Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

A research hypothesis, in its plural form “hypotheses,” is a specific, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its outset. It is a key component of the scientific method .

Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding

Some key points about hypotheses:

  • A hypothesis expresses an expected pattern or relationship. It connects the variables under investigation.
  • It is stated in clear, precise terms before any data collection or analysis occurs. This makes the hypothesis testable.
  • A hypothesis must be falsifiable. It should be possible, even if unlikely in practice, to collect data that disconfirms rather than supports the hypothesis.
  • Hypotheses guide research. Scientists design studies to explicitly evaluate hypotheses about how nature works.
  • For a hypothesis to be valid, it must be testable against empirical evidence. The evidence can then confirm or disprove the testable predictions.
  • Hypotheses are informed by background knowledge and observation, but go beyond what is already known to propose an explanation of how or why something occurs.
Predictions typically arise from a thorough knowledge of the research literature, curiosity about real-world problems or implications, and integrating this to advance theory. They build on existing literature while providing new insight.

Types of Research Hypotheses

Alternative hypothesis.

The research hypothesis is often called the alternative or experimental hypothesis in experimental research.

It typically suggests a potential relationship between two key variables: the independent variable, which the researcher manipulates, and the dependent variable, which is measured based on those changes.

The alternative hypothesis states a relationship exists between the two variables being studied (one variable affects the other).

A hypothesis is a testable statement or prediction about the relationship between two or more variables. It is a key component of the scientific method. Some key points about hypotheses:

  • Important hypotheses lead to predictions that can be tested empirically. The evidence can then confirm or disprove the testable predictions.

In summary, a hypothesis is a precise, testable statement of what researchers expect to happen in a study and why. Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding.

An experimental hypothesis predicts what change(s) will occur in the dependent variable when the independent variable is manipulated.

It states that the results are not due to chance and are significant in supporting the theory being investigated.

The alternative hypothesis can be directional, indicating a specific direction of the effect, or non-directional, suggesting a difference without specifying its nature. It’s what researchers aim to support or demonstrate through their study.

Null Hypothesis

The null hypothesis states no relationship exists between the two variables being studied (one variable does not affect the other). There will be no changes in the dependent variable due to manipulating the independent variable.

It states results are due to chance and are not significant in supporting the idea being investigated.

The null hypothesis, positing no effect or relationship, is a foundational contrast to the research hypothesis in scientific inquiry. It establishes a baseline for statistical testing, promoting objectivity by initiating research from a neutral stance.

Many statistical methods are tailored to test the null hypothesis, determining the likelihood of observed results if no true effect exists.

This dual-hypothesis approach provides clarity, ensuring that research intentions are explicit, and fosters consistency across scientific studies, enhancing the standardization and interpretability of research outcomes.

Nondirectional Hypothesis

A non-directional hypothesis, also known as a two-tailed hypothesis, predicts that there is a difference or relationship between two variables but does not specify the direction of this relationship.

It merely indicates that a change or effect will occur without predicting which group will have higher or lower values.

For example, “There is a difference in performance between Group A and Group B” is a non-directional hypothesis.

Directional Hypothesis

A directional (one-tailed) hypothesis predicts the nature of the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable. It predicts in which direction the change will take place. (i.e., greater, smaller, less, more)

It specifies whether one variable is greater, lesser, or different from another, rather than just indicating that there’s a difference without specifying its nature.

For example, “Exercise increases weight loss” is a directional hypothesis.

hypothesis

Falsifiability

The Falsification Principle, proposed by Karl Popper , is a way of demarcating science from non-science. It suggests that for a theory or hypothesis to be considered scientific, it must be testable and irrefutable.

Falsifiability emphasizes that scientific claims shouldn’t just be confirmable but should also have the potential to be proven wrong.

It means that there should exist some potential evidence or experiment that could prove the proposition false.

However many confirming instances exist for a theory, it only takes one counter observation to falsify it. For example, the hypothesis that “all swans are white,” can be falsified by observing a black swan.

For Popper, science should attempt to disprove a theory rather than attempt to continually provide evidence to support a research hypothesis.

Can a Hypothesis be Proven?

Hypotheses make probabilistic predictions. They state the expected outcome if a particular relationship exists. However, a study result supporting a hypothesis does not definitively prove it is true.

All studies have limitations. There may be unknown confounding factors or issues that limit the certainty of conclusions. Additional studies may yield different results.

In science, hypotheses can realistically only be supported with some degree of confidence, not proven. The process of science is to incrementally accumulate evidence for and against hypothesized relationships in an ongoing pursuit of better models and explanations that best fit the empirical data. But hypotheses remain open to revision and rejection if that is where the evidence leads.
  • Disproving a hypothesis is definitive. Solid disconfirmatory evidence will falsify a hypothesis and require altering or discarding it based on the evidence.
  • However, confirming evidence is always open to revision. Other explanations may account for the same results, and additional or contradictory evidence may emerge over time.

We can never 100% prove the alternative hypothesis. Instead, we see if we can disprove, or reject the null hypothesis.

If we reject the null hypothesis, this doesn’t mean that our alternative hypothesis is correct but does support the alternative/experimental hypothesis.

Upon analysis of the results, an alternative hypothesis can be rejected or supported, but it can never be proven to be correct. We must avoid any reference to results proving a theory as this implies 100% certainty, and there is always a chance that evidence may exist which could refute a theory.

How to Write a Hypothesis

  • Identify variables . The researcher manipulates the independent variable and the dependent variable is the measured outcome.
  • Operationalized the variables being investigated . Operationalization of a hypothesis refers to the process of making the variables physically measurable or testable, e.g. if you are about to study aggression, you might count the number of punches given by participants.
  • Decide on a direction for your prediction . If there is evidence in the literature to support a specific effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, write a directional (one-tailed) hypothesis. If there are limited or ambiguous findings in the literature regarding the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, write a non-directional (two-tailed) hypothesis.
  • Make it Testable : Ensure your hypothesis can be tested through experimentation or observation. It should be possible to prove it false (principle of falsifiability).
  • Clear & concise language . A strong hypothesis is concise (typically one to two sentences long), and formulated using clear and straightforward language, ensuring it’s easily understood and testable.

Consider a hypothesis many teachers might subscribe to: students work better on Monday morning than on Friday afternoon (IV=Day, DV= Standard of work).

Now, if we decide to study this by giving the same group of students a lesson on a Monday morning and a Friday afternoon and then measuring their immediate recall of the material covered in each session, we would end up with the following:

  • The alternative hypothesis states that students will recall significantly more information on a Monday morning than on a Friday afternoon.
  • The null hypothesis states that there will be no significant difference in the amount recalled on a Monday morning compared to a Friday afternoon. Any difference will be due to chance or confounding factors.

More Examples

  • Memory : Participants exposed to classical music during study sessions will recall more items from a list than those who studied in silence.
  • Social Psychology : Individuals who frequently engage in social media use will report higher levels of perceived social isolation compared to those who use it infrequently.
  • Developmental Psychology : Children who engage in regular imaginative play have better problem-solving skills than those who don’t.
  • Clinical Psychology : Cognitive-behavioral therapy will be more effective in reducing symptoms of anxiety over a 6-month period compared to traditional talk therapy.
  • Cognitive Psychology : Individuals who multitask between various electronic devices will have shorter attention spans on focused tasks than those who single-task.
  • Health Psychology : Patients who practice mindfulness meditation will experience lower levels of chronic pain compared to those who don’t meditate.
  • Organizational Psychology : Employees in open-plan offices will report higher levels of stress than those in private offices.
  • Behavioral Psychology : Rats rewarded with food after pressing a lever will press it more frequently than rats who receive no reward.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

helpful professor logo

Directional Hypothesis: Definition and 10 Examples

directional hypothesis examples and definition, explained below

A directional hypothesis refers to a type of hypothesis used in statistical testing that predicts a particular direction of the expected relationship between two variables.

In simpler terms, a directional hypothesis is an educated, specific guess about the direction of an outcome—whether an increase, decrease, or a proclaimed difference in variable sets.

For example, in a study investigating the effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance, a directional hypothesis might state that as sleep deprivation (Independent Variable) increases, cognitive performance (Dependent Variable) decreases (Killgore, 2010). Such a hypothesis offers a clear, directional relationship whereby a specific increase or decrease is anticipated.

Global warming provides another notable example of a directional hypothesis. A researcher might hypothesize that as carbon dioxide (CO2) levels increase, global temperatures also increase (Thompson, 2010). In this instance, the hypothesis clearly articulates an upward trend for both variables. 

In any given circumstance, it’s imperative that a directional hypothesis is grounded on solid evidence. For instance, the CO2 and global temperature relationship is based on substantial scientific evidence, and not on a random guess or mere speculation (Florides & Christodoulides, 2009).

Directional vs Non-Directional vs Null Hypotheses

A directional hypothesis is generally contrasted to a non-directional hypothesis. Here’s how they compare:

  • Directional hypothesis: A directional hypothesis provides a perspective of the expected relationship between variables, predicting the direction of that relationship (either positive, negative, or a specific difference). 
  • Non-directional hypothesis: A non-directional hypothesis denotes the possibility of a relationship between two variables ( the independent and dependent variables ), although this hypothesis does not venture a prediction as to the direction of this relationship (Ali & Bhaskar, 2016). For example, a non-directional hypothesis might state that there exists a relationship between a person’s diet (independent variable) and their mood (dependent variable), without indicating whether improvement in diet enhances mood positively or negatively. Overall, the choice between a directional or non-directional hypothesis depends on the known or anticipated link between the variables under consideration in research studies.

Another very important type of hypothesis that we need to know about is a null hypothesis :

  • Null hypothesis : The null hypothesis stands as a universality—the hypothesis that there is no observed effect in the population under study, meaning there is no association between variables (or that the differences are down to chance). For instance, a null hypothesis could be constructed around the idea that changing diet (independent variable) has no discernible effect on a person’s mood (dependent variable) (Yan & Su, 2016). This proposition is the one that we aim to disprove in an experiment.

While directional and non-directional hypotheses involve some integrated expectations about the outcomes (either distinct direction or a vague relationship), a null hypothesis operates on the premise of negating such relationships or effects.

The null hypotheses is typically proposed to be negated or disproved by statistical tests, paving way for the acceptance of an alternate hypothesis (either directional or non-directional).

Directional Hypothesis Examples

1. exercise and heart health.

Research suggests that as regular physical exercise (independent variable) increases, the risk of heart disease (dependent variable) decreases (Jakicic, Davis, Rogers, King, Marcus, Helsel, Rickman, Wahed, Belle, 2016). In this example, a directional hypothesis anticipates that the more individuals maintain routine workouts, the lesser would be their odds of developing heart-related disorders. This assumption is based on the underlying fact that routine exercise can help reduce harmful cholesterol levels, regulate blood pressure, and bring about overall health benefits. Thus, a direction – a decrease in heart disease – is expected in relation with an increase in exercise. 

2. Screen Time and Sleep Quality

Another classic instance of a directional hypothesis can be seen in the relationship between the independent variable, screen time (especially before bed), and the dependent variable, sleep quality. This hypothesis predicts that as screen time before bed increases, sleep quality decreases (Chang, Aeschbach, Duffy, Czeisler, 2015). The reasoning behind this hypothesis is the disruptive effect of artificial light (especially blue light from screens) on melatonin production, a hormone needed to regulate sleep. As individuals spend more time exposed to screens before bed, it is predictably hypothesized that their sleep quality worsens. 

3. Job Satisfaction and Employee Turnover

A typical scenario in organizational behavior research posits that as job satisfaction (independent variable) increases, the rate of employee turnover (dependent variable) decreases (Cheng, Jiang, & Riley, 2017). This directional hypothesis emphasizes that an increased level of job satisfaction would lead to a reduced rate of employees leaving the company. The theoretical basis for this hypothesis is that satisfied employees often tend to be more committed to the organization and are less likely to seek employment elsewhere, thus reducing turnover rates.

4. Healthy Eating and Body Weight

Healthy eating, as the independent variable, is commonly thought to influence body weight, the dependent variable, in a positive way. For example, the hypothesis might state that as consumption of healthy foods increases, an individual’s body weight decreases (Framson, Kristal, Schenk, Littman, Zeliadt, & Benitez, 2009). This projection is based on the premise that healthier foods, such as fruits and vegetables, are generally lower in calories than junk food, assisting in weight management.

5. Sun Exposure and Skin Health

The association between sun exposure (independent variable) and skin health (dependent variable) allows for a definitive hypothesis declaring that as sun exposure increases, the risk of skin damage or skin cancer increases (Whiteman, Whiteman, & Green, 2001). The premise aligns with the understanding that overexposure to the sun’s ultraviolet rays can deteriorate skin health, leading to conditions like sunburn or, in extreme cases, skin cancer.

6. Study Hours and Academic Performance

A regularly assessed relationship in academia suggests that as the number of study hours (independent variable) rises, so too does academic performance (dependent variable) (Nonis, Hudson, Logan, Ford, 2013). The hypothesis proposes a positive correlation , with an increase in study time expected to contribute to enhanced academic outcomes.

7. Screen Time and Eye Strain

It’s commonly hypothesized that as screen time (independent variable) increases, the likelihood of experiencing eye strain (dependent variable) also increases (Sheppard & Wolffsohn, 2018). This is based on the idea that prolonged engagement with digital screens—computers, tablets, or mobile phones—can cause discomfort or fatigue in the eyes, attributing to symptoms of eye strain.

8. Physical Activity and Stress Levels

In the sphere of mental health, it’s often proposed that as physical activity (independent variable) increases, levels of stress (dependent variable) decrease (Stonerock, Hoffman, Smith, Blumenthal, 2015). Regular exercise is known to stimulate the production of endorphins, the body’s natural mood elevators, helping to alleviate stress.

9. Water Consumption and Kidney Health

A common health-related hypothesis might predict that as water consumption (independent variable) increases, the risk of kidney stones (dependent variable) decreases (Curhan, Willett, Knight, & Stampfer, 2004). Here, an increase in water intake is inferred to reduce the risk of kidney stones by diluting the substances that lead to stone formation.

10. Traffic Noise and Sleep Quality

In urban planning research, it’s often supposed that as traffic noise (independent variable) increases, sleep quality (dependent variable) decreases (Muzet, 2007). Increased noise levels, particularly during the night, can result in sleep disruptions, thus, leading to poor sleep quality.

11. Sugar Consumption and Dental Health

In the field of dental health, an example might be stating as one’s sugar consumption (independent variable) increases, dental health (dependent variable) decreases (Sheiham, & James, 2014). This stems from the fact that sugar is a major factor in tooth decay, and increased consumption of sugary foods or drinks leads to a decline in dental health due to the high likelihood of cavities.

See 15 More Examples of Hypotheses Here

A directional hypothesis plays a critical role in research, paving the way for specific predicted outcomes based on the relationship between two variables. These hypotheses clearly illuminate the expected direction—the increase or decrease—of an effect. From predicting the impacts of healthy eating on body weight to forecasting the influence of screen time on sleep quality, directional hypotheses allow for targeted and strategic examination of phenomena. In essence, directional hypotheses provide the crucial path for inquiry, shaping the trajectory of research studies and ultimately aiding in the generation of insightful, relevant findings.

Ali, S., & Bhaskar, S. (2016). Basic statistical tools in research and data analysis. Indian Journal of Anaesthesia, 60 (9), 662-669. doi: https://doi.org/10.4103%2F0019-5049.190623  

Chang, A. M., Aeschbach, D., Duffy, J. F., & Czeisler, C. A. (2015). Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 112 (4), 1232-1237. doi: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1418490112  

Cheng, G. H. L., Jiang, D., & Riley, J. H. (2017). Organizational commitment and intrinsic motivation of regular and contractual primary school teachers in China. New Psychology, 19 (3), 316-326. Doi: https://doi.org/10.4103%2F2249-4863.184631  

Curhan, G. C., Willett, W. C., Knight, E. L., & Stampfer, M. J. (2004). Dietary factors and the risk of incident kidney stones in younger women: Nurses’ Health Study II. Archives of Internal Medicine, 164 (8), 885–891.

Florides, G. A., & Christodoulides, P. (2009). Global warming and carbon dioxide through sciences. Environment international , 35 (2), 390-401. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2008.07.007

Framson, C., Kristal, A. R., Schenk, J. M., Littman, A. J., Zeliadt, S., & Benitez, D. (2009). Development and validation of the mindful eating questionnaire. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109 (8), 1439-1444. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2009.05.006  

Jakicic, J. M., Davis, K. K., Rogers, R. J., King, W. C., Marcus, M. D., Helsel, D., … & Belle, S. H. (2016). Effect of wearable technology combined with a lifestyle intervention on long-term weight loss: The IDEA randomized clinical trial. JAMA, 316 (11), 1161-1171.

Khan, S., & Iqbal, N. (2013). Study of the relationship between study habits and academic achievement of students: A case of SPSS model. Higher Education Studies, 3 (1), 14-26.

Killgore, W. D. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Progress in brain research , 185 , 105-129. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5  

Marczinski, C. A., & Fillmore, M. T. (2014). Dissociative antagonistic effects of caffeine on alcohol-induced impairment of behavioral control. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22 (4), 298–311. doi: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/1064-1297.11.3.228  

Muzet, A. (2007). Environmental Noise, Sleep and Health. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 11 (2), 135-142. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2006.09.001  

Nonis, S. A., Hudson, G. I., Logan, L. B., & Ford, C. W. (2013). Influence of perceived control over time on college students’ stress and stress-related outcomes. Research in Higher Education, 54 (5), 536-552. doi: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018753706925  

Sheiham, A., & James, W. P. (2014). A new understanding of the relationship between sugars, dental caries and fluoride use: implications for limits on sugars consumption. Public health nutrition, 17 (10), 2176-2184. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S136898001400113X  

Sheppard, A. L., & Wolffsohn, J. S. (2018). Digital eye strain: prevalence, measurement and amelioration. BMJ open ophthalmology , 3 (1), e000146. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmjophth-2018-000146

Stonerock, G. L., Hoffman, B. M., Smith, P. J., & Blumenthal, J. A. (2015). Exercise as Treatment for Anxiety: Systematic Review and Analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49 (4), 542–556. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9685-9  

Thompson, L. G. (2010). Climate change: The evidence and our options. The Behavior Analyst , 33 , 153-170. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392211  

Whiteman, D. C., Whiteman, C. A., & Green, A. C. (2001). Childhood sun exposure as a risk factor for melanoma: a systematic review of epidemiologic studies. Cancer Causes & Control, 12 (1), 69-82. doi: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008980919928

Yan, X., & Su, X. (2009). Linear regression analysis: theory and computing . New Jersey: World Scientific.

Chris

Chris Drew (PhD)

Dr. Chris Drew is the founder of the Helpful Professor. He holds a PhD in education and has published over 20 articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education. [Image Descriptor: Photo of Chris]

  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd/ 5 Top Tips for Succeeding at University
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd/ 50 Durable Goods Examples
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd/ 100 Consumer Goods Examples
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd/ 30 Globalization Pros and Cons

Leave a Comment Cancel Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Statology

Statistics Made Easy

What is a Directional Hypothesis? (Definition & Examples)

A statistical hypothesis is an assumption about a population parameter . For example, we may assume that the mean height of a male in the U.S. is 70 inches.

The assumption about the height is the statistical hypothesis and the true mean height of a male in the U.S. is the population parameter .

To test whether a statistical hypothesis about a population parameter is true, we obtain a random sample from the population and perform a hypothesis test on the sample data.

Whenever we perform a hypothesis test, we always write down a null and alternative hypothesis:

  • Null Hypothesis (H 0 ): The sample data occurs purely from chance.
  • Alternative Hypothesis (H A ): The sample data is influenced by some non-random cause.

A hypothesis test can either contain a directional hypothesis or a non-directional hypothesis:

  • Directional hypothesis: The alternative hypothesis contains the less than (“<“) or greater than (“>”) sign. This indicates that we’re testing whether or not there is a positive or negative effect.
  • Non-directional hypothesis: The alternative hypothesis contains the not equal (“≠”) sign. This indicates that we’re testing whether or not there is some effect, without specifying the direction of the effect.

Note that directional hypothesis tests are also called “one-tailed” tests and non-directional hypothesis tests are also called “two-tailed” tests.

Check out the following examples to gain a better understanding of directional vs. non-directional hypothesis tests.

Example 1: Baseball Programs

A baseball coach believes a certain 4-week program will increase the mean hitting percentage of his players, which is currently 0.285.

To test this, he measures the hitting percentage of each of his players before and after participating in the program.

He then performs a hypothesis test using the following hypotheses:

  • H 0 : μ = .285 (the program will have no effect on the mean hitting percentage)
  • H A : μ > .285 (the program will cause mean hitting percentage to increase)

This is an example of a directional hypothesis because the alternative hypothesis contains the greater than “>” sign. The coach believes that the program will influence the mean hitting percentage of his players in a positive direction.

Example 2: Plant Growth

A biologist believes that a certain pesticide will cause plants to grow less during a one-month period than they normally do, which is currently 10 inches.

To test this, she applies the pesticide to each of the plants in her laboratory for one month.

She then performs a hypothesis test using the following hypotheses:

  • H 0 : μ = 10 inches (the pesticide will have no effect on the mean plant growth)
  • H A : μ < 10 inches (the pesticide will cause mean plant growth to decrease)

This is also an example of a directional hypothesis because the alternative hypothesis contains the less than “<” sign. The biologist believes that the pesticide will influence the mean plant growth in a negative direction.

Example 3: Studying Technique

A professor believes that a certain studying technique will influence the mean score that her students receive on a certain exam, but she’s unsure if it will increase or decrease the mean score, which is currently 82.

To test this, she lets each student use the studying technique for one month leading up to the exam and then administers the same exam to each of the students.

  • H 0 : μ = 82 (the studying technique will have no effect on the mean exam score)
  • H A : μ ≠ 82 (the studying technique will cause the mean exam score to be different than 82)

This is an example of a non-directional hypothesis because the alternative hypothesis contains the not equal “≠” sign. The professor believes that the studying technique will influence the mean exam score, but doesn’t specify whether it will cause the mean score to increase or decrease.

Additional Resources

Introduction to Hypothesis Testing Introduction to the One Sample t-test Introduction to the Two Sample t-test Introduction to the Paired Samples t-test

' src=

Published by Zach

Leave a reply cancel reply.

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

psychologyrocks

Hypotheses; directional and non-directional, what is the difference between an experimental and an alternative hypothesis.

Nothing much! If the study is a laboratory experiment then we can call the hypothesis “an experimental hypothesis”, where we make a prediction about how the IV causes an effect on the DV. If we have a non-experimental design, i.e. we are not able to manipulate the IV as in a natural or quasi-experiment , or if some other research method has been used, then we call it an “alternativehypothesis”, alternative to the null.

Directional hypothesis: A directional (or one tailed hypothesis) states which way you think the results are going to go, for example in an experimental study we might say…”Participants who have been deprived of sleep for 24 hours will have more cold symptoms in the following week after exposure to a virus than participants who have not been sleep deprived”; the hypothesis compares the two groups/conditions and states which one will ….have more/less, be quicker/slower, etc.

If we had a correlational study, the directional hypothesis would state whether we expect a positive or a negative correlation, we are stating how the two variables will be related to each other, e.g. there will be a positive correlation between the number of stressful life events experienced in the last year and the number of coughs and colds suffered, whereby the more life events you have suffered the more coughs and cold you will have had”. The directional hypothesis can also state a negative correlation, e.g. the higher the number of face-book friends, the lower the life satisfaction score “

Non-directional hypothesis: A non-directional (or two tailed hypothesis) simply states that there will be a difference between the two groups/conditions but does not say which will be greater/smaller, quicker/slower etc. Using our example above we would say “There will be a difference between the number of cold symptoms experienced in the following week after exposure to a virus for those participants who have been sleep deprived for 24 hours compared with those who have not been sleep deprived for 24 hours.”

When the study is correlational, we simply state that variables will be correlated but do not state whether the relationship will be positive or negative, e.g. there will be a significant correlation between variable A and variable B.

Null hypothesis The null hypothesis states that the alternative or experimental hypothesis is NOT the case, if your experimental hypothesis was directional you would say…

Participants who have been deprived of sleep for 24 hours will NOT have more cold symptoms in the following week after exposure to a virus than participants who have not been sleep deprived and any difference that does arise will be due to chance alone.

or with a directional correlational hypothesis….

There will NOT be a positive correlation between the number of stress life events experienced in the last year and the number of coughs and colds suffered, whereby the more life events you have suffered the more coughs and cold you will have had”

With a non-directional or  two tailed hypothesis…

There will be NO difference between the number of cold symptoms experienced in the following week after exposure to a virus for those participants who have been sleep deprived for 24 hours compared with those who have not been sleep deprived for 24 hours.

or for a correlational …

there will be NO correlation between variable A and variable B.

When it comes to conducting an inferential stats test, if you have a directional hypothesis , you must do a one tailed test to find out whether your observed value is significant. If you have a non-directional hypothesis , you must do a two tailed test .

Exam Techniques/Advice

  • Remember, a decent hypothesis will contain two variables, in the case of an experimental hypothesis there will be an IV and a DV; in a correlational hypothesis there will be two co-variables
  • both variables need to be fully operationalised to score the marks, that is you need to be very clear and specific about what you mean by your IV and your DV; if someone wanted to repeat your study, they should be able to look at your hypothesis and know exactly what to change between the two groups/conditions and exactly what to measure (including any units/explanation of rating scales etc, e.g. “where 1 is low and 7 is high”)
  • double check the question, did it ask for a directional or non-directional hypothesis?
  • if you were asked for a null hypothesis, make sure you always include the phrase “and any difference/correlation (is your study experimental or correlational?) that does arise will be due to chance alone”

Practice Questions:

  • Mr Faraz wants to compare the levels of attendance between his psychology group and those of Mr Simon, who teaches a different psychology group. Which of the following is a suitable directional (one tailed) hypothesis for Mr Faraz’s investigation?

A There will be a difference in the levels of attendance between the two psychology groups.

B Students’ level of attendance will be higher in Mr Faraz’s group than Mr Simon’s group.

C Any difference in the levels of attendance between the two psychology groups is due to chance.

D The level of attendance of the students will depend upon who is teaching the groups.

2. Tracy works for the local council. The council is thinking about reducing the number of people it employs to pick up litter from the street. Tracy has been asked to carry out a study to see if having the streets cleaned at less regular intervals will affect the amount of litter the public will drop. She studies a street to compare how much litter is dropped at two different times, once when it has just been cleaned and once after it has not been cleaned for a month.

Write a fully operationalised non-directional (two-tailed) hypothesis for Tracy’s study. (2)

3. Jamila is conducting a practical investigation to look at gender differences in carrying out visuo-spatial tasks. She decides to give males and females a jigsaw puzzle and will time them to see who completes it the fastest. She uses a random sample of pupils from a local school to get her participants.

(a) Write a fully operationalised directional (one tailed) hypothesis for Jamila’s study. (2) (b) Outline one strength and one weakness of the random sampling method. You may refer to Jamila’s use of this type of sampling in your answer. (4)

4. Which of the following is a non-directional (two tailed) hypothesis?

A There is a difference in driving ability with men being better drivers than women

B Women are better at concentrating on more than one thing at a time than men

C Women spend more time doing the cooking and cleaning than men

D There is a difference in the number of men and women who participate in sports

Revision Activity

writing-hypotheses-revision-sheet

Quizizz link for teachers: https://quizizz.com/admin/quiz/5bf03f51add785001bc5a09e

Share this:

' src=

  • Already have a WordPress.com account? Log in now.
  • Subscribe Subscribed
  • Copy shortlink
  • Report this content
  • View post in Reader
  • Manage subscriptions
  • Collapse this bar

Providing a study guide and revision resources for students and psychology teaching resources for teachers.

Aims And Hypotheses, Directional And Non-Directional

March 7, 2021 - paper 2 psychology in context | research methods.

  • Back to Paper 2 - Research Methods

In Psychology, hypotheses are predictions made by the researcher about the outcome of a study. The research can chose to make a specific prediction about what they feel will happen in their research (a directional hypothesis) or they can make a ‘general,’ ‘less specific’ prediction about the outcome of their research (a non-directional hypothesis). The type of prediction that a researcher makes is usually dependent on whether or not any previous research has also investigated their research aim.

Variables Recap:

The  independent variable  (IV)  is the variable that psychologists  manipulate/change  to see if changing this variable has an effect on the  depen dent variable  (DV).

The  dependent variable (DV)  is the variable that the psychologists  measures  (to see if the IV has had an effect).

It is important that the only variable that is changed in research is the  independent variable (IV),   all other variables have to be kept constant across the control condition and the experimental conditions. Only then will researchers be able to observe the true effects of  just  the independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV).

Research/Experimental Aim(S):

Aim

An aim is a clear and precise statement of the purpose of the study. It is a statement of why a research study is taking place. This should include what is being studied and what the study is trying to achieve. (e.g. “This study aims to investigate the effects of alcohol on reaction times”.

It is important that aims created in research are realistic and ethical.

Hypotheses:

This is a testable statement that predicts what the researcher expects to happen in their research. The research study itself is therefore a means of testing whether or not the hypothesis is supported by the findings. If the findings do support the hypothesis then the hypothesis can be retained (i.e., accepted), but if not, then it must be rejected.

Three Different Hypotheses:

Bitcoin-Price-Prediction-300x201

We're not around right now. But you can send us an email and we'll get back to you, asap.

Start typing and press Enter to search

Cookie Policy - Terms and Conditions - Privacy Policy

Directional and non-directional hypothesis: A Comprehensive Guide

Karolina Konopka

Customer support manager

Karolina Konopka

In the world of research and statistical analysis, hypotheses play a crucial role in formulating and testing scientific claims. Understanding the differences between directional and non-directional hypothesis is essential for designing sound experiments and drawing accurate conclusions. Whether you’re a student, researcher, or simply curious about the foundations of hypothesis testing, this guide will equip you with the knowledge and tools to navigate this fundamental aspect of scientific inquiry.

Understanding Directional Hypothesis

Understanding directional hypotheses is crucial for conducting hypothesis-driven research, as they guide the selection of appropriate statistical tests and aid in the interpretation of results. By incorporating directional hypotheses, researchers can make more precise predictions, contribute to scientific knowledge, and advance their fields of study.

Definition of directional hypothesis

Directional hypotheses, also known as one-tailed hypotheses, are statements in research that make specific predictions about the direction of a relationship or difference between variables. Unlike non-directional hypotheses, which simply state that there is a relationship or difference without specifying its direction, directional hypotheses provide a focused and precise expectation.

A directional hypothesis predicts either a positive or negative relationship between variables or predicts that one group will perform better than another. It asserts a specific direction of effect or outcome. For example, a directional hypothesis could state that “increased exposure to sunlight will lead to an improvement in mood” or “participants who receive the experimental treatment will exhibit higher levels of cognitive performance compared to the control group.”

Directional hypotheses are formulated based on existing theory, prior research, or logical reasoning, and they guide the researcher’s expectations and analysis. They allow for more targeted predictions and enable researchers to test specific hypotheses using appropriate statistical tests.

The role of directional hypothesis in research

Directional hypotheses also play a significant role in research surveys. Let’s explore their role specifically in the context of survey research:

  • Objective-driven surveys : Directional hypotheses help align survey research with specific objectives. By formulating directional hypotheses, researchers can focus on gathering data that directly addresses the predicted relationship or difference between variables of interest.
  • Question design and measurement : Directional hypotheses guide the design of survey question types and the selection of appropriate measurement scales. They ensure that the questions are tailored to capture the specific aspects related to the predicted direction, enabling researchers to obtain more targeted and relevant data from survey respondents.
  • Data analysis and interpretation : Directional hypotheses assist in data analysis by directing researchers towards appropriate statistical tests and methods. Researchers can analyze the survey data to specifically test the predicted relationship or difference, enhancing the accuracy and reliability of their findings. The results can then be interpreted within the context of the directional hypothesis, providing more meaningful insights.
  • Practical implications and decision-making : Directional hypotheses in surveys often have practical implications. When the predicted relationship or difference is confirmed, it informs decision-making processes, program development, or interventions. The survey findings based on directional hypotheses can guide organizations, policymakers, or practitioners in making informed choices to achieve desired outcomes.
  • Replication and further research : Directional hypotheses in survey research contribute to the replication and extension of studies. Researchers can replicate the survey with different populations or contexts to assess the generalizability of the predicted relationships. Furthermore, if the directional hypothesis is supported, it encourages further research to explore underlying mechanisms or boundary conditions.

By incorporating directional hypotheses in survey research, researchers can align their objectives, design effective surveys, conduct focused data analysis, and derive practical insights. They provide a framework for organizing survey research and contribute to the accumulation of knowledge in the field.

Examples of research questions for directional hypothesis

Here are some examples of research questions that lend themselves to directional hypotheses:

  • Does increased daily exercise lead to a decrease in body weight among sedentary adults?
  • Is there a positive relationship between study hours and academic performance among college students?
  • Does exposure to violent video games result in an increase in aggressive behavior among adolescents?
  • Does the implementation of a mindfulness-based intervention lead to a reduction in stress levels among working professionals?
  • Is there a difference in customer satisfaction between Product A and Product B, with Product A expected to have higher satisfaction ratings?
  • Does the use of social media influence self-esteem levels, with higher social media usage associated with lower self-esteem?
  • Is there a negative relationship between job satisfaction and employee turnover, indicating that lower job satisfaction leads to higher turnover rates?
  • Does the administration of a specific medication result in a decrease in symptoms among individuals with a particular medical condition?
  • Does increased access to early childhood education lead to improved cognitive development in preschool-aged children?
  • Is there a difference in purchase intention between advertisements with celebrity endorsements and advertisements without, with celebrity endorsements expected to have a higher impact?

These research questions generate specific predictions about the direction of the relationship or difference between variables and can be tested using appropriate research methods and statistical analyses.

Definition of non-directional hypothesis

Non-directional hypotheses, also known as two-tailed hypotheses, are statements in research that indicate the presence of a relationship or difference between variables without specifying the direction of the effect. Instead of making predictions about the specific direction of the relationship or difference, non-directional hypotheses simply state that there is an association or distinction between the variables of interest.

Non-directional hypotheses are often used when there is no prior theoretical basis or clear expectation about the direction of the relationship. They leave the possibility open for either a positive or negative relationship, or for both groups to differ in some way without specifying which group will perform better or worse.

Advantages and utility of non-directional hypothesis

Non-directional hypotheses in survey s offer several advantages and utilities, providing flexibility and comprehensive analysis of survey data. Here are some of the key advantages and utilities of using non-directional hypotheses in surveys:

  • Exploration of Relationships : Non-directional hypotheses allow researchers to explore and examine relationships between variables without assuming a specific direction. This is particularly useful in surveys where the relationship between variables may not be well-known or there may be conflicting evidence regarding the direction of the effect.
  • Flexibility in Question Design : With non-directional hypotheses, survey questions can be designed to measure the relationship between variables without being biased towards a particular outcome. This flexibility allows researchers to collect data and analyze the results more objectively.
  • Open to Unexpected Findings : Non-directional hypotheses enable researchers to be open to unexpected or surprising findings in survey data. By not committing to a specific direction of the effect, researchers can identify and explore relationships that may not have been initially anticipated, leading to new insights and discoveries.
  • Comprehensive Analysis : Non-directional hypotheses promote comprehensive analysis of survey data by considering the possibility of an effect in either direction. Researchers can assess the magnitude and significance of relationships without limiting their analysis to only one possible outcome.
  • S tatistical Validity : Non-directional hypotheses in surveys allow for the use of two-tailed statistical tests, which provide a more conservative and robust assessment of significance. Two-tailed tests consider both positive and negative deviations from the null hypothesis, ensuring accurate and reliable statistical analysis of survey data.
  • Exploratory Research : Non-directional hypotheses are particularly useful in exploratory research, where the goal is to gather initial insights and generate hypotheses. Surveys with non-directional hypotheses can help researchers explore various relationships and identify patterns that can guide further research or hypothesis development.

It is worth noting that the choice between directional and non-directional hypotheses in surveys depends on the research objectives, existing knowledge, and the specific variables being investigated. Researchers should carefully consider the advantages and limitations of each approach and select the one that aligns best with their research goals and survey design.

  • Share with others
  • Twitter Twitter Icon
  • LinkedIn LinkedIn Icon

Related posts

How to implement nps surveys: a step-by-step guide, 15 best website survey questions to ask your visitors, how to write a good survey introduction, 7 best ai survey generators, multiple choice questions: types, examples & samples, how to make a gdpr compliant survey, get answers today.

  • No credit card required
  • No time limit on Free plan

You can modify this template in every possible way.

All templates work great on every device.

Logo for Maricopa Open Digital Press

9 Chapter 9 Hypothesis testing

The first unit was designed to prepare you for hypothesis testing. In the first chapter we discussed the three major goals of statistics:

  • Describe: connects to unit 1 with descriptive statistics and graphing
  • Decide: connects to unit 1 knowing your data and hypothesis testing
  • Predict: connects to hypothesis testing and unit 3

The remaining chapters will cover many different kinds of hypothesis tests connected to different inferential statistics. Needless to say, hypothesis testing is the central topic of this course. This lesson is important but that does not mean the same thing as difficult. There is a lot of new language we will learn about when conducting a hypothesis test. Some of the components of a hypothesis test are the topics we are already familiar with:

  • Test statistics
  • Probability
  • Distribution of sample means

Hypothesis testing is an inferential procedure that uses data from a sample to draw a general conclusion about a population. It is a formal approach and a statistical method that uses sample data to evaluate hypotheses about a population. When interpreting a research question and statistical results, a natural question arises as to whether the finding could have occurred by chance. Hypothesis testing is a statistical procedure for testing whether chance (random events) is a reasonable explanation of an experimental finding. Once you have mastered the material in this lesson you will be used to solving hypothesis testing problems and the rest of the course will seem much easier. In this chapter, we will introduce the ideas behind the use of statistics to make decisions – in particular, decisions about whether a particular hypothesis is supported by the data.

Logic and Purpose of Hypothesis Testing

The statistician Ronald Fisher explained the concept of hypothesis testing with a story of a lady tasting tea. Fisher was a statistician from London and is noted as the first person to formalize the process of hypothesis testing. His elegantly simple “Lady Tasting Tea” experiment demonstrated the logic of the hypothesis test.

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Figure 1. A depiction of the lady tasting tea Photo Credit

Fisher would often have afternoon tea during his studies. He usually took tea with a woman who claimed to be a tea expert. In particular, she told Fisher that she could tell which was poured first in the teacup, the milk or the tea, simply by tasting the cup. Fisher, being a scientist, decided to put this rather bizarre claim to the test. The lady accepted his challenge. Fisher brought her 8 cups of tea in succession; 4 cups would be prepared with the milk added first, and 4 with the tea added first. The cups would be presented in a random order unknown to the lady.

The lady would take a sip of each cup as it was presented and report which ingredient she believed was poured first. Using the laws of probability, Fisher determined the chances of her guessing all 8 cups correctly was 1/70, or about 1.4%. In other words, if the lady was indeed guessing there was a 1.4% chance of her getting all 8 cups correct. On the day of the experiment, Fisher had 8 cups prepared just as he had requested. The lady drank each cup and made her decisions for each one.

After the experiment, it was revealed that the lady got all 8 cups correct! Remember, had she been truly guessing, the chance of getting this result was 1.4%. Since this probability was so low , Fisher instead concluded that the lady could indeed differentiate between the milk or the tea being poured first. Fisher’s original hypothesis that she was just guessing was demonstrated to be false and was therefore rejected. The alternative hypothesis, that the lady could truly tell the cups apart, was then accepted as true.

This story demonstrates many components of hypothesis testing in a very simple way. For example, Fisher started with a hypothesis that the lady was guessing. He then determined that if she was indeed guessing, the probability of guessing all 8 right was very small, just 1.4%. Since that probability was so tiny, when she did get all 8 cups right, Fisher determined it was extremely unlikely she was guessing. A more reasonable conclusion was that the lady had the skill to tell the cups apart.

In hypothesis testing, we will always set up a particular hypothesis that we want to demonstrate to be true. We then use probability to determine the likelihood of our hypothesis is correct. If it appears our original hypothesis was wrong, we reject it and accept the alternative hypothesis. The alternative hypothesis is usually the opposite of our original hypothesis. In Fisher’s case, his original hypothesis was that the lady was guessing. His alternative hypothesis was the lady was not guessing.

This result does not prove that he does; it could be he was just lucky and guessed right 13 out of 16 times. But how plausible is the explanation that he was just lucky? To assess its plausibility, we determine the probability that someone who was just guessing would be correct 13/16 times or more. This probability can be computed to be 0.0106. This is a pretty low probability, and therefore someone would have to be very lucky to be correct 13 or more times out of 16 if they were just guessing. A low probability gives us more confidence there is evidence Bond can tell whether the drink was shaken or stirred. There is also still a chance that Mr. Bond was very lucky (more on this later!). The hypothesis that he was guessing is not proven false, but considerable doubt is cast on it. Therefore, there is strong evidence that Mr. Bond can tell whether a drink was shaken or stirred.

You may notice some patterns here:

  • We have 2 hypotheses: the original (researcher prediction) and the alternative
  • We collect data
  • We determine how likley or unlikely the original hypothesis is to occur based on probability.
  • We determine if we have enough evidence to support the original hypothesis and draw conclusions.

Now let’s being in some specific terminology:

Null hypothesis : In general, the null hypothesis, written H 0 (“H-naught”), is the idea that nothing is going on: there is no effect of our treatment, no relation between our variables, and no difference in our sample mean from what we expected about the population mean. The null hypothesis indicates that an apparent effect is due to chance. This is always our baseline starting assumption, and it is what we (typically) seek to reject . For mathematical notation, one uses =).

Alternative hypothesis : If the null hypothesis is rejected, then we will need some other explanation, which we call the alternative hypothesis, H A or H 1 . The alternative hypothesis is simply the reverse of the null hypothesis. Thus, our alternative hypothesis is the mathematical way of stating our research question.  In general, the alternative hypothesis (also called the research hypothesis)is there is an effect of treatment, the relation between variables, or differences in a sample mean compared to a population mean. The alternative hypothesis essentially shows evidence the findings are not due to chance.  It is also called the research hypothesis as this is the most common outcome a researcher is looking for: evidence of change, differences, or relationships. There are three options for setting up the alternative hypothesis, depending on where we expect the difference to lie. The alternative hypothesis always involves some kind of inequality (≠not equal, >, or <).

  • If we expect a specific direction of change/differences/relationships, which we call a directional hypothesis , then our alternative hypothesis takes the form based on the research question itself.  One would expect a decrease in depression from taking an anti-depressant as a specific directional hypothesis.  Or the direction could be larger, where for example, one might expect an increase in exam scores after completing a student success exam preparation module.  The directional hypothesis (2 directions) makes up 2 of the 3 alternative hypothesis options.  The other alternative is to state there are differences/changes, or a relationship but not predict the direction.  We use a non-directional alternative hypothesis  (typically see ≠ for mathematical notation).

Probability value (p-value) : the probability of a certain outcome assuming a certain state of the world. In statistics, it is conventional to refer to possible states of the world as hypotheses since they are hypothesized states of the world. Using this terminology, the probability value is the probability of an outcome given the hypothesis. It is not the probability of the hypothesis given the outcome. It is very important to understand precisely what the probability values mean. In the James Bond example, the computed probability of 0.0106 is the probability he would be correct on 13 or more taste tests (out of 16) if he were just guessing. It is easy to mistake this probability of 0.0106 as the probability he cannot tell the difference. This is not at all what it means. The probability of 0.0106 is the probability of a certain outcome (13 or more out of 16) assuming a certain state of the world (James Bond was only guessing).

A low probability value casts doubt on the null hypothesis. How low must the probability value be in order to conclude that the null hypothesis is false? Although there is clearly no right or wrong answer to this question, it is conventional to conclude the null hypothesis is false if the probability value is less than 0.05 (p < .05). More conservative researchers conclude the null hypothesis is false only if the probability value is less than 0.01 (p<.01). When a researcher concludes that the null hypothesis is false, the researcher is said to have rejected the null hypothesis. The probability value below which the null hypothesis is rejected is called the α level or simply α (“alpha”). It is also called the significance level . If α is not explicitly specified, assume that α = 0.05.

Decision-making is part of the process and we have some language that goes along with that. Importantly, null hypothesis testing operates under the assumption that the null hypothesis is true unless the evidence shows otherwise. We (typically) seek to reject the null hypothesis, giving us evidence to support the alternative hypothesis .  If the probability of the outcome given the hypothesis is sufficiently low, we have evidence that the null hypothesis is false. Note that all probability calculations for all hypothesis tests center on the null hypothesis. In the James Bond example, the null hypothesis is that he cannot tell the difference between shaken and stirred martinis. The probability value is low that one is able to identify 13 of 16 martinis as shaken or stirred (0.0106), thus providing evidence that he can tell the difference. Note that we have not computed the probability that he can tell the difference.

The specific type of hypothesis testing reviewed is specifically known as null hypothesis statistical testing (NHST). We can break the process of null hypothesis testing down into a number of steps a researcher would use.

  • Formulate a hypothesis that embodies our prediction ( before seeing the data )
  • Specify null and alternative hypotheses
  • Collect some data relevant to the hypothesis
  • Compute a test statistic
  • Identify the criteria probability (or compute the probability of the observed value of that statistic) assuming that the null hypothesis is true
  • Drawing conclusions. Assess the “statistical significance” of the result

Steps in hypothesis testing

Step 1: formulate a hypothesis of interest.

The researchers hypothesized that physicians spend less time with obese patients. The researchers hypothesis derived from an identified population. In creating a research hypothesis, we also have to decide whether we want to test a directional or non-directional hypotheses. Researchers typically will select a non-directional hypothesis for a more conservative approach, particularly when the outcome is unknown (more about why this is later).

Step 2: Specify the null and alternative hypotheses

Can you set up the null and alternative hypotheses for the Physician’s Reaction Experiment?

Step 3: Determine the alpha level.

For this course, alpha will be given to you as .05 or .01.  Researchers will decide on alpha and then determine the associated test statistic based from the sample. Researchers in the Physician Reaction study might set the alpha at .05 and identify the test statistics associated with the .05 for the sample size.  Researchers might take extra precautions to be more confident in their findings (more on this later).

Step 4: Collect some data

For this course, the data will be given to you.  Researchers collect the data and then start to summarize it using descriptive statistics. The mean time physicians reported that they would spend with obese patients was 24.7 minutes as compared to a mean of 31.4 minutes for normal-weight patients.

Step 5: Compute a test statistic

We next want to use the data to compute a statistic that will ultimately let us decide whether the null hypothesis is rejected or not. We can think of the test statistic as providing a measure of the size of the effect compared to the variability in the data. In general, this test statistic will have a probability distribution associated with it, because that allows us to determine how likely our observed value of the statistic is under the null hypothesis.

To assess the plausibility of the hypothesis that the difference in mean times is due to chance, we compute the probability of getting a difference as large or larger than the observed difference (31.4 – 24.7 = 6.7 minutes) if the difference were, in fact, due solely to chance.

Step 6: Determine the probability of the observed result under the null hypothesis 

Using methods presented in later chapters, this probability associated with the observed differences between the two groups for the Physician’s Reaction was computed to be 0.0057. Since this is such a low probability, we have confidence that the difference in times is due to the patient’s weight (obese or not) (and is not due to chance). We can then reject the null hypothesis (there are no differences or differences seen are due to chance).

Keep in mind that the null hypothesis is typically the opposite of the researcher’s hypothesis. In the Physicians’ Reactions study, the researchers hypothesized that physicians would expect to spend less time with obese patients. The null hypothesis that the two types of patients are treated identically as part of the researcher’s control of other variables. If the null hypothesis were true, a difference as large or larger than the sample difference of 6.7 minutes would be very unlikely to occur. Therefore, the researchers rejected the null hypothesis of no difference and concluded that in the population, physicians intend to spend less time with obese patients.

This is the step where NHST starts to violate our intuition. Rather than determining the likelihood that the null hypothesis is true given the data, we instead determine the likelihood under the null hypothesis of observing a statistic at least as extreme as one that we have observed — because we started out by assuming that the null hypothesis is true! To do this, we need to know the expected probability distribution for the statistic under the null hypothesis, so that we can ask how likely the result would be under that distribution. This will be determined from a table we use for reference or calculated in a statistical analysis program. Note that when I say “how likely the result would be”, what I really mean is “how likely the observed result or one more extreme would be”. We need to add this caveat as we are trying to determine how weird our result would be if the null hypothesis were true, and any result that is more extreme will be even more weird, so we want to count all of those weirder possibilities when we compute the probability of our result under the null hypothesis.

Let’s review some considerations for Null hypothesis statistical testing (NHST)!

Null hypothesis statistical testing (NHST) is commonly used in many fields. If you pick up almost any scientific or biomedical research publication, you will see NHST being used to test hypotheses, and in their introductory psychology textbook, Gerrig & Zimbardo (2002) referred to NHST as the “backbone of psychological research”. Thus, learning how to use and interpret the results from hypothesis testing is essential to understand the results from many fields of research.

It is also important for you to know, however, that NHST is flawed, and that many statisticians and researchers think that it has been the cause of serious problems in science, which we will discuss in further in this unit. NHST is also widely misunderstood, largely because it violates our intuitions about how statistical hypothesis testing should work. Let’s look at an example to see this.

There is great interest in the use of body-worn cameras by police officers, which are thought to reduce the use of force and improve officer behavior. However, in order to establish this we need experimental evidence, and it has become increasingly common for governments to use randomized controlled trials to test such ideas. A randomized controlled trial of the effectiveness of body-worn cameras was performed by the Washington, DC government and DC Metropolitan Police Department in 2015-2016. Officers were randomly assigned to wear a body-worn camera or not, and their behavior was then tracked over time to determine whether the cameras resulted in less use of force and fewer civilian complaints about officer behavior.

Before we get to the results, let’s ask how you would think the statistical analysis might work. Let’s say we want to specifically test the hypothesis of whether the use of force is decreased by the wearing of cameras. The randomized controlled trial provides us with the data to test the hypothesis – namely, the rates of use of force by officers assigned to either the camera or control groups. The next obvious step is to look at the data and determine whether they provide convincing evidence for or against this hypothesis. That is: What is the likelihood that body-worn cameras reduce the use of force, given the data and everything else we know?

It turns out that this is not how null hypothesis testing works. Instead, we first take our hypothesis of interest (i.e. that body-worn cameras reduce use of force), and flip it on its head, creating a null hypothesis – in this case, the null hypothesis would be that cameras do not reduce use of force. Importantly, we then assume that the null hypothesis is true. We then look at the data, and determine how likely the data would be if the null hypothesis were true. If the data are sufficiently unlikely under the null hypothesis that we can reject the null in favor of the alternative hypothesis which is our hypothesis of interest. If there is not sufficient evidence to reject the null, then we say that we retain (or “fail to reject”) the null, sticking with our initial assumption that the null is true.

Understanding some of the concepts of NHST, particularly the notorious “p-value”, is invariably challenging the first time one encounters them, because they are so counter-intuitive. As we will see later, there are other approaches that provide a much more intuitive way to address hypothesis testing (but have their own complexities).

Step 7: Assess the “statistical significance” of the result. Draw conclusions.

The next step is to determine whether the p-value that results from the previous step is small enough that we are willing to reject the null hypothesis and conclude instead that the alternative is true. In the Physicians Reactions study, the probability value is 0.0057. Therefore, the effect of obesity is statistically significant and the null hypothesis that obesity makes no difference is rejected. It is very important to keep in mind that statistical significance means only that the null hypothesis of exactly no effect is rejected; it does not mean that the effect is important, which is what “significant” usually means. When an effect is significant, you can have confidence the effect is not exactly zero. Finding that an effect is significant does not tell you about how large or important the effect is.

How much evidence do we require and what considerations are needed to better understand the significance of the findings? This is one of the most controversial questions in statistics, in part because it requires a subjective judgment – there is no “correct” answer.

What does a statistically significant result mean?

There is a great deal of confusion about what p-values actually mean (Gigerenzer, 2004). Let’s say that we do an experiment comparing the means between conditions, and we find a difference with a p-value of .01. There are a number of possible interpretations that one might entertain.

Does it mean that the probability of the null hypothesis being true is .01? No. Remember that in null hypothesis testing, the p-value is the probability of the data given the null hypothesis. It does not warrant conclusions about the probability of the null hypothesis given the data.

Does it mean that the probability that you are making the wrong decision is .01? No. Remember as above that p-values are probabilities of data under the null, not probabilities of hypotheses.

Does it mean that if you ran the study again, you would obtain the same result 99% of the time? No. The p-value is a statement about the likelihood of a particular dataset under the null; it does not allow us to make inferences about the likelihood of future events such as replication.

Does it mean that you have found a practially important effect? No. There is an essential distinction between statistical significance and practical significance . As an example, let’s say that we performed a randomized controlled trial to examine the effect of a particular diet on body weight, and we find a statistically significant effect at p<.05. What this doesn’t tell us is how much weight was actually lost, which we refer to as the effect size (to be discussed in more detail). If we think about a study of weight loss, then we probably don’t think that the loss of one ounce (i.e. the weight of a few potato chips) is practically significant. Let’s look at our ability to detect a significant difference of 1 ounce as the sample size increases.

A statistically significant result is not necessarily a strong one. Even a very weak result can be statistically significant if it is based on a large enough sample. This is why it is important to distinguish between the statistical significance of a result and the practical significance of that result. Practical significance refers to the importance or usefulness of the result in some real-world context and is often referred to as the effect size .

Many differences are statistically significant—and may even be interesting for purely scientific reasons—but they are not practically significant. In clinical practice, this same concept is often referred to as “clinical significance.” For example, a study on a new treatment for social phobia might show that it produces a statistically significant positive effect. Yet this effect still might not be strong enough to justify the time, effort, and other costs of putting it into practice—especially if easier and cheaper treatments that work almost as well already exist. Although statistically significant, this result would be said to lack practical or clinical significance.

Be aware that the term effect size can be misleading because it suggests a causal relationship—that the difference between the two means is an “effect” of being in one group or condition as opposed to another. In other words, simply calling the difference an “effect size” does not make the relationship a causal one.

Figure 1 shows how the proportion of significant results increases as the sample size increases, such that with a very large sample size (about 262,000 total subjects), we will find a significant result in more than 90% of studies when there is a 1 ounce difference in weight loss between the diets. While these are statistically significant, most physicians would not consider a weight loss of one ounce to be practically or clinically significant. We will explore this relationship in more detail when we return to the concept of statistical power in Chapter X, but it should already be clear from this example that statistical significance is not necessarily indicative of practical significance.

The proportion of signifcant results for a very small change (1 ounce, which is about .001 standard deviations) as a function of sample size.

Figure 1: The proportion of significant results for a very small change (1 ounce, which is about .001 standard deviations) as a function of sample size.

Challenges with using p-values

Historically, the most common answer to this question has been that we should reject the null hypothesis if the p-value is less than 0.05. This comes from the writings of Ronald Fisher, who has been referred to as “the single most important figure in 20th century statistics” (Efron, 1998 ) :

“If P is between .1 and .9 there is certainly no reason to suspect the hypothesis tested. If it is below .02 it is strongly indicated that the hypothesis fails to account for the whole of the facts. We shall not often be astray if we draw a conventional line at .05 … it is convenient to draw the line at about the level at which we can say: Either there is something in the treatment, or a coincidence has occurred such as does not occur more than once in twenty trials” (Fisher, 1925 )

Fisher never intended p<0.05p < 0.05 to be a fixed rule:

“no scientific worker has a fixed level of significance at which from year to year, and in all circumstances, he rejects hypotheses; he rather gives his mind to each particular case in the light of his evidence and his ideas” (Fisher, 1956 )

Instead, it is likely that p < .05 became a ritual due to the reliance upon tables of p-values that were used before computing made it easy to compute p values for arbitrary values of a statistic. All of the tables had an entry for 0.05, making it easy to determine whether one’s statistic exceeded the value needed to reach that level of significance. Although we use tables in this class, statistical software examines the specific probability value for the calculated statistic.

Assessing Error Rate: Type I and Type II Error

Although there are challenges with p-values for decision making, we will examine a way we can think about hypothesis testing in terms of its error rate.  This was proposed by Jerzy Neyman and Egon Pearson:

“no test based upon a theory of probability can by itself provide any valuable evidence of the truth or falsehood of a hypothesis. But we may look at the purpose of tests from another viewpoint. Without hoping to know whether each separate hypothesis is true or false, we may search for rules to govern our behaviour with regard to them, in following which we insure that, in the long run of experience, we shall not often be wrong” (Neyman & Pearson, 1933 )

That is: We can’t know which specific decisions are right or wrong, but if we follow the rules, we can at least know how often our decisions will be wrong in the long run.

To understand the decision-making framework that Neyman and Pearson developed, we first need to discuss statistical decision-making in terms of the kinds of outcomes that can occur. There are two possible states of reality (H0 is true, or H0 is false), and two possible decisions (reject H0, or retain H0). There are two ways in which we can make a correct decision:

  • We can reject H0 when it is false (in the language of signal detection theory, we call this a hit )
  • We can retain H0 when it is true (somewhat confusingly in this context, this is called a correct rejection )

There are also two kinds of errors we can make:

  • We can reject H0 when it is actually true (we call this a false alarm , or Type I error ), Type I error  means that we have concluded that there is a relationship in the population when in fact there is not. Type I errors occur because even when there is no relationship in the population, sampling error alone will occasionally produce an extreme result.
  • We can retain H0 when it is actually false (we call this a miss , or Type II error ). Type II error  means that we have concluded that there is no relationship in the population when in fact there is.

Summing up, when you perform a hypothesis test, there are four possible outcomes depending on the actual truth (or falseness) of the null hypothesis H0 and the decision to reject or not. The outcomes are summarized in the following table:

Table 1. The four possible outcomes in hypothesis testing.

  • The decision is not to reject H0 when H0 is true (correct decision).
  • The decision is to reject H0 when H0 is true (incorrect decision known as a Type I error ).
  • The decision is not to reject H0 when, in fact, H0 is false (incorrect decision known as a Type II error ).
  • The decision is to reject H0 when H0 is false ( correct decision ).

Neyman and Pearson coined two terms to describe the probability of these two types of errors in the long run:

  • P(Type I error) = αalpha
  • P(Type II error) = βbeta

That is, if we set αalpha to .05, then in the long run we should make a Type I error 5% of the time. The 𝞪 (alpha) , is associated with the p-value for the level of significance. Again it’s common to set αalpha as .05. In fact, when the null hypothesis is true and α is .05, we will mistakenly reject the null hypothesis 5% of the time. (This is why α is sometimes referred to as the “Type I error rate.”) In principle, it is possible to reduce the chance of a Type I error by setting α to something less than .05. Setting it to .01, for example, would mean that if the null hypothesis is true, then there is only a 1% chance of mistakenly rejecting it. But making it harder to reject true null hypotheses also makes it harder to reject false ones and therefore increases the chance of a Type II error.

In practice, Type II errors occur primarily because the research design lacks adequate statistical power to detect the relationship (e.g., the sample is too small).  Statistical power is the complement of Type II error. We will have more to say about statistical power shortly. The standard value for an acceptable level of β (beta) is .2 – that is, we are willing to accept that 20% of the time we will fail to detect a true effect when it truly exists. It is possible to reduce the chance of a Type II error by setting α to something greater than .05 (e.g., .10). But making it easier to reject false null hypotheses also makes it easier to reject true ones and therefore increases the chance of a Type I error. This provides some insight into why the convention is to set α to .05. There is some agreement among researchers that level of α keeps the rates of both Type I and Type II errors at acceptable levels.

The possibility of committing Type I and Type II errors has several important implications for interpreting the results of our own and others’ research. One is that we should be cautious about interpreting the results of any individual study because there is a chance that it reflects a Type I or Type II error. This is why researchers consider it important to replicate their studies. Each time researchers replicate a study and find a similar result, they rightly become more confident that the result represents a real phenomenon and not just a Type I or Type II error.

Test Statistic Assumptions

Last consideration we will revisit with each test statistic (e.g., t-test, z-test and ANOVA) in the coming chapters.  There are four main assumptions. These assumptions are often taken for granted in using prescribed data for the course.  In the real world, these assumptions would need to be examined, often tested using statistical software.

  • Assumption of random sampling. A sample is random when each person (or animal) point in your population has an equal chance of being included in the sample; therefore selection of any individual happens by chance, rather than by choice. This reduces the chance that differences in materials, characteristics or conditions may bias results. Remember that random samples are more likely to be representative of the population so researchers can be more confident interpreting the results. Note: there is no test that statistical software can perform which assures random sampling has occurred but following good sampling techniques helps to ensure your samples are random.
  • Assumption of Independence. Statistical independence is a critical assumption for many statistical tests including the 2-sample t-test and ANOVA. It is assumed that observations are independent of each other often but often this assumption. Is not met. Independence means the value of one observation does not influence or affect the value of other observations. Independent data items are not connected with one another in any way (unless you account for it in your study). Even the smallest dependence in your data can turn into heavily biased results (which may be undetectable) if you violate this assumption. Note: there is no test statistical software can perform that assures independence of the data because this should be addressed during the research planning phase. Using a non-parametric test is often recommended if a researcher is concerned this assumption has been violated.
  • Assumption of Normality. Normality assumes that the continuous variables (dependent variable) used in the analysis are normally distributed. Normal distributions are symmetric around the center (the mean) and form a bell-shaped distribution. Normality is violated when sample data are skewed. With large enough sample sizes (n > 30) the violation of the normality assumption should not cause major problems (remember the central limit theorem) but there is a feature in most statistical software that can alert researchers to an assumption violation.
  • Assumption of Equal Variance. Variance refers to the spread or of scores from the mean. Many statistical tests assume that although different samples can come from populations with different means, they have the same variance. Equality of variance (i.e., homogeneity of variance) is violated when variances across different groups or samples are significantly different. Note: there is a feature in most statistical software to test for this.

We will use 4 main steps for hypothesis testing:

  • Usually the hypotheses concern population parameters and predict the characteristics that a sample should have
  • Null: Null hypothesis (H0) states that there is no difference, no effect or no change between population means and sample means. There is no difference.
  • Alternative: Alternative hypothesis (H1 or HA) states that there is a difference or a change between the population and sample. It is the opposite of the null hypothesis.
  • Set criteria for a decision. In this step we must determine the boundary of our distribution at which the null hypothesis will be rejected. Researchers usually use either a 5% (.05) cutoff or 1% (.01) critical boundary. Recall from our earlier story about Ronald Fisher that the lower the probability the more confident the was that the Tea Lady was not guessing.  We will apply this to z in the next chapter.
  • Compare sample and population to decide if the hypothesis has support
  • When a researcher uses hypothesis testing, the individual is making a decision about whether the data collected is sufficient to state that the population parameters are significantly different.

Further considerations

The probability value is the probability of a result as extreme or more extreme given that the null hypothesis is true. It is the probability of the data given the null hypothesis. It is not the probability that the null hypothesis is false.

A low probability value indicates that the sample outcome (or one more extreme) would be very unlikely if the null hypothesis were true. We will learn more about assessing effect size later in this unit.

3.  A non-significant outcome means that the data do not conclusively demonstrate that the null hypothesis is false. There is always a chance of error and 4 outcomes associated with hypothesis testing.

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  • It is important to take into account the assumptions for each test statistic.

Learning objectives

Having read the chapter, you should be able to:

  • Identify the components of a hypothesis test, including the parameter of interest, the null and alternative hypotheses, and the test statistic.
  • State the hypotheses and identify appropriate critical areas depending on how hypotheses are set up.
  • Describe the proper interpretations of a p-value as well as common misinterpretations.
  • Distinguish between the two types of error in hypothesis testing, and the factors that determine them.
  • Describe the main criticisms of null hypothesis statistical testing
  • Identify the purpose of effect size and power.

Exercises – Ch. 9

  • In your own words, explain what the null hypothesis is.
  • What are Type I and Type II Errors?
  • Why do we phrase null and alternative hypotheses with population parameters and not sample means?
  • If our null hypothesis is “H0: μ = 40”, what are the three possible alternative hypotheses?
  • Why do we state our hypotheses and decision criteria before we collect our data?
  • When and why do you calculate an effect size?

Answers to Odd- Numbered Exercises – Ch. 9

1. Your answer should include mention of the baseline assumption of no difference between the sample and the population.

3. Alpha is the significance level. It is the criteria we use when decided to reject or fail to reject the null hypothesis, corresponding to a given proportion of the area under the normal distribution and a probability of finding extreme scores assuming the null hypothesis is true.

5. μ > 40; μ < 40; μ ≠ 40

7. We calculate effect size to determine the strength of the finding.  Effect size should always be calculated when the we have rejected the null hypothesis.  Effect size can be calculated for non-significant findings as a possible indicator of Type II error.

Introduction to Statistics for Psychology Copyright © 2021 by Alisa Beyer is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Introduction to Psychology/Research Methods in Psychology

RESEARCH METHOD IN PSYCHOLOGY.

Research psychology encompasses the study of behavior for use in academic settings, and contains numerous areas. It contains the areas of abnormal psychology, biological psychology, cognitive psychology, comparative psychology, developmental psychology, personality psychology, social psychology and others. All branches of psychology can have a research component to them. Research psychology is contrasted with applied psychology.

Research in psychology is conducted in broad accord with the standards of the scientific method, encompassing both qualitative ethological and quantitative statistical modalities to generate and evaluate explanatory hypotheses with regard to psychological phenomena. Where research ethics and the state of development in a given research domain permits, investigation may be pursued by experimental protocols. Psychology tends to be eclectic, drawing on scientific knowledge from other fields to help explain and understand psychological phenomena. Qualitative psychological research utilizes a broad spectrum of observational methods, including action research, ethography, exploratory statistics, structured interviews, and participant observation, to enable the gathering of rich information unattainable by classical experimentation. Research in humanistic psychology is more typically pursued by ethnographic, historical, and historiographic methods.

The testing of different aspects of psychological function is a significant area of contemporary psychology. Psychometric and statistical methods predominate, including various well-known standardized tests as well as those created ad hoc as the situation or experiment requires.

Academic psychologists may focus purely on research and psychological theory, aiming to further psychological understanding in a particular area, while other psychologists may work in applied psychology to deploy such knowledge for immediate and practical benefit. However, these approaches are not mutually exclusive and most psychologists will be involved in both researching and applying psychology at some point during their career. Clinical psychology, among many of the various disciplines of psychology, aims at developing in practicing psychologists knowledge of and experience with research and experimental methods which they will continue to build up as well as employ as they treat individuals with psychological issues or use psychology to help others.

When an area of interest requires specific training and specialist knowledge, especially in applied areas, psychological associations normally establish a governing body to manage training requirements. Similarly, requirements may be laid down for university degrees in psychology, so that students acquire an adequate knowledge in a number of areas. Additionally, areas of practical psychology, where psychologists offer treatment to others, may require that psychologists be licensed by government regulatory bodies as well.

Quantitative psychology involves the application of statistical analysis to psychological research, and the development of novel statistical approaches for measuring and explaining human behavior. It is a young field (only recently have Ph.D. programs in quantitative psychology been formed), and it is loosely comprised of the subfields psychometrics and mathematical psychology.

Psychometrics is the field of psychology concerned with the theory and technique of psychological measurement, which includes the measurement of knowledge, abilities, attitudes, interests, achievement in particular degree or course, and personality traits (Carl Dellomos, 2009). Measurement of these unobservable phenomena is difficult, and much of the research and accumulated knowledge in this discipline has been developed in an attempt to properly define and quantify such phenomena. Psychometric research typically involves two major research tasks, namely: (i) the construction of instruments and procedures for measurement; and (ii) the development and refinement of theoretical approaches to measurement.

Psychology is a science, to be approached as such. Experiments should be designed using the scientific method.

There are several research methods that psychologists employ:

  • 1.1 Nomothetic (Quantitative Approach)
  • 1.2 Idiographic (Qualitative Approach)
  • 2.1 Descriptive Studies
  • 2.2 Factorial Design
  • 2.3 Correlational Study
  • 2.4 Experiments
  • 2.5 Naturalistic Observation
  • 2.6 Self Report
  • 3.1.1 Statistical Symbols
  • 3.1.2 Frequency Distribution
  • 3.1.3 Measures of Central Tendency
  • 3.1.4 Measures of Variability
  • 3.2 Case Studies
  • 4 Basic Concepts

Approaches in Psychology Research [ edit | edit source ]

Nomothetic (quantitative approach) [ edit | edit source ].

This approach is basically used in inferential and descriptive statistics as both mediums of scientific method of investigation in analyzing, presenting, and interpretation of data gathered by the researcher through standardized or objective instruments (e.g. psychological Tests). The term “nomothetic” comes from the Greek word “nomos” meaning “law”. Psychologists who adopt this approach are mainly concerned with studying what we share with others. That is to say in establishing laws or generalisations. (Carl Dellomos, 2009)

Idiographic (Qualitative Approach) [ edit | edit source ]

This approach tends not to use inferential or descriptive statistics, but rather uses qualitative methods of data gathering such as interviews, diaries, and other written materials, obtained from or provided by the expected or anticipated respondents of a particular research. The term “idiographic” comes from the Greek word “idios” meaning “own” or “private”. Psychologists interested in this aspect of experience seek to discover what makes each of us unique. Despite the importance of our genetic individuality, proceeding from biology, the distinction between the nomothetic and the idiographic is often equated with two types of science — the natural sciences concerned with discovering laws of nature, and the social sciences concerned with individual meanings. We can examine these differences further by seeing how they relate to personality theory.(Carl Dellomos, 2009)

Both approaches were introduced by Gordon Allport. (Carl Dellomos, 2009)

Research Designs [ edit | edit source ]

Although there are many different kinds of research designs in psychology, studies may be categorized into descriptive or qualitative, correlational, and experimental. The method of data collection also varies, with self-report on one end of the spectrum, and naturalistic observation on the other.

Descriptive Studies [ edit | edit source ]

The Studies that do not test specific relationships between variables are called descriptive studies. In this research method, general or specific behaviors or attributes are observed and measured, without respect to each other. These studies are generally the design of choice for breaking into new areas, as the vast but often inconclusive amount of information collected can be drawn upon for future hypotheses.

An example of such a study would be a researcher inquiring into the quality of mental health institutions. This would be done by observation or measurements of various criteria, as opposed to relationships between variables. Alternatively, the study could be conducted without any specific criteria in mind.

Factorial Design [ edit | edit source ]

Every level of one independent variable is joined with every level of the others in a factorial design to create every possible combination. Because this particular design contains two variables, each of which has two levels, it is known as a 2 × 2 (or "two-by-two") factorial design. There would be six different conditions in a 3 × 2 factorial design if one of the independent variables had a third level. Observe that the product of the levels' numbers is the total number of possible circumstances. There are four conditions in a 2 × 2 factorial design, six in a 3 × 2 factorial design, twenty in a 4 × 5 factorial design, and so on. It's also important to note that every number in the notation indicates a single independent variable or component. Because this particular design contains two variables, each of which has two levels, it is known as a 2 × 2 (or "two-by-two") factorial design. There would be six different conditions in a 3 × 2 factorial design if one of the independent variables had a third level. Observe that the product of the levels' numbers is the total number of possible circumstances. There are four conditions in a 2 × 2 factorial design, six in a 3 × 2 factorial design, twenty in a 4 × 5 factorial design, and so on. It's also important to note that every number in the notation indicates a single independent variable or component.

Correlational Study [ edit | edit source ]

This method of statistical analysis shows the relationship between two variables. For example, research has shown that alcohol dependence correlates with depression. That is to say, the more alcohol people consume, the more depressed they become. On the other hand, it could be the other way around as well: the more depressed people become, the more likely they are to consume alcohol.

The attributes of correlations include strength and direction. The direction may be positive (both variables both increase or decrease together), negative (one variable increases while the other decreases) or unrelated (a random relationship between variables). The strength of a correlation ranges from -1 to +1 with a 0 reflecting no relationship between variables. A correlational study serves only to describe/predict behavior and not to explain it. This is so because a third variable could be shown to cause the occurrence of one of the variables. Furthermore, only experiments can prove causation.

Experiments [ edit | edit source ]

Experiments are generally the studies that are the most precise and have the most weight to them due to their conclusive power. They are particularly effective in proving hypotheses about cause and effect relationships between variables. A hypothesis is a prediction of how one variable relates to another. There are two types of hypotheses, null and directional . The null is a prediction that there will not be any change in the dependent variable when the researcher changes the independent variable. The directional hypothesis states that the change in the independent variable will induce a change in the dependent variable. In a true experiment, all variables are held constant except for the independent variable, which is manipulated. Thus, any changes in the experimental groups can be solely attributed to the action of the independent variable. This is called being objective .

For instance, in an experiment to test whether music improves people's memories, we would have a sheet of paper with ten unrelated words on it for people to memorize. The control group would have no music playing in the background while the experimental group would have some music in the background. Because as researchers we have adhered to the scientific method and held all variables as constant as possible, if the experimental group does report better recollection of words, then we could assume that the music had an effect on memory. However, we must be certain to do our best to ensure that any controllable differences between the two groups are eliminated in order to ensure that no confounding variable interferes with the experiment.

There are two main ways to pick, or sample the subjects in an experiment, random and stratified . In a random sampling each person has an equal chance at being picked. This means that if 80% of the population being sampled from are Christian, then 80% of the sample will be Christian. If the researcher wanted all religions represented equally, he would employ stratified sampling. For instance, the experiment could be performed only on women, or on mixed groups with equal numbers of each sex in them, to eliminate the possibility of biased results from one gender having better average memory than the other.

Steps must be taken to make sure that there is no experimenter bias . Two common forms of bias are demand characteristics and expectancy effects. If a researcher expects certain results from an experiment and influences the subjects response this is called demand characteristics. If the experimenter inadvertently interprets the information to be as expected in their hypothesis it is called expectancy effect. To counteract experimenter bias the subjects can be kept uninformed on the intentions of the experiment, which is called single blind. If the people collecting the information and the subjects giving it are kept uninformed then it is called a double blind experiment.

The experiment should also be reported so that other researchers can repeat it. If an experiment isn't repeatable it will not hold much weight in the scientific community. To help an experiment be repeatable the researcher should have the variables be measureable, this is called being empirical .

Whether researching humans or animals the experiment should be ethical. When humans are the subjects they should be informed of what the study is, consent to being in it, be debriefed afterwards, and their information should also be kept confidential .

Naturalistic Observation [ edit | edit source ]

Researchers study organisms in their natural environments or habitats without trying to manipulate or control anything. In this method, the researcher observes the behavior under study in its natural setting while attempting to avoid influencing or controlling it. The observations are done in a naturalistic setting without any preparation or participation of the researcher. Therefore, the behavior is observed in public places, streets, homes, and schools. Observing people from other cultures response in the same setting is a way to provide information for cross-cultural research .

Self Report [ edit | edit source ]

This method includes tests , questionnaires , and interviews . All of which do the same thing, give the subject a stimuli, i.e. the question, and get a response. The advantage of using these is the ability to inexpensively and rapidly collect vast amounts of data. This allows a psychologist to compare one person, or a group of peoples results to thousands of others. The disadvantage is that they are not always telling what the subject's response is but what the subject says is the response.

Information Display [ edit | edit source ]

Statistics [ edit | edit source ].

Once the information is gathered it has to be put into some kind of form, usually numerical. Statistics deals with the collection, analysis, interpretation, and presentation of numerical data. The goal of statistics is to summarize the data and let descriptions or inferences be made . Inferences are used when making predictions of the relationships of variables. Descriptions are concise displays, using statistical symbols ,of the information in frequency distributions, measures of central tendency , or measures of variability .

Statistical Symbols [ edit | edit source ]

There are agreed upon standard symbols used in statistical displays. These symbols can be used by themselves or in equations.

N = number of scores

X = score (or scores)

d = difference of a score from the mean

D = difference in rank

r or ρ = correlation

SD = standard deviation

Frequency Distribution [ edit | edit source ]

A frequency distribution is obtained by taking the score and splitting them into subgroups. The subgroups are then put on either a histogram (bar graph) or a frequency polygram (line graph). When a frequency distribution has most of the scores on one side of the graph it is considered skewed. If it has most of the scores in the middle with equal amounts on both sides it is considered symmetrical.

Measures of Central Tendency [ edit | edit source ]

In measures of central tendency there is one number that is used to represent a group of numbers. This number is either the mean , median , or the mode .

Measures of Variability [ edit | edit source ]

Variability is concerned with the dispersal of the scores, called variability i.e. are the scores clustered together or spread out. Range and standard deviation are the measures most commonly used. To find the range just subtract the number of the lowest score from the number of the highest score. This can be deceiving if most of the scores are bunched together and one of the scores is very far away from it. In this case standard deviation must be used. A formula commonly used for standard deviation is SD = the square root of Σd²/N.

Case Studies [ edit | edit source ]

In the course of treating a patient a psychologist will take records of problems, insights, and techniques that were important in the patients treatment. A clinical case history may be drawn upon by researchers to expose a factor that is important for understanding a behavior. Case studies repeat and are used as guides for psychologists.

Basic Concepts [ edit | edit source ]

  • a. independent variable = variable that one manipulates in order to see if it has any effect on the dependent variable (eg. in the example above, the independent variable would be music and its effect on memory)
  • b. dependent variable = variable that depends on the effect of the independent variable (e.g. in the example above, the dependent variable would be memory and better recollection of words)
  • c. double-blind procedure = procedure in which neither the researcher nor the subjects know which group (experimental or control) the subjects are in in order to minimize experimenter cues.
  • d. single-blind procedure = procedure in which only the researcher knows which group which kind of subject is in.
  • e. experimenter cues - subtle and often unintentional cues that the experimenter makes which implies which group which kind of subject is in. for example, if an experimenter believes that music does indeed improve memory, some cues would be the experimenter's smiling/winking at the experimental group. This smile/wink would imply to the subjects in the experimental group that the researcher is secretly implying that they're in the experimental group.
  • f. placebo effect - a treatment works because of the patient's belief that it works and not because it actually does.
  • g. experimenter - the person who is researching through the participant.

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  • Book:Introduction to Psychology

Navigation menu

Logo for Pressbooks

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Overview of the Scientific Method

Learning Objectives

  • Distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis.
  • Discover how theories are used to generate hypotheses and how the results of studies can be used to further inform theories.
  • Understand the characteristics of a good hypothesis.

Theories and Hypotheses

Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A  theory  is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes, functions, or organizing principles that have not been observed directly. Consider, for example, Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation and social inhibition (1965) [1] . He proposed that being watched by others while performing a task creates a general state of physiological arousal, which increases the likelihood of the dominant (most likely) response. So for highly practiced tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make correct responses, but for relatively unpracticed tasks, being watched increases the tendency to make incorrect responses. Notice that this theory—which has come to be called drive theory—provides an explanation of both social facilitation and social inhibition that goes beyond the phenomena themselves by including concepts such as “arousal” and “dominant response,” along with processes such as the effect of arousal on the dominant response.

Outside of science, referring to an idea as a theory often implies that it is untested—perhaps no more than a wild guess. In science, however, the term theory has no such implication. A theory is simply an explanation or interpretation of a set of phenomena. It can be untested, but it can also be extensively tested, well supported, and accepted as an accurate description of the world by the scientific community. The theory of evolution by natural selection, for example, is a theory because it is an explanation of the diversity of life on earth—not because it is untested or unsupported by scientific research. On the contrary, the evidence for this theory is overwhelmingly positive and nearly all scientists accept its basic assumptions as accurate. Similarly, the “germ theory” of disease is a theory because it is an explanation of the origin of various diseases, not because there is any doubt that many diseases are caused by microorganisms that infect the body.

A  hypothesis , on the other hand, is a specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate. It is an explanation that relies on just a few key concepts. Hypotheses are often specific predictions about what will happen in a particular study. They are developed by considering existing evidence and using reasoning to infer what will happen in the specific context of interest. Hypotheses are often but not always derived from theories. So a hypothesis is often a prediction based on a theory but some hypotheses are a-theoretical and only after a set of observations have been made, is a theory developed. This is because theories are broad in nature and they explain larger bodies of data. So if our research question is really original then we may need to collect some data and make some observations before we can develop a broader theory.

Theories and hypotheses always have this  if-then  relationship. “ If   drive theory is correct,  then  cockroaches should run through a straight runway faster, and a branching runway more slowly, when other cockroaches are present.” Although hypotheses are usually expressed as statements, they can always be rephrased as questions. “Do cockroaches run through a straight runway faster when other cockroaches are present?” Thus deriving hypotheses from theories is an excellent way of generating interesting research questions.

But how do researchers derive hypotheses from theories? One way is to generate a research question using the techniques discussed in this chapter  and then ask whether any theory implies an answer to that question. For example, you might wonder whether expressive writing about positive experiences improves health as much as expressive writing about traumatic experiences. Although this  question  is an interesting one  on its own, you might then ask whether the habituation theory—the idea that expressive writing causes people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings—implies an answer. In this case, it seems clear that if the habituation theory is correct, then expressive writing about positive experiences should not be effective because it would not cause people to habituate to negative thoughts and feelings. A second way to derive hypotheses from theories is to focus on some component of the theory that has not yet been directly observed. For example, a researcher could focus on the process of habituation—perhaps hypothesizing that people should show fewer signs of emotional distress with each new writing session.

Among the very best hypotheses are those that distinguish between competing theories. For example, Norbert Schwarz and his colleagues considered two theories of how people make judgments about themselves, such as how assertive they are (Schwarz et al., 1991) [2] . Both theories held that such judgments are based on relevant examples that people bring to mind. However, one theory was that people base their judgments on the  number  of examples they bring to mind and the other was that people base their judgments on how  easily  they bring those examples to mind. To test these theories, the researchers asked people to recall either six times when they were assertive (which is easy for most people) or 12 times (which is difficult for most people). Then they asked them to judge their own assertiveness. Note that the number-of-examples theory implies that people who recalled 12 examples should judge themselves to be more assertive because they recalled more examples, but the ease-of-examples theory implies that participants who recalled six examples should judge themselves as more assertive because recalling the examples was easier. Thus the two theories made opposite predictions so that only one of the predictions could be confirmed. The surprising result was that participants who recalled fewer examples judged themselves to be more assertive—providing particularly convincing evidence in favor of the ease-of-retrieval theory over the number-of-examples theory.

Theory Testing

The primary way that scientific researchers use theories is sometimes called the hypothetico-deductive method  (although this term is much more likely to be used by philosophers of science than by scientists themselves). Researchers begin with a set of phenomena and either construct a theory to explain or interpret them or choose an existing theory to work with. They then make a prediction about some new phenomenon that should be observed if the theory is correct. Again, this prediction is called a hypothesis. The researchers then conduct an empirical study to test the hypothesis. Finally, they reevaluate the theory in light of the new results and revise it if necessary. This process is usually conceptualized as a cycle because the researchers can then derive a new hypothesis from the revised theory, conduct a new empirical study to test the hypothesis, and so on. As  Figure 2.3  shows, this approach meshes nicely with the model of scientific research in psychology presented earlier in the textbook—creating a more detailed model of “theoretically motivated” or “theory-driven” research.

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

As an example, let us consider Zajonc’s research on social facilitation and inhibition. He started with a somewhat contradictory pattern of results from the research literature. He then constructed his drive theory, according to which being watched by others while performing a task causes physiological arousal, which increases an organism’s tendency to make the dominant response. This theory predicts social facilitation for well-learned tasks and social inhibition for poorly learned tasks. He now had a theory that organized previous results in a meaningful way—but he still needed to test it. He hypothesized that if his theory was correct, he should observe that the presence of others improves performance in a simple laboratory task but inhibits performance in a difficult version of the very same laboratory task. To test this hypothesis, one of the studies he conducted used cockroaches as subjects (Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969) [3] . The cockroaches ran either down a straight runway (an easy task for a cockroach) or through a cross-shaped maze (a difficult task for a cockroach) to escape into a dark chamber when a light was shined on them. They did this either while alone or in the presence of other cockroaches in clear plastic “audience boxes.” Zajonc found that cockroaches in the straight runway reached their goal more quickly in the presence of other cockroaches, but cockroaches in the cross-shaped maze reached their goal more slowly when they were in the presence of other cockroaches. Thus he confirmed his hypothesis and provided support for his drive theory. (Zajonc also showed that drive theory existed in humans [Zajonc & Sales, 1966] [4] in many other studies afterward).

Incorporating Theory into Your Research

When you write your research report or plan your presentation, be aware that there are two basic ways that researchers usually include theory. The first is to raise a research question, answer that question by conducting a new study, and then offer one or more theories (usually more) to explain or interpret the results. This format works well for applied research questions and for research questions that existing theories do not address. The second way is to describe one or more existing theories, derive a hypothesis from one of those theories, test the hypothesis in a new study, and finally reevaluate the theory. This format works well when there is an existing theory that addresses the research question—especially if the resulting hypothesis is surprising or conflicts with a hypothesis derived from a different theory.

To use theories in your research will not only give you guidance in coming up with experiment ideas and possible projects, but it lends legitimacy to your work. Psychologists have been interested in a variety of human behaviors and have developed many theories along the way. Using established theories will help you break new ground as a researcher, not limit you from developing your own ideas.

Characteristics of a Good Hypothesis

There are three general characteristics of a good hypothesis. First, a good hypothesis must be testable and falsifiable . We must be able to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and if you’ll recall Popper’s falsifiability criterion, it must be possible to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false. Second, a good hypothesis must be logical. As described above, hypotheses are more than just a random guess. Hypotheses should be informed by previous theories or observations and logical reasoning. Typically, we begin with a broad and general theory and use  deductive reasoning to generate a more specific hypothesis to test based on that theory. Occasionally, however, when there is no theory to inform our hypothesis, we use  inductive reasoning  which involves using specific observations or research findings to form a more general hypothesis. Finally, the hypothesis should be positive. That is, the hypothesis should make a positive statement about the existence of a relationship or effect, rather than a statement that a relationship or effect does not exist. As scientists, we don’t set out to show that relationships do not exist or that effects do not occur so our hypotheses should not be worded in a way to suggest that an effect or relationship does not exist. The nature of science is to assume that something does not exist and then seek to find evidence to prove this wrong, to show that it really does exist. That may seem backward to you but that is the nature of the scientific method. The underlying reason for this is beyond the scope of this chapter but it has to do with statistical theory.

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation.  Science, 149 , 269–274 ↵
  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 195–202. ↵
  • Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. M. (1969). Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13 , 83–92. ↵
  • Zajonc, R.B. & Sales, S.M. (1966). Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 , 160-168. ↵

A coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena.

A specific prediction about a new phenomenon that should be observed if a particular theory is accurate.

A cyclical process of theory development, starting with an observed phenomenon, then developing or using a theory to make a specific prediction of what should happen if that theory is correct, testing that prediction, refining the theory in light of the findings, and using that refined theory to develop new hypotheses, and so on.

The ability to test the hypothesis using the methods of science and the possibility to gather evidence that will disconfirm the hypothesis if it is indeed false.

Research Methods in Psychology Copyright © 2019 by Rajiv S. Jhangiani, I-Chant A. Chiang, Carrie Cuttler, & Dana C. Leighton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Final dates! Join the tutor2u subject teams in London for a day of exam technique and revision at the cinema. Learn more →

Reference Library

Collections

  • See what's new
  • All Resources
  • Student Resources
  • Assessment Resources
  • Teaching Resources
  • CPD Courses
  • Livestreams

Study notes, videos, interactive activities and more!

Psychology news, insights and enrichment

Currated collections of free resources

Browse resources by topic

  • All Psychology Resources

Resource Selections

Currated lists of resources

  • Study Notes

Aims and Hypotheses

Last updated 22 Mar 2021

  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on Twitter
  • Share by Email

Observations of events or behaviour in our surroundings provoke questions as to why they occur. In turn, one or multiple theories might attempt to explain a phenomenon, and investigations are consequently conducted to test them. One observation could be that athletes tend to perform better when they have a training partner, and a theory might propose that this is because athletes are more motivated with peers around them.

The aim of an investigation, driven by a theory to explain a given observation, states the intent of the study in general terms. Continuing the above example, the consequent aim might be “to investigate the effect of having a training partner on athletes’ motivation levels”.

The theory attempting to explain an observation will help to inform hypotheses - predictions of an investigation’s outcome that make specific reference to the independent variables (IVs) manipulated and dependent variables (DVs) measured by the researchers.

There are two types of hypothesis:

  • - H 1 – Research hypothesis
  • - H 0 – Null hypothesis

H 1 – The Research Hypothesis

This predicts a statistically significant effect of an IV on a DV (i.e. an experiment), or a significant relationship between variables (i.e. a correlation study), e.g.

  • In an experiment: “Athletes who have a training partner are likely to score higher on a questionnaire measuring motivation levels than athletes who train alone.”
  • In a correlation study: ‘There will be a significant positive correlation between athletes’ motivation questionnaire scores and the number of partners athletes train with.”

The research hypothesis will be directional (one-tailed) if theory or existing evidence argues a particular ‘direction’ of the predicted results, as demonstrated in the two hypothesis examples above.

Non-directional (two-tailed) research hypotheses do not predict a direction, so here would simply predict “a significant difference” between questionnaire scores in athletes who train alone and with a training partner (in an experiment), or “a significant relationship” between questionnaire scores and number of training partners (in a correlation study).

H 0 – The Null Hypothesis

This predicts that a statistically significant effect or relationship will not be found, e.g.

  • In an experiment: “There will be no significant difference in motivation questionnaire scores between athletes who train with and without a training partner.”
  • In a correlation study: “There will be no significant relationship between motivation questionnaire scores and the number of partners athletes train with.”

When the investigation concludes, analysis of results will suggest that either the research hypothesis or null hypothesis can be retained, with the other rejected. Ultimately this will either provide evidence to support of refute the theory driving a hypothesis, and may lead to further research in the field.

You might also like

A level psychology topic quiz - research methods.

Quizzes & Activities

Research Methods: MCQ Revision Test 1 for AQA A Level Psychology

Topic Videos

Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June 2018 (AQA)

Exam Support

Our subjects

  • › Criminology
  • › Economics
  • › Geography
  • › Health & Social Care
  • › Psychology
  • › Sociology
  • › Teaching & learning resources
  • › Student revision workshops
  • › Online student courses
  • › CPD for teachers
  • › Livestreams
  • › Teaching jobs

Boston House, 214 High Street, Boston Spa, West Yorkshire, LS23 6AD Tel: 01937 848885

  • › Contact us
  • › Terms of use
  • › Privacy & cookies

© 2002-2024 Tutor2u Limited. Company Reg no: 04489574. VAT reg no 816865400.

Library homepage

  • school Campus Bookshelves
  • menu_book Bookshelves
  • perm_media Learning Objects
  • login Login
  • how_to_reg Request Instructor Account
  • hub Instructor Commons
  • Download Page (PDF)
  • Download Full Book (PDF)
  • Periodic Table
  • Physics Constants
  • Scientific Calculator
  • Reference & Cite
  • Tools expand_more
  • Readability

selected template will load here

This action is not available.

Statistics LibreTexts

8.4: The Alternative Hypothesis

  • Last updated
  • Save as PDF
  • Page ID 14493

  • Foster et al.
  • University of Missouri-St. Louis, Rice University, & University of Houston, Downtown Campus via University of Missouri’s Affordable and Open Access Educational Resources Initiative

If the null hypothesis is rejected, then we will need some other explanation, which we call the alternative hypothesis, \(H_A\) or \(H_1\). The alternative hypothesis is simply the reverse of the null hypothesis, and there are three options, depending on where we expect the difference to lie. Thus, our alternative hypothesis is the mathematical way of stating our research question. If we expect our obtained sample mean to be above or below the null hypothesis value, which we call a directional hypothesis, then our alternative hypothesis takes the form:

\[\mathrm{H}_{\mathrm{A}}: \mu>7.47 \quad \text { or } \quad \mathrm{H}_{\mathrm{A}}: \mu<7.47 \nonumber \]

based on the research question itself. We should only use a directional hypothesis if we have good reason, based on prior observations or research, to suspect a particular direction. When we do not know the direction, such as when we are entering a new area of research, we use a non-directional alternative:

\[\mathrm{H}_{\mathrm{A}}: \mu \neq 7.47 \nonumber \]

We will set different criteria for rejecting the null hypothesis based on the directionality (greater than, less than, or not equal to) of the alternative. To understand why, we need to see where our criteria come from and how they relate to \(z\)-scores and distributions.

Writing hypotheses in words

As we alluded to in the null hypothesis section, we can write our hypotheses in word statements (in addition to the statements with symbols). These statements should be specific enough to the particular experiment or situation being referred to. That is, don't make them generic enough so that they would apply to any hypothesis test that you would conduct. 

Examples for how to write null and alternate hypotheses in words for directional and non-directional situations are given throughout the chapters. 

Contributors and Attributions

Foster et al.  (University of Missouri-St. Louis, Rice University, & University of Houston, Downtown Campus)

MIM Learnovate

Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypothesis in Research

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

In the world of research and statistical analysis, formulating hypotheses is a crucial step in the scientific process. Hypotheses guide researchers in making predictions and testing relationships between variables. When it comes to hypotheses, there are two main types: directional and non-directional.

In this blog post, we will explore the differences between Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypothesis in Research and their implications in research.

  • Table of Contents

Directional Hypothesis

A directional hypothesis, also known as a one-tailed hypothesis, is formulated with a specific predicted direction of the relationship between variables. It indicates an expectation of the relationship being either positive or negative.

Directional Hypothesis

The directional hypothesis is often used when there is prior knowledge or theoretical reasoning supporting the predicted direction of the relationship. It allows researchers to make more specific predictions and draw conclusions based on the expected direction of the effect.

Example of Directional Hypothesis

For example, a directional hypothesis might state that “increased physical activity will lead to a decrease in body weight.” Here, the researcher expects a negative relationship between physical activity and body weight.

Advantages of Directional Hypothesis

  • Specific predictions: Directional hypotheses provide a clear prediction of the expected relationship between variables, allowing for a focused investigation.
  • Increased statistical power: By focusing on one direction of the relationship, researchers can allocate more statistical power to that specific direction, increasing the chances of detecting a significant effect if it exists.

Non-Directional Hypothesis

A non-directional hypothesis, also known as a two-tailed hypothesis, does not make a specific prediction about the direction of the relationship between variables. Instead, it states that there is a relationship, but without indicating whether it will be positive or negative.

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Non-directional hypotheses are often used when there is insufficient prior knowledge or theoretical basis to predict the direction of the relationship. It allows for a more exploratory approach, where the researcher is open to discovering the nature of the relationship through data analysis .

Read More: Internal Validity vs External Validity | Examples

Example of Non-Directional Hypothesis

For example, a non-directional hypothesis might state that “there is a relationship between caffeine consumption and reaction time.” Here, the researcher expects a relationship between the variables but does not specify the direction.

Read More: Population vs Sample | Examples

Advantages of Non-Directional Hypothesis:

  • Flexibility: Non-directional hypotheses provide flexibility in exploring relationships between variables without preconceived notions about the direction of the effect.
  • Open to unexpected findings : By not specifying the direction, researchers remain open to unexpected results or alternative explanations that may emerge during the analysis.

Difference Between Directional and Non-Directional Hypotheses

Choosing Between Directional and Non-Directional Hypotheses: The choice between a directional and non-directional hypothesis depends on the research question, existing knowledge, and theoretical background. Here are a few considerations for selecting the appropriate type of hypothesis:

Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypothesis

  • Prior research: If previous studies have established a clear direction of the relationship, a directional hypothesis may be more appropriate.
  • Theoretical reasoning: If there is a strong theoretical foundation supporting a specific direction, a directional hypothesis can provide a focused investigation.
  • Exploratory nature: If the research question is exploratory or lacks prior knowledge, a non-directional hypothesis allows for a more open-ended investigation.

Read More: Reliability vs Validity | Examples

  • Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypothesis

Formulating hypotheses is an essential step in the research process, guiding researchers in testing relationships between variables.

Directional hypotheses offer specific predictions about the expected direction of the relationship, whereas non-directional hypotheses allow for more exploratory investigations without preconceived notions of the direction.

The choice between these types of hypotheses depends on the research question, prior knowledge, and theoretical background.

By understanding the distinctions between directional and non-directional hypotheses, researchers can effectively formulate hypotheses that align with their research goals and contribute to the advancement of scientific knowledge.

Remember, hypotheses serve as a roadmap for research, and regardless of their type, they play a crucial role in scientific inquiry and the pursuit of knowledge.

Other articles

Please read through some of our other articles with examples and explanations if you’d like to learn more about research methodology.

Comparision

  • Basic and Applied Research
  • Cross-Sectional vs Longitudinal Studies
  • Survey vs Questionnaire
  • Open Ended vs Closed Ended Questions
  • Experimental and Non-Experimental Research
  • Inductive vs Deductive Approach
  • Null and Alternative Hypothesis
  • Reliability vs Validity
  • Population vs Sample
  • Conceptual Framework and Theoretical Framework
  • Bibliography and Reference
  • Stratified vs Cluster Sampling
  • Sampling Error vs Sampling Bias
  • Internal Validity vs External Validity
  • Full-Scale, Laboratory-Scale and Pilot-Scale Studies
  • Plagiarism and Paraphrasing
  • Research Methodology Vs. Research Method
  • Mediator and Moderator
  • Type I vs Type II error
  • Descriptive and Inferential Statistics
  • Microsoft Excel and SPSS
  • Parametric and Non-Parametric Test
  • Independent vs. Dependent Variable – MIM Learnovate
  • Research Article and Research Paper
  • Proposition and Hypothesis
  • Principal Component Analysis and Partial Least Squares
  • Academic Research vs Industry Research
  • Clinical Research vs Lab Research
  • Research Lab and Hospital Lab
  • Thesis Statement and Research Question
  • Quantitative Researchers vs. Quantitative Traders
  • Premise, Hypothesis and Supposition
  • Survey Vs Experiment
  • Hypothesis and Theory
  • Independent vs. Dependent Variable
  • APA vs. MLA
  • Ghost Authorship vs. Gift Authorship
  • Research Methods
  • Quantitative Research
  • Qualitative Research
  • Case Study Research
  • Survey Research
  • Conclusive Research
  • Descriptive Research
  • Cross-Sectional Research
  • Theoretical Framework
  • Conceptual Framework
  • Triangulation
  • Grounded Theory
  • Quasi-Experimental Design
  • Mixed Method
  • Correlational Research
  • Randomized Controlled Trial
  • Stratified Sampling
  • Ethnography
  • Ghost Authorship
  • Secondary Data Collection
  • Primary Data Collection
  • Ex-Post-Facto
  •   Dissertation Topic
  • Thesis Statement
  • Research Proposal
  • Research Questions
  • Research Problem
  • Research Gap
  • Types of Research Gaps
  • Operationalization of Variables
  • Literature Review
  • Research Hypothesis
  • Questionnaire
  • Reliability
  • Measurement of Scale
  • Sampling Techniques
  • Acknowledgements
  • PLS-SEM model
  • Principal Components Analysis
  • Multivariate Analysis
  • Friedman Test
  • Chi-Square Test (Χ²)
  • Effect Size

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Related Posts

9 qualitative research designs and research methods, tips to increase your journal citation score, types of research quiz, difference between cohort study and case control study, convenience sampling: method and examples, difference between cohort and panel study, why is a pilot study important in research, panel survey: definition with examples, what is panel study, what is a cohort study | definition & examples, leave a reply cancel reply.

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Premier-Dissertations-Logo

Get an experienced writer start working

Review our examples before placing an order, learn how to draft academic papers, research hypotheses: directional vs. non-directional hypotheses.

Is AP Psychology Hard? Exploring the Challenges and Rewards

Is AP Psychology Hard? Exploring the Challenges and Rewards

100+ Quantitative Research Titles and Topics

100+ Quantitative Research Titles and Topics

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  • Dissertation

Research Hypotheses: Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypotheses

A research hypothesis is a statement that predicts or expects a relationship between variables, and it is tested through research. To create a hypothesis, researchers often review existing literature on the topic. This hypothesis is based on theories, observations, or empirical evidence. It guides the research process, including experiment design, data collection, and analysis. Ultimately, the hypothesis aims to predict the outcome of the study.

What is a Hypothesis in a Dissertation?

This article compares directional and non-directional hypotheses and provides guidelines for writing an effective hypothesis in research. The study explores the differences in predictions and research design implications between the two hypotheses.

3-Step  Dissertation Process!

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Get 3+ Topics

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Dissertation Proposal

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Get Final Dissertation

Types of hypothesis.

There are two main types of hypotheses in research:

Null Hypothesis (H0) 

The null hypothesis is the default assumption in statistical analysis that there is no significant relationship or effect between the variables being studied. It suggests that any observed differences or relationships are due to chance.

Alternative Hypothesis (Ha or H1)

The alternative hypothesis proposes a significant relationship or effect between variables, contradicting the null hypothesis. It reflects the researcher's expectations based on existing theories or observations.

What is Directional Hypotheses?

A directional hypothesis is a type of hypothesis that is used to predict a specific change or outcome in a research study. It is typically used when researchers have a clear idea of the direction in which they expect their results to go, either an increase or decrease, and want to test this prediction. By making a directional hypothesis, researchers can focus their research efforts and design studies that are more likely to uncover meaningful results. In essence, a directional hypothesis is a statement that predicts the direction of the change that is expected to occur between two groups or variables that are being investigated.

Examples of Directional Hypothesis

Example 1: Online versus Traditional Classroom Learning

For instance, consider a study comparing the average study time of college students in online courses versus those in traditional classroom settings. Drawing on prior research indicating that online learning might lead to reduced engagement, a potential directional hypothesis could be: "Students enrolled in online classes will spend fewer weekly study hours than those in traditional classrooms."

In this scenario, our hypothesis presents a clear expectation—that the average number of weekly study hours among online learners will be lower than that of traditional learners. If the actual findings reveal no significant difference or even higher study times among online learners, then our hypothesis would be refuted.

Example 2: Carbon Dioxide Levels and Global Warming

A directional hypothesis in this scenario would propose a specific change in direction between these two variables. For instance, a directional hypothesis might state that as carbon dioxide levels increase, global temperatures will also rise. This hypothesis suggests a causal relationship between the increase in CO2 levels and the phenomenon of global warming, indicating a direction of change in global temperatures corresponding to changes in CO2 levels.

What is a Non-Directional Hypotheses?

In scientific research, a non-directional hypothesis, or null hypothesis, is a statement that suggests the absence of a relationship or difference between the variables being studied. This type of hypothesis is used to test the validity of a research question by assuming that there is no significant effect or relationship between the variables under investigation. The null hypothesis is typically tested against an alternative hypothesis, which proposes that there is a significant effect or relationship between the variables. If the null hypothesis is rejected, it means that there is enough evidence to suggest that the alternative hypothesis is true, and the variables are indeed related or different from each other.

Non-Directional Hypothesis Example

Example: Is there a difference in anxiety levels between students who receive traditional classroom instruction and those who participate in online learning?

In this non-directional hypothesis, researchers are interested in understanding if there's a disparity in anxiety levels between students who are taught in traditional classrooms versus those who learn online. The non-directional hypothesis posits that there won't be any notable variance in anxiety levels between the two groups. This means that the researchers are not predicting whether one group will have higher or lower anxiety levels; rather, they are exploring if there's any difference at all.

Testimonials

Very satisfied students

This is our reason for working. We want to make all students happy, every day. Review us on Sitejabber

Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypotheses in Research

Both directional and non directional hypothesis have their place in research, and choosing the appropriate type depends on the research question being investigated. Researchers can use directional or non-directional hypotheses in their studies, depending on their specific expectations about the relationship between variables. A directional hypothesis predicts a specific direction of change, while a non-directional hypothesis predicts that there will be a difference between groups or conditions without specifying the direction of that difference. It's important to understand the difference between these types of hypotheses to conduct rigorous and insightful research. Directional hypotheses are useful when researchers want to test a specific expectation about the relationship between variables, while non-directional hypotheses are more appropriate when researchers simply want to test if there is any difference between groups or conditions.

How to Write an Effective Hypothesis in Research?

Writing an effective hypothesis involves several key steps to ensure clarity, testability, and relevance to the research question. Here's a guide on how to write an effective hypothesis:

  • Identify the Research Question: Start by clearly defining the research question or problem you want to investigate. Your hypothesis should directly address this question.
  • State the Null Hypothesis: The null hypothesis (H0) is a statement that there is no relationship or effect between the variables being studied. It serves as the default assumption and is typically stated as the absence of an effect or difference.
  • Formulate the Alternative Hypothesis: The alternative hypothesis (H1 or Ha) is the statement that contradicts the null hypothesis and suggests that there is a relationship or effect between the variables. It reflects what you expect to find in your research.
  • Make it Testable: Your hypothesis should be testable through empirical observation or experimentation. This means that there must be a way to collect data or evidence to support or refute the hypothesis.
  • Be Specific and Clear: Clearly state the variables involved and the expected relationship between them. Avoid vague or ambiguous language to ensure that your hypothesis is easy to understand and interpret.
  • Use Quantifiable Terms: Whenever possible, use quantifiable terms or measurable variables in your hypothesis. This makes it easier to collect data and analyze results objectively.
  • Consider the Scope: Ensure that your hypothesis is focused and specific to the research hypothesis at hand. Avoid making broad generalizations that are difficult to test or validate.
  • Revise and Refine: Once you've drafted your hypothesis, review it carefully to ensure accuracy and coherence. Revise as needed to clarify any ambiguities or inconsistencies.

Need Help with Academic Writing? Get a Response within 24 Hours!

How Does It Work ?

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Fill the Form

Please fill the free topic form and share your requirements

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

Writer Starts Working

The writer starts to find a topic for you (based on your requirements)

directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

3+ Topics Emailed!

The writer shared custom topics with you within 24 hours

In conclusion, directional hypotheses predict whether variables will increase or decrease, providing a definite expectation about the direction of the relationship under investigation. Non-directional hypotheses, on the other hand, only claim that there is a difference between variables without specifying the direction of the change, leaving it open to any possibility. Both types of hypotheses play an important role in guiding research investigations and developing testable predictions.

Get 3+ Free   Dissertation Topics within 24 hours?

Your Number

Academic Level Select Academic Level Undergraduate Masters PhD

Area of Research

Editor Arsalan

Editor Arsalan

Related posts.

Understanding TOK Concepts | A Beginner's Guide

Understanding TOK Concepts: A Beginner’s Guide

Is AP Psychology Hard? Exploring the Challenges and Rewards

Table of Abbreviations for Thesis: A Beginner’s Guide

Comments are closed.

psychology

Operational Hypothesis

An Operational Hypothesis is a testable statement or prediction made in research that not only proposes a relationship between two or more variables but also clearly defines those variables in operational terms, meaning how they will be measured or manipulated within the study. It forms the basis of an experiment that seeks to prove or disprove the assumed relationship, thus helping to drive scientific research.

The Core Components of an Operational Hypothesis

Understanding an operational hypothesis involves identifying its key components and how they interact.

The Variables

An operational hypothesis must contain two or more variables — factors that can be manipulated, controlled, or measured in an experiment.

The Proposed Relationship

Beyond identifying the variables, an operational hypothesis specifies the type of relationship expected between them. This could be a correlation, a cause-and-effect relationship, or another type of association.

The Importance of Operationalizing Variables

Operationalizing variables — defining them in measurable terms — is a critical step in forming an operational hypothesis. This process ensures the variables are quantifiable, enhancing the reliability and validity of the research.

Constructing an Operational Hypothesis

Creating an operational hypothesis is a fundamental step in the scientific method and research process. It involves generating a precise, testable statement that predicts the outcome of a study based on the research question. An operational hypothesis must clearly identify and define the variables under study and describe the expected relationship between them. The process of creating an operational hypothesis involves several key steps:

Steps to Construct an Operational Hypothesis

  • Define the Research Question : Start by clearly identifying the research question. This question should highlight the key aspect or phenomenon that the study aims to investigate.
  • Identify the Variables : Next, identify the key variables in your study. Variables are elements that you will measure, control, or manipulate in your research. There are typically two types of variables in a hypothesis: the independent variable (the cause) and the dependent variable (the effect).
  • Operationalize the Variables : Once you’ve identified the variables, you must operationalize them. This involves defining your variables in such a way that they can be easily measured, manipulated, or controlled during the experiment.
  • Predict the Relationship : The final step involves predicting the relationship between the variables. This could be an increase, decrease, or any other type of correlation between the independent and dependent variables.

By following these steps, you will create an operational hypothesis that provides a clear direction for your research, ensuring that your study is grounded in a testable prediction.

Evaluating the Strength of an Operational Hypothesis

Not all operational hypotheses are created equal. The strength of an operational hypothesis can significantly influence the validity of a study. There are several key factors that contribute to the strength of an operational hypothesis:

  • Clarity : A strong operational hypothesis is clear and unambiguous. It precisely defines all variables and the expected relationship between them.
  • Testability : A key feature of an operational hypothesis is that it must be testable. That is, it should predict an outcome that can be observed and measured.
  • Operationalization of Variables : The operationalization of variables contributes to the strength of an operational hypothesis. When variables are clearly defined in measurable terms, it enhances the reliability of the study.
  • Alignment with Research : Finally, a strong operational hypothesis aligns closely with the research question and the overall goals of the study.

By carefully crafting and evaluating an operational hypothesis, researchers can ensure that their work provides valuable, valid, and actionable insights.

Examples of Operational Hypotheses

To illustrate the concept further, this section will provide examples of well-constructed operational hypotheses in various research fields.

The operational hypothesis is a fundamental component of scientific inquiry, guiding the research design and providing a clear framework for testing assumptions. By understanding how to construct and evaluate an operational hypothesis, we can ensure our research is both rigorous and meaningful.

Examples of Operational Hypothesis:

  • In Education : An operational hypothesis in an educational study might be: “Students who receive tutoring (Independent Variable) will show a 20% improvement in standardized test scores (Dependent Variable) compared to students who did not receive tutoring.”
  • In Psychology : In a psychological study, an operational hypothesis could be: “Individuals who meditate for 20 minutes each day (Independent Variable) will report a 15% decrease in self-reported stress levels (Dependent Variable) after eight weeks compared to those who do not meditate.”
  • In Health Science : An operational hypothesis in a health science study might be: “Participants who drink eight glasses of water daily (Independent Variable) will show a 10% decrease in reported fatigue levels (Dependent Variable) after three weeks compared to those who drink four glasses of water daily.”
  • In Environmental Science : In an environmental study, an operational hypothesis could be: “Cities that implement recycling programs (Independent Variable) will see a 25% reduction in landfill waste (Dependent Variable) after one year compared to cities without recycling programs.”

IMAGES

  1. PPT

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  2. 13 Different Types of Hypothesis (2024)

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  3. Directional Hypothesis: Definition and 10 Examples (2024)

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  4. PPT

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  5. PPT

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

  6. Using Theories in Psychological Research

    directional hypothesis in psychological experiments

VIDEO

  1. Types of Hypothesis difference between Directional hypothesis and Non-directional hypothesis?

  2. Hypothesis Testing in Psychological Research

  3. Psychology Experiments, அறிவியலும் உளவியலும் (Ep2) Basic Psychology in Tamil

  4. Human Resources: 7/9 'Social Engineering in the 20th Century'

  5. Research Hypothesis and its Types with examples /urdu/hindi

  6. Schizophrenia| A2 Level

COMMENTS

  1. Research Hypothesis In Psychology: Types, & Examples

    Examples. A research hypothesis, in its plural form "hypotheses," is a specific, testable prediction about the anticipated results of a study, established at its outset. It is a key component of the scientific method. Hypotheses connect theory to data and guide the research process towards expanding scientific understanding.

  2. Directional Hypothesis: Definition and 10 Examples

    Directional Hypothesis Examples. 1. Exercise and Heart Health. Research suggests that as regular physical exercise (independent variable) increases, the risk of heart disease (dependent variable) decreases (Jakicic, Davis, Rogers, King, Marcus, Helsel, Rickman, Wahed, Belle, 2016). In this example, a directional hypothesis anticipates that the ...

  3. Directional Hypothesis

    Definition: A directional hypothesis is a specific type of hypothesis statement in which the researcher predicts the direction or effect of the relationship between two variables. Key Features. 1. Predicts direction: Unlike a non-directional hypothesis, which simply states that there is a relationship between two variables, a directional ...

  4. What is a Directional Hypothesis? (Definition & Examples)

    A statistical hypothesis is an assumption about a population parameter.For example, we may assume that the mean height of a male in the U.S. is 70 inches. The assumption about the height is the statistical hypothesis and the true mean height of a male in the U.S. is the population parameter.. To test whether a statistical hypothesis about a population parameter is true, we obtain a random ...

  5. Hypotheses; directional and non-directional

    The directional hypothesis can also state a negative correlation, e.g. the higher the number of face-book friends, the lower the life satisfaction score ". Non-directional hypothesis: A non-directional (or two tailed hypothesis) simply states that there will be a difference between the two groups/conditions but does not say which will be ...

  6. Aims And Hypotheses, Directional And Non-Directional

    If the findings do support the hypothesis then the hypothesis can be retained (i.e., accepted), but if not, then it must be rejected. Three Different Hypotheses: (1) Directional Hypothesis: states that the IV will have an effect on the DV and what that effect will be (the direction of results). For example, eating smarties will significantly ...

  7. How to Write a Directional Hypothesis: A Step-by-Step Guide

    Review your directional hypothesis to ensure that it accurately reflects your research question and predictions. Make any necessary revisions to improve clarity and specificity. Conclusion. Writing a directional hypothesis is an essential skill for researchers conducting experiments and investigations.

  8. 7.2.2 Hypothesis

    The Experimental Hypothesis: Directional A directional experimental hypothesis (also known as one-tailed) predicts the direction of the change/difference (it anticipates more specifically what might happen); A directional hypothesis is usually used when there is previous research which support a particular theory or outcome i.e. what a researcher might expect to happen

  9. Directional and non-directional hypothesis: A Comprehensive Guide

    Examples of research questions for directional hypothesis. In the world of research and statistical analysis, hypotheses play a crucial role in formulating and testing scientific claims. Understanding the differences between directional and non-directional hypothesis is essential for designing sound experiments and drawing accurate conclusions.

  10. PDF Chapter 6: Research methods Hypotheses: directional or non-directional

    assume a hypothesis is directional when in fact it is non-directional. For example, everyone knows the more you revise, the better you do in exams but a hypothesis may say 'There is a difference in the exam results between those who revise a lot and those who do not revise' and this is, of course, a non-directional hypothesis. Extension task

  11. Chapter 9 Hypothesis testing

    9. Chapter 9 Hypothesis testing. The first unit was designed to prepare you for hypothesis testing. In the first chapter we discussed the three major goals of statistics: Describe: connects to unit 1 with descriptive statistics and graphing. Decide: connects to unit 1 knowing your data and hypothesis testing.

  12. Introduction to Psychology/Research Methods in Psychology

    Psychometrics is the field of psychology concerned with the theory and technique of psychological measurement, which includes the measurement of knowledge, abilities, attitudes, interests, achievement in particular degree or course, and personality traits (Carl Dellomos, 2009). Measurement of these unobservable phenomena is difficult, and much ...

  13. Developing a Hypothesis

    Theories and Hypotheses. Before describing how to develop a hypothesis, it is important to distinguish between a theory and a hypothesis. A theory is a coherent explanation or interpretation of one or more phenomena. Although theories can take a variety of forms, one thing they have in common is that they go beyond the phenomena they explain by including variables, structures, processes ...

  14. Aims and Hypotheses

    Hypotheses. A hypothesis (plural hypotheses) is a precise, testable statement of what the researchers predict will be the outcome of the study. This usually involves proposing a possible relationship between two variables: the independent variable (what the researcher changes) and the dependant variable (what the research measures).

  15. Directional Hypothesis

    A directional hypothesis is a one-tailed hypothesis that states the direction of the difference or relationship (e.g. boys are more helpful than girls). ... Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June 2018 (AQA) Exam Support. Example Answer for Question 14 Paper 2: AS Psychology, June 2017 (AQA) ...

  16. Aims and Hypotheses

    The theory attempting to explain an observation will help to inform hypotheses - predictions of an investigation's outcome that make specific reference to the independent variables (IVs) manipulated and dependent variables (DVs) measured by the researchers. There are two types of hypothesis: H1 - The Research Hypothesis.

  17. Hypotheses AO1 AO2

    EXEMPLAR ESSAYHow to write a 8-mark answer. Assess how hypotheses are used in the Cognitive Approach. (8 marks) A 8-mark "apply" question awards 4 marks for describing the use of hypotheses (AO1) and 4 marks for applying the Cognitive Approach to this (AO2). You need a conclusion to get a mark in the top band (7-8 marks).

  18. 8.4: The Alternative Hypothesis

    Thus, our alternative hypothesis is the mathematical way of stating our research question. If we expect our obtained sample mean to be above or below the null hypothesis value, which we call a directional hypothesis, then our alternative hypothesis takes the form: HA: μ > 7.47 or HA: μ < 7.47 H A: μ > 7.47 or H A: μ < 7.47.

  19. PDF Task 4

    When designing experiments psychologists need to have ... Task 1: Without knowing much about how to write a hypothesis in psychology, try and write a hypothesis for this research aim: investigating the power of uniforms in ... I can write a non-directional hypothesis I can write a directional hypothesis Don [t worry if you have ticked any of ...

  20. Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypothesis in Research

    Conclusion. Formulating hypotheses is an essential step in the research process, guiding researchers in testing relationships between variables. Directional hypotheses offer specific predictions about the expected direction of the relationship, whereas non-directional hypotheses allow for more exploratory investigations without preconceived ...

  21. PDF Aice 9990 Psychology Research Methods: Experimental & Non-experimental

    A field experiment is: conducted in more everyday/natural environment. such as classroom, mall, street, etc. an experiment where subjects may not know they are being observed. the only field study from the 12 core study is Piliavin because the study takes place on the NY subway. Advantages of field experiments:

  22. Research Hypotheses: Directional vs. Non-Directional Hypotheses

    A directional hypothesis predicts a specific direction of change, while a non-directional hypothesis predicts that there will be a difference between groups or conditions without specifying the direction of that difference. It's important to understand the difference between these types of hypotheses to conduct rigorous and insightful research ...

  23. Operational Hypothesis

    An Operational Hypothesis is a testable statement or prediction made in research that not only proposes a relationship between two or more variables but also clearly defines those variables in operational terms, meaning how they will be measured or manipulated within the study. It forms the basis of an experiment that seeks to prove or disprove ...