). There was a reduction of 44% in homicide (P<0.05) and 35% in suicide rates between biennia's. The female/ male ratio of homicides changed from 1/7.3 to 1/4.6. A significant increase in the rate of alcohol positive dead bodies was observed (20.3% to 41.5%), (Relative Risk (RR) = 2.03, (95% Confidence Interval (CI) = (1.09-3.8), χ² (1) =5.24, (P< 0.05). Conclusions: The reduction of violence was probably due to the ban, indicating the importance of programs to control alcohol consumption which lead to decrease the rate of violence and its' consequences like homicides, impulsive violence.

It is a pleasure to be with you here and an honour to be starting off our substantive discussion. The topic we have taken on discussing together is enormously complicated, and we must not imagine that our weekend together will knit up all its complexities, but I hope we shall be able to do some unraveling that gives us some good loose ends for future weaving. If you asked the conventional wisdom one hundred years ago whether alcohol caused crime, the most likely answer would be an unequivocal" yes".

Erlan Bakiev , Bekchoro Aliiaskarov

RELATED PAPERS

Heung-Kook Choi

preetpal kour

sscardio.org

Gloria Vergara vergara

Revista Brasileira de Anestesiologia

JOAQUIM EDSON VIEIRA

Proceedings of the 19th IFAC World Congress

José Nilson Rodrigues de souza

Leonard Amekudzi

Bishal khatiwada

Fidel Camacho Pérez

Grosir CD Golden Nick

D de de Kool

Progress In Electromagnetics Research C

Surendra Rathod

Jambura Journal of Mathematics Education

Nezuko Kamado

Bulletin of Anesthesia History

Sukumar Desai

Revista Cientí­fica/FAP

Lurdi Blauth

Veterinary Sciences: Research and Reviews

Yonis Mohamed

Revista question

Hernan Pajoni

AGROTEK: Jurnal Ilmiah Ilmu Pertanian

Blanca Henriquez Espinoza

Applied Surface Science

Laurent Gremillard

Soil Science Society of America Journal

Paulo Gomes

Rudie van Leeuwen

Sensing and Imaging

Iman Ansaripour

Thermochimica Acta

deepika thakur

Archives of Virology

Vicente Pallas

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Psychological Theories of Alcohol Consumption

  • First Online: 11 May 2021

Cite this chapter

thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  • Richard Cooke 8 , 9  

1110 Accesses

1 Citations

This chapter begins with an outline of several psychological theories of alcohol consumption, including alcohol expectancy theory, the cognitive model of binge drinking, incentive motivation model, and the theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour, which have been proposed to account for alcohol consumption. An outline of each theory is provided before the evidence base for each is reviewed. Next, the chapter compares and contrasts the theories in terms of evidence before considering theoretical overlap between constructs and outlining studies that have tested multiple theories in the same sample. After a brief discussion of theoretical integration, a number of methodological issues with existing tests of theories are outlined and discussed. The chapter ends with suggestions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50 , 179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Article   Google Scholar  

Ajzen, I. (1996). The directive influence of attitudes on behavior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Psychology of action (pp. 385–403). New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Google Scholar  

Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32 , 665–683.

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1973). Attitudinal and normative variables as predictors of specific behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27 , 41–57.

Ajzen, I., & Sheikh, S. (2013). Action versus inaction: Anticipated affect in the theory of planned behavior: Anticipated affect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43 (1), 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00989.x

Atwell, K., Abraham, C., & Duka, T. (2011). A parsimonious, integrative model of key psychological correlates of UK university students’ alcohol consumption. Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire), 46 (3), 253–260. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agr016

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191–215.

Burgess, M., Cooke, R., & Davies, E. L. (2019). My own personal hell : Approaching and exceeding thresholds of too much alcohol. Psychology & Health, 34 (12), 1451–1469. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2019.1616087

Cameron, D., Epton, T., Norman, P., Sheeran, P., Harris, P. R., Webb, T. L., … Shah, I. (2015). A theory-based online health behaviour intervention for new university students (U@Uni:LifeGuide): Results from a repeat randomized controlled trial. Trials, 16 . https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-015-1092-4

Cooke, R., Bailey, O., Jennings, J., Yuen, C., & Gardner, B. (2020). Do preparatory behaviours predict alcohol consumption among UK university students? British Journal of Health Psychology , https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12489

Cooke, R., Beccaria, F., Demant, J., Fernandes-Jesus, M., Fleig, L., Negreiros, J., Scholz, U., & de Visser, R. (2019). Patterns of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harm among European university students. European Journal of Public Health , ckz067. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckz067 .

Cooke, R., Beccaria, F., Demant, J., Fernandes-Jesus, M., Fleig, L., Scholz, U., & de Visser, R. O. (2021). Predicting alcohol consumption among European university students using data from the CALIBRATE study. Unpublished manuscript

Cooke, R., Dahdah, M., Norman, P., & French, D. P. (2016). How well does the theory of planned behaviour predict alcohol consumption? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 10 , 148–167. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2014.947547

Cooke, R., Sniehotta, F., & Schuz, B. (2007). Predicting binge-drinking behaviour using an extended TPB: Examining the impact of anticipated regret and descriptive norms. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 42 (2), 84–91. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl115

Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6 (2), 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.117

Cooper, M. L., Krull, J. L., Agocha, V. B., Flanagan, M. E., Orcutt, H. K., Grabe, S., … Jackson, M. (2008). Motivational pathways to alcohol use and abuse among Black and White adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117 (3), 485–501. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012592

Cooper, M. L., Kuntsche, E., Levitt, A., Barber, L. L., & Wolf, S. (2015). Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco. In K. J. Sher (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of substance use disorders (Vol. 1). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199381678.013.017

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 (2), 168–180.

Epton, T., Norman, P., Dadzie, A.-S., Harris, P. R., Webb, T. L., Sheeran, P., … Shah, I. (2014). A theory-based online health behaviour intervention for new university students (U@Uni): Results from a randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health, 14 . https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-563

Fearnow-Kenney, M., Wyrick, D. L., Milroy, J. J., Reifsteck, E. J., Day, T., & Kelly, S. E. (2016). The effects of a web-based alcohol prevention program on social norms, expectancies, and intentions to prevent harm among college student-athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 30 , 113–122.

Fernandes-Jesus, M., Beccaria, F., Demant, J., Fleig, L., Menezes, I., Scholz, U., … Cooke, R. (2016). Validation of the drinking motives questionnaire—Revised in six European countries. Addictive Behaviors, 62 , 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.06.010

Foster, D. W., Dukes, K., & Sartor, C. E. (2016). The road to drink is paved with high intentions: Expectancies, refusal self-efficacy, and intentions among heavy drinking college students. Alcohol, 50 , 65–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.alcohol.2015.11.004

French, D. P., Cooke, R., Mclean, N., Williams, M., & Sutton, S. (2007). What do people think about when they answer theory of planned behaviour questionnaires?: A ‘think aloud’ study. Journal of Health Psychology, 12 (4), 672–687. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105307078174

Gardner, B., de Bruijn, G.-J., & Lally, P. (2012). Habit, identity, and repetitive action: A prospective study of binge-drinking in UK students: Habit, identity, and repetitive action. British Journal of Health Psychology, 17 (3), 565–581. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8287.2011.02056.x

Goldsmith, A. A., Thompson, R. D., Black, J. J., Tran, G. Q., & Smith, J. P. (2012). Drinking refusal self-efficacy and tension-reduction alcohol expectancies moderating the relationship between generalized anxiety and drinking behaviors in young adult drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26 (1), 59–67. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024766

Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 8 , 249–266.

Hasking, P., Boyes, M., & Mullan, B. (2015). Reward and cognition: Integrating reinforcement sensitivity theory and social cognitive theory to predict drinking behavior. Substance Use & Misuse, 50 (10), 1316–1324. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826084.2015.1005315

Hauck-Filho, N., Teixeira, M. A. P., & Cooper, M. L. (2012). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Brazilian version of the Drinking Motives Questionnaire-Revised (DMQ-R). Addictive Behaviors, 37 (4), 524–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.11.023

Kuntsche, E., & Cooper, M. L. (2010). Drinking to have fun and to get drunk: Motives as predictors of weekend drinking over and above usual drinking habits. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 110 (3), 259–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.021

Leigh, B. C., & Stacy, A. W. (1993). Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological Assessment, 5 (2), 216–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.5.2.216

Morawska, A., & Oei, T. P. S. (2005). Binge drinking in university students: A test of the cognitive model. Addictive Behaviors, 30 (2), 203–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.05.011

Newton, N. C., Barrett, E. L., Swaffield, L., & Teesson, M. (2014). Risky cognitions associated with adolescent alcohol misuse: Moral disengagement, alcohol expectancies and perceived self-regulatory efficacy. Addictive Behaviors, 39 (1), 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.030

Norman, P. (2011). The theory of planned behavior and binge drinking among undergraduate students: Assessing the impact of habit strength. Addictive Behaviors, 36 (5), 502–507. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.01.025

Norman, P., Bennett, P., & Lewis, H. (1998). Understanding binge drinking among young people: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Health Education Research, 13 (2), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/13.2.163-a

Norman, P., Cameron, D., Epton, T., Webb, T. L., Harris, P. R., Millings, A., & Sheeran, P. (2018). A randomized controlled trial of a brief online intervention to reduce alcohol consumption in new university students: Combining self-affirmation, theory of planned behaviour messages, and implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 23 (1), 108–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12277

Norman, P., & Conner, M. (2006). The theory of planned behaviour and binge drinking: Assessing the moderating role of past behaviour within the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11 (1), 55–70. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910705X43741

Oei, T. P. S., & Baldwin, A. R. (1994). Expectancy theory: A two-process model of alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55 (5), 525–534. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1994.55.525

Oei, T. P. S., & Burrow, T. (2000). Alcohol expectancy and drinking refusal self-efficacy: A test of specificity theory. Addictive Behaviors, 25 (4), 499–507. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4603(99)00044-1

Oei, T. P. S., & Jardim, C. L. (2007). Alcohol expectancies, drinking refusal self-efficacy and drinking behaviour in Asian and Australian students. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 87 (2–3), 281–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.08.019

Oei, T. P. S., & Morawska, A. (2004). A cognitive model of binge drinking: The influence of alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self-efficacy. Addictive Behaviors, 29 (1), 159–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4603(03)00076-5

Oh, H., & Kim, Y. (2014). Drinking behavior and drinking refusal self-efficacy in Korean college students. Psychological Reports, 115 (3), 872–883. https://doi.org/10.2466/21.18.PR0.115c31z2

Radtke, T., Ostergaard, M., Cooke, R., & Scholz, U. (2017). Web-based alcohol intervention: Study of systematic attrition of heavy drinkers. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 19 (6), e217. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.6780

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Schlegel, R. P., D’Avernas, J. R., Zanna, M. P., DeCourville, N. H., & Manske, S. R. (1992). Problem drinking: A problem for the theory of reasoned action? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22 (5), 358–385. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01545.x

Urbán, R., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2008). Alcohol outcome expectancies and drinking motives mediate the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use among adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 33 (10), 1344–1352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.006

Young, R., & Oei, T. P. S. (1990). Drinking Expectancy Profile: A manual . University of Queensland.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK

Richard Cooke

Liverpool Centre for Alcohol Research, Liverpool, UK

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard Cooke .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

School of Psychology, University of East London, London, UK

Dominic Conroy

Department of Psychology, Health and Professional Development, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, UK

Emma Louise Davies

Department of Psychological Sciences, University of California, Merced, Merced, CA, USA

Martin S. Hagger

Faculty of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland

Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK

Richard O. de Visser

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Cooke, R. (2021). Psychological Theories of Alcohol Consumption. In: Cooke, R., Conroy, D., Davies, E.L., Hagger, M.S., de Visser, R.O. (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Psychological Perspectives on Alcohol Consumption. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-66941-6_2

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-66941-6_2

Published : 11 May 2021

Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-66940-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-66941-6

eBook Packages : Behavioral Science and Psychology Behavioral Science and Psychology (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Alcohol marketing and drunkenness among students in the

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  2. Position Paper Example Philippines / A Position Paper On The

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  3. Case Study Alcoholism

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  4. (PDF) history of alcoholism

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  5. 8.6.3 Alcohol Consumption and Crime Incidence in the Philippines

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

  6. (PDF) E. M. Jellinek's Disease Concept of Alcoholism

    thesis about alcoholism in philippines pdf

VIDEO

  1. Research title defense tips #philippines #research #thesis

  2. Research sites for students #philippines #thesis #research #researchtips #rrl #thesisdefense

  3. 酒はどのように彼の脳を蝕んだのか。無意識の観点から考察

  4. Asawa Ng Asawa Ko: Shaira MEETS Jeff while DRUNK! (Episode 36)

  5. TUP manila Architecture Thesis Deliberation 11-12

  6. Research title defense tips #philippines #research #thesis #presentation

COMMENTS

  1. (PDF) Inequities in alcohol-related harm in the Philippines

    Inequities in alcohol-related harm in the Philippines. May 2022. The Lancet Gastroenterology & Hepatology 7 (5):387-388. DOI: 10.1016/S2468-1253 (22)00053-X. Authors:

  2. Inequities in alcohol-related harm in the Philippines

    Despite there being lower levels of alcohol consumption in low-income and middle-income countries than in high-income countries, the burden of alcohol-attributable mortality falls disproportionately on countries with lower economic wealth.1 According to the WHO Global status report on alcohol and health, the age-standardised burden of death is highest among low-income and middle-income ...

  3. Alcohol marketing and drunkenness among students in the Philippines

    Background. Alcohol use is the most commonly used psychoactive substance in the world and is one of the leading causes of death and disability [].Alcohol abuse causes 3.2% of all deaths worldwide annually and also accounts for 4.0% of the global disease burden each year [].Research has shown that alcohol use is associated with alcohol addiction [], other drug use [], unintentional injuries [3 ...

  4. PDF THE FILIPINO AS THE QUINTESSENTIAL DRINKER: A Study of Alcohol

    3.75 litres of pure alcohol for thoselitres of pure alcohol for those 15 years and aboveyears and above and increases by 10% every year (Filipinos only third to Thailand and Japan) z38.9% of Filipinos are occasional alcohol drinkers, 11.1% are regular drinkers (13% males and 5.9%

  5. PDF Alcohol Consumption and Crime Incidence in The Philippines

    In the Philippines, alcohol plays a large role in criminal activities and violence. Excessive drinking has the ability to lower self-consciousness, impair a person's judgment and increase the risk . of aggressive behaviors. Because of this, alcohol-related violence and crime rates are high throughout the country (www.alcoholrehab.com). ...

  6. Thesis About Alcoholism in Philippines

    Thesis About Alcoholism in Philippines - Free download as PDF File (.pdf), Text File (.txt) or read online for free. Scribd is the world's largest social reading and publishing site.

  7. Alcohol marketing and drunkenness among students in the Philippines

    A largely unaddressed issue in lower income countries and the Philippines, in particular, is the role of alcohol marketing and its potential link to early alcohol use among youth. This study examines the associations between exposures to alcohol marketing and Filipino youths' drinking prevalence and drunkenness. Cross-sectional analyses were used to examine the Global School-based Student ...

  8. (PDF) Alcohol Drinking Behavior Among High School Students In Low

    A cross-sectional survey was conducted to assess the alcohol drinking behavior of adolescents and its related factors among 280 3rd year and 4th year high school students in one purposively ...

  9. [Pdf] Alcohol Consumption and Crime Incidence in The Philippines

    With this conviction, it is imperative to investigate the determinants of alcohol consumption and examine how alcohol consumption affects crime incidence in the Philippines. The study employed cross-sectional data and panel data in the estimation. There is an upward trend in the consumption of alcohol from 2004-2012.

  10. Alcohol, Alcoholism and Conditioning: A Review of the Literature and

    Alcoholism is a grave social, economic and psychiatric problem which has attracted the attention of countless research workers and theorists. From a survey of the recent literature two conclusions emerge. Firstly, that there is at present no general agreement as to the aetiology, dynamics and treatment of alcoholism (1).

  11. Alcohol use in the family and other factors, their relationship to

    This study was conducted to determine the alcohol use among family members and other factors, and their relationship to alcohol use among the third year and fourth year student nurses of the College of St. John-Roxas. A structured questionnaire was administered to 183 third and fourth year student nurses of the college. The results of the study showed that the respondents were mostly female ...

  12. (PDF) Alcohol marketing and drunkenness among students in the

    A largely unaddressed issue in lower income countries and the Philippines, in particular, is the role of alcohol marketing and its potential link to early alcohol use among youth.

  13. (Pdf) Smoking Cigarettes, Drinking Alcoholic Beverages, and Students

    Alcohol use prevalence was 55%; there were more male(35%) than female(20%) drinkers; 66% consumed beer, 86% of them consumed alcoholic wine; 45% began drinking at 11-15 years; 42% drank at ceremonies; 10% drank for pleasure; 22% drank because they feel it was a sociable thing to do; 4% and 2% respectively drink because their parents and friends ...

  14. (PDF) The Risks Associated With Alcohol Use and Alcoholism

    The growing scale of alcoholism, as well as its multidimensional consequences, justify the need for an in--depth analysis of this phenomenon. Harmful alcohol use and dependence is a risk factor ...

  15. PDF RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Alcohol marketing and drunkenness among

    Keywords: Alcohol, Alcohol marketing, Drunkenness, Philippines, Survey Background Alcohol use is the most commonly used psychoactive substance in the world and is one of the leading causes of death and disability [1]. Alcohol abuse causes 3.2% of all deaths worldwide annually and also accounts for 4.0% of the global disease burden each year [2 ...

  16. PDF The Effects of Alcohol Consumption on Academic Performance: A

    ABSTRACT: Alcohol consumption is known to be an addiction that provides negative outcomes mainly on health, excessive drinking of alcohol brings adverse effects on human health, also on activities that focus on school performance. This research aims to examine the link between alcohol use and the academic success of high school students.

  17. Philippine Traditional Alcoholic Beverages: A Germinal Study

    Abstract. Tapuey (rice wine), basi (sugarcane wine), tuba (palm wine), and lambanog (distilled liquor from tuba) are traditional alcoholic beverages of the Philippines that are intrinsically linked to the economic, social, and cultural life of the Filipinos. Despite their existence that predates the Spanish colonization that happened in the ...

  18. Theoretical Frameworks and Mechanistic Aspects of Alcohol Addiction

    Clinically and in animal models, the occasional but limited use of alcohol with the potential for abuse or dependence is distinct from escalated alcohol intake and the emergence of a chronic alcohol-dependent state. The thesis argued in the present synthesis is that alcoholism, similar to drug addiction, is a reward deficit disorder, and the ...

  19. (PDF) The Effects of Alcohol Consumption on Academic Performance: A

    Abstract. Alcohol consumption is known to be an addiction that provides negative outcomes mainly on health, excessive drinking of alcohol brings adverse effects on human health, also on activities ...

  20. Effects Nof Students' Alcoholism on Their Academic Performance in Jose

    Drinking at tertiary institutions has become part of the average student's lifestyle. According the World Health Organisation (2011), approximately 320 000 young people between the ages of 15 and 29 die from alcohol-related causes every year thus representing 9% of all deaths in that age group.

  21. (PDF) Alcohol Consumption and Crime Incidence in the Philippines

    Jennifer Hinlo. 2019. Most police reports about crime incidence are attributed to alcohol drinking. With this conviction, it is imperative to investigate the determinants of alcohol consumption and examine how alcohol consumption affects crime incidence in the Philippines. The study employed cross-sectional data and panel data in the estimation.

  22. Psychological Theories of Alcohol Consumption

    The cognitive model of binge drinking (Oei & Morawska, 2004) is an application of Alcohol Expectancy Theory to predict a drinking pattern, binge drinking, which has been defined as consuming more than a threshold number of drinks, or volume of alcohol, during a single drinking episode (e.g., HED, see Chap. 1).Like Alcohol Expectancy Theory, the model comprises alcohol expectancies and drinking ...

  23. Students Perceptions of Alcohol Use on University Campuses

    A report by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2020) showed that 53 percent of full-time college students drink alcohol in the US, with more than 13% participating in binge drinking. Similarly, a survey was. conducted among 7000 college students in New England and the result determined.