REVIEW article
Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: biological and psychological benefits.
- 1 Department of Movement Sciences and Wellbeing, Parthenope University of Naples, Naples, Italy
- 2 IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
- 3 Istituto di Diagnosi e Cura Hermitage Capodimonte, Naples, Italy
- 4 Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, Magna Graecia University, Catanzaro, Italy
- 5 Department of Science and Technology, Parthenope University of Naples, Naples, Italy
- 6 Department of Engineering, Parthenope University of Naples, Naples, Italy
- 7 Institute of Applied Sciences and Intelligent Systems, CNR, Pozzuoli, Italy
Much evidence shows that physical exercise (PE) is a strong gene modulator that induces structural and functional changes in the brain, determining enormous benefit on both cognitive functioning and wellbeing. PE is also a protective factor for neurodegeneration. However, it is unclear if such protection is granted through modifications to the biological mechanisms underlying neurodegeneration or through better compensation against attacks. This concise review addresses the biological and psychological positive effects of PE describing the results obtained on brain plasticity and epigenetic mechanisms in animal and human studies, in order to clarify how to maximize the positive effects of PE while avoiding negative consequences, as in the case of exercise addiction.
Introduction
Many evidences demonstrated that physical exercise (PE) affects brain plasticity, influencing cognition and wellbeing ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ; for review see Fernandes et al., 2017 ). In fact, experimental and clinical studies have reported that PE induces structural and functional changes in the brain, determining enormous biological, and psychological benefits.
In general, when reported PE effects, it is customary to separate the biological aspects from the psychological ones. In fact, most of the studies documented either the effects of PE on the brain (and then on the cognitive functioning) or on the wellbeing (in terms of physical and mental health). In this review, we merge both these aspects as they influence each other. In fact, behaviorally appropriate choices depend upon efficient cognitive functioning. Furthermore, emotional states influence cognitive functions through specific cerebral circuitry involving prefrontal areas and limbic structures ( Barbas, 2000 ).
Before analyzing the benefits of PE, it is necessary to define PE precisely. Indeed, PE is a term often incorrectly used interchangeably with physical activity (PA) that is “any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that requires energy expenditure” ( World Health Organization, 2010 ). Then, PA includes any motor behavior such as daily and leisure activities and it is considered a determinant lifestyle for general health status ( Burkhalter and Hillman, 2011 ). Instead, PE is “a sub classification of PA that is planned, structured, repetitive, and has as a final or an intermediate objective the improvement or maintenance of one or more components of physical fitness” ( World Health Organization, 2010 ). Examples of PE are aerobic and anaerobic activity, characterized by a precise frequency, duration and intensity.
In this review, we illustrate the biological and psychological benefits of PE on cognition and wellbeing both in health and diseases, reporting data from both animal and human studies. The biological basis at both molecular and supramolecular level have been largely studied. The other aim of present work is to report the actual evidence on the epigenetic mechanisms that determine or modulate the biological effects of PE on the brain. In fact, while the biologic mechanisms are sufficiently studied both at the molecular and supramolecular levels (see Lista and Sorrentino, 2010 ), little is known about the epigenetic ones. Finally, the modality with which PE should be practiced to gain such advantages while avoiding negative consequences will be discussed. In Table 1 are reported the inclusion and exclusion criteria for studies discussed in this review.
Table 1 . Inclusion and exclusion criteria for studies included in this review.
Physical Exercise, Brain, and Cognition
Among the biological effects of PE, those linked to “neuroplasticity” are quite important.
Neuroplasticity is an important feature of the nervous system, which can modify itself in response to experience ( Bavelier and Neville, 2002 ). For this reason, PE may be considered as an enhancer environmental factor promoting neuroplasticity.
In animal studies, the structural changes analyzed concern the cellular (neurogenesis, gliogenesis, synaptogenesis, angiogenesis) and molecular (alteration in neurotransmission systems and increasing in some neurotrophic factors) level ( Gelfo et al., 2018 ), while the functional activity has been measured using the levels of performance in behavioral tasks, such as spatial tasks that allow to analyze the different facets of spatial cognitive functions ( Mandolesi et al., 2017 ). In humans, indicators of structural changes correspond for example to brain volumes, measures of white matter integrity or modulation in neurotrophins levels (by correlation with trophic factors plasma levels). Such metrics can be correlated to cognitive performances, defining the functional neural efficiency ( Serra et al., 2011 ). To this regard, it should be emphasized that any morphological change results in a modification of the functional properties of a neural circuit and vice versa any change in neuronal efficiency and functionality is based on morphological modifications ( Mandolesi et al., 2017 ).
Experimental and clinical studies have shown that PE induces important structural and functional changes in brain functioning. In Table 2 are reported the more evident effects induced by PE.
Table 2 . Structural and functional effects of PE.
Animal Studies
In animals, motor activity or motor exercise are terms often used instead of PE. The effects of motor exercise are mainly studied in rodents by means of specific training on wheels or by locomotor activity analyses.
Studies on healthy animals have demonstrated that intense motor activity increases neurons and glia cells proliferation rates in the hippocampus and the neocortex ( van Praag et al., 1999a , b ; Brown et al., 2003 ; Ehninger and Kempermann, 2003 ; Steiner et al., 2004 ; Hirase and Shinohara, 2014 ) and induces angiogenesis in the neocortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum ( Black et al., 1990 ; Isaacs et al., 1992 ; Kleim et al., 2002 ; Swain et al., 2003 ; Ekstrand et al., 2008 ; Gelfo et al., 2018 ). At the molecular level, motor activity causes changes in neurotrasmitters such as serotonin, noradrenalin, and acetylcholine ( Lista and Sorrentino, 2010 ; for a review, see Lin and Kuo, 2013 ) and induces the release of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF Vaynman et al., 2004 ; Lafenetre et al., 2011 ) and the insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1; for a review, van Praag, 2009 ).
Animals performing motor exercise showed improvements in spatial abilities ( van Praag et al., 2005 ; Snigdha et al., 2014 ) and in other cognitive domains such as executive functions ( Langdon and Corbett, 2012 ), evidencing thus that motor exercise improve cognitive functions.
Similar structural and functional changes were evident even in older animals ( Kronenberg et al., 2006 ) and in animal models of neurodegenerative diseases ( Nithianantharajah and Hannan, 2006 ), suggesting that motor exercise is a potent neuroprotective factor against physiological and pathological aging ( Gelfo et al., 2018 ). In this context, one can use transgenic models to determine exactly when a structural alteration occurs, and then to study when the animals should undergo motor training in order to maximize its effects. To this regard, converging evidence is showing that motor activity should be performed before the development of neurodegeneration in order to exert its protective role ( Richter et al., 2008 ; Lin et al., 2015 ) such as before the formation of beta amyloid plaques in Alzheimer's disease ( Adlard et al., 2005 ). However, there are some experimental evidences showing that motor exercise performed after neurodegenerative lesions permits to improve spatial abilities, hence being also a potent therapeutic agent ( Sim, 2014 ; Ji et al., 2015 ).
Interestingly, PE induces modifications that can be passed on to the offspring. In fact, positive maternal experiences can influence the offspring at both behavioral and biochemical levels (see Cutuli et al., 2017 , 2018 ). Preclinical studies also indicated that the effects of maternal exercise during pregnancy can be passed on to offspring ( Robinson et al., 2012 ). However, it is not clear if the possibilities of inheritance are limited to motor exercise alone. To this regard, it has been seen that pregnant rats exposed to motor exercise on wheel-running and treadmill running have offspring with improved spatial memory, and increased hippocampal BDNF level ( Akhavan et al., 2008 ; Aksu et al., 2012 ). However, further studies are necessary since it remains unclear whether these beneficial effects result from physiological changes to the in utero environment and/or from epigenetic modifications to the developing embryo ( Short et al., 2017 ). On the other hand, few studies, conflicting and hard to replicate, do not yet allow to explore the transgenerational effects of paternal motor exercise ( Short et al., 2017 ).
Human Studies
Neuroplasticity phenomena following PE have been evidenced even in humans. A great number of studies demonstrated that in adults, PE determines structural changes such as increased gray matter volume in frontal and hippocampal regions ( Colcombe et al., 2006 ; Erickson et al., 2011 ) and reduced damage in the gray matter ( Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2014 ).
Moreover, PE facilitates the release of neurotrophic factors such as peripheral BDNF ( Hötting et al., 2016 ), increases blood flow, improves cerebrovascular health and determines benefits on glucose and lipid metabolism carrying “food” to the brain ( Mandolesi et al., 2017 ).
These effects are reflected on cognitive functioning (for a review see Hötting and Röder, 2013 ). In fact, the results of cross-sectional and epidemiological studies showed that PE enhances cognitive functions in young and older adults ( Lista and Sorrentino, 2010 ; Fernandes et al., 2017 ), improving memory abilities, efficiency of attentional processes and executive-control processes ( Kramer et al., 1999 ; Colcombe and Kramer, 2003 ; Grego et al., 2005 ; Pereira et al., 2007 ; Winter et al., 2007 ; Chieffi et al., 2017 ). Furthermore, structural changes following PE have been related to academic achievement in comparison to sedentary individuals ( Lees and Hopkins, 2013 ; Donnelly et al., 2016 ). In this line, it has been also showed that children who practice regular aerobic activity performed better on verbal, perceptual and arithmetic test in comparison to sedentary ones of same age ( Sibley and Etnier, 2003 ; Voss et al., 2011 ).
Numerous studies have demonstrated that PE prevents cognitive decline linked to aging ( Yaffe et al., 2009 ; Hötting and Röder, 2013 ; Niemann et al., 2014 ), reduces the risk of developing dementia ( Colberg et al., 2008 ; Mandolesi et al., 2017 ), the level of deterioration in executive functions ( Hollamby et al., 2017 ) and improves the quality of life ( Pedrinolla et al., 2017 ). Furthermore, positron emission tomography based studies evidenced that PE determines changes in metabolic networks that are related to cognition ( Huang et al., 2016 ).
Recently, studies on magnetoencephalography based (MEG) functional connectivity evidenced that PE influences network topology ( Foster, 2015 ). It is important to underlie that MEG is a much more direct measure of neural activity in comparison to fRMI, with the advantage of combining good spatial and high temporal resolution. In healthy individuals, PE was related to better intermodular integration ( Douw et al., 2014 ) and to improvements in cognitive functions ( Huang et al., 2016 ). Benefits of PE are evidenced even in individuals at risk for AD ( Deeny et al., 2008 ), thus once again suggesting a protective role of PE.
A possible explanation for these ameliorative structural and functional effects could be that PE stimulates blood circulation in the neural circuits involved in cognitive functioning ( Erickson et al., 2012 ). Another interpretation could be found in the concept of “cerebral reserves” ( Stern, 2002 , 2012 ) a mechanisms that might explain why, in the face of neurodegenerative changes that are similar in nature and extent, individuals vary considerably in the severity of cognitive aging and clinical dementia ( Petrosini et al., 2009 ). Two types of reserves are recognized: brain reserve and cognitive reserve. The former is based on the protective potential of anatomical features such as brain size, neuronal density and synaptic connectivity, the latter is based on the efficient connectivity among neural circuits ( Stern, 2002 ; Mandolesi et al., 2017 ).
According to the reserves hypothesis and taking into account the numerous evidences described above, we could advance that PE is an environmental factor that permits to gain reserves.
However, one must underline that if on the one hand PE improves the cognitive functioning, providing reserves to be spent in the case of a brain lesion, on the other hand the modifications of the clinical expression of neurodegeneration delays the diagnosis. It has been seen that patients with higher cognitive reserve take longer to manifest the symptoms of memory loss ( Zanetti et al., 2017 ). It has been hypothesized a neural compensation mechanism that permits to perform complex activities ( Stern, 2009 ). Obviously, these conclusions open important reflections more for the diagnosis of neurodegenerative disease than for the practice of PE.
The effects of PE on cognitive functioning have been shown across the lifespan from childhood to the old age ( Hötting and Röder, 2013 ). In particular, it has been evidenced that cognitive functions that are influenced the most by brain maturation, such as attention or cognitive flexibility, and the cognitive functions that depend the most upon experiences, such as memory, are the most sensitive ones to PE ( Hötting and Röder, 2013 ). Overall, these studies, together with those analyzing the effects of combined environmental factors, suggest that for a positive effect on cognitive function, it is necessary to maintain an “enriched lifestyle” up to middle life. In fact, the exposure to PE together to other many experiences provides a “reserve”-like advantage which supports an enduring preservation of cognitive function in old age ( Chang et al., 2010 ; Loprinzi et al., 2018 ).
Physical Exercise and Wellbeing
There are consistent evidences that PE has many benefits for people of any age, improving psychological wellbeing ( Zubala et al., 2017 ) and quality of life ( Penedo and Dahn, 2005 ; Windle et al., 2010 ; Table 3 ).
Table 3 . Biological and psychological effects of PE (Adapted from Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ).
In children, PE is correlated with high levels of self-efficacy, tasks goal orientation, and perceived competence ( Biddle et al., 2011 ). In youth and adulthood, most studies evidenced that PE is associated with better health outcomes, such as better mood and self-concept ( Berger and Motl, 2001 ; Landers and Arent, 2001 ; Penedo and Dahn, 2005 ). In the aging population, PE helps maintaining independence ( Stessman et al., 2009 ), favoring social relations and mental health.
It was now well-accepted that is the interaction between biological and psychological mechanisms linked to PE enhances the wellbeing ( Penedo and Dahn, 2005 ). Biological mechanisms of beneficial effects of PE are mainly related to increasing in cerebral blood flow and in maximal oxygen consumption, to delivery of oxygen to cerebral tissue, to reduction in muscle tension and to increased serum concentrations of endocannabinoid receptors ( Thomas et al., 1989 ; Dietrich and McDaniel, 2004 ; Querido and Sheel, 2007 ; Gomes da Silva et al., 2010 ; Ferreira-Vieira et al., 2014 ). Moreover, neuroplasticity phenomena such as changes in neurotransmitters are recognized to affect wellbeing. For example, PE increases the levels of serotonin ( Young, 2007 ; Korb et al., 2010 ) and the levels of beta-endorphins, such as anandamide ( Fuss et al., 2015 ).
Among the psychological hypothesis proposed to explain how PE enhances wellbeing, it has been underlined feeling of control ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ), competency and self-efficacy ( Craft, 2005 ; Rodgers et al., 2014 ), improved self-concept and self-esteem ( Marsh and Sonstroem, 1995 ; Fox, 2000 ; Zamani Sani et al., 2016 ), positive social interactions and opportunities for fun and enjoyment ( Raedeke, 2007 ; Bartlett et al., 2011 ).
Psychological research evidenced that PE can even modulate the personality and the development of Self ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ). Moreover, PE has been correlated with hardiness, a personality style that enables a person to withstand or cope with stressful situations ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ).
In the following sections, we will focus on correlations among PE and the most common mental illnesses.
Depression and Anxiety
Depression is the most common type of mental illness and will be the second leading cause of disease by 2020 ( Farioli-Vecchioli et al., 2018 ). Similar entity concerns anxiety disorders that are among the most prevalent mental disorders in the world population ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ).
Epidemiological studies have consistently reported benefits of PE on reductions in depression ( Mammen and Faulkner, 2013 ) and anxiety ( DeBoer et al., 2012 ). For example, it has been seen that individuals that practice PE regularly are less depressed or anxious than those who do not ( De Moor et al., 2006 ), suggesting the use of exercise as a treatment for these illnesses ( Carek et al., 2011 ).
Most of the research on the relationship between PE and positive changes in mood state has evidenced positive effects, especially as a consequence of aerobic exercise, regardless of the specific type of activity ( Knapen et al., 2009 ), even if the correct intensity of aerobic PE to control and reduce symptoms is debated ( de Souza Moura et al., 2015 ). For example, it has been revealed that after about 16 weeks of an aerobic exercise program, individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD), significantly reduced their depressive symptoms ( Craft and Perna, 2004 ). However, there are evidenced that documented that even anaerobic activity has positive effects on treatment of clinical depression ( Martinsen, 1990 ). For anxiety disorders, it has been evidenced that the positive effects of PE are visible even with short bursts of exercise, independently from the nature of the exercise ( Scully et al., 1998 ).
A physiologic mechanism correlated to the improvement in depressed mood post-exercise PE was identified in modulation of peripheral levels of BDNF ( Coelho et al., 2013 ). In this line, it was suggested recently that the intensity of exercise to improve mood should be prescribed on individual basis and not on the patient's preferred intensity ( Meyer et al., 2016a , b ). Conversely, physical inactivity correlated to worse depressive symptoms and, then, to lower peripheral levels of BDNF ( Brunoni et al., 2008 ). Post-PE mood improvement might also be due to lower oxidative stress ( Thomson et al., 2015 ). In this contest, it was evidenced that there is an abnormal oxidative stress in individuals with MDD or bipolar disorder ( Cataldo et al., 2010 ; Andreazza et al., 2013 ) and that PE, particularly in higher intensity, decreases oxidative stress with consequent mood improvement ( Urso and Clarkson, 2003 ).
Addictive and Unhealthy Behaviors
PE has been widely evidenced to be an effective tool for treating several addictive and unhealthy behaviors. PE tends to reduce and prevent behaviors such as smoking, alcohol, and gambling, and to regulate the impulse for hunger and satiety ( Vatansever-Ozen et al., 2011 ; Tiryaki-Sonmez et al., 2015 ). In this context, several studies evidenced substance abusers benefit from regular PE, that also helps increasing healthy behaviors ( Giesen et al., 2015 ). It has been evidenced that regular PE reduces tobacco cravings and cigarette use ( Haasova et al., 2013 ). Although PE has positive effects on psychological wellbeing, in this context it is right underline that in some cases PE could reveal unhealthy behaviors with negative consequence on health ( Schwellnus et al., 2016 ). It is the case of exercise addiction, a dependence on a regular regimen of exercise that is characterized by withdrawal symptoms, after 24–36 h without exercise ( Sachs, 1981 ), such as anxiety, irritability, guilt, muscle twitching, a bloated feeling, and nervousness ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ). There is a strong correlation between exercise addiction and eating disorders ( Scully et al., 1998 ) suggesting thus a comorbidity of these disorders and a common biological substrate. In particular, recent studies have shown that these unhealthy behaviors are associated to lower prefrontal cortex volume, activity and oxygenation, with consequent impairment in cognitive functions, such as the inhibitory control with the consequent compulsive behaviors ( Asensio et al., 2016 ; Wang et al., 2016 ; Pahng et al., 2017 ). Also, it has been seen that a few days of PE increase oxygenation of prefrontal cortex, improving mental health ( Cabral et al., 2017 ).
Epigenetic Mechanisms
Biological and psychological effects of PE could be partly explained through epigenetic mechanisms. The term “epigenetics,” coined by Waddington (1939) , is based on a conceptual model designed to account for how genes might interact with their environment to produce the phenotype ( Waddington, 1939 ; Fernandes et al., 2017 ).
In particular, epigenetics is referred to all those mechanisms, including functional modifications of the genome such as DNA methylation, post-translational histone modifications (i.e., acetylation and methylation) and microRNA expression ( Deibel et al., 2015 ; Grazioli et al., 2017 ), which tend to regulate gene expression, modeling the chromatin structure but maintaining the nucleotide sequence of DNA unchanged.
The current literature clearly demonstrates that these mechanisms are strongly influenced by different biological and environmental factors, such as PE ( Grazioli et al., 2017 ), which determine the nature and the mode of epigenetic mechanisms activation.
Epigenetics plays an essential role in neural reorganization, including those that govern the brain plasticity ( Deibel et al., 2015 ). For example, a growing body of evidence indicates that regulates neuroplasticity and memory processes ( Ieraci et al., 2015 ).
Several animal studies reveal how motor activity is able to improve cognitive performances acting on epigenetic mechanisms and influencing the expression of those genes involved in neuroplasticity ( Fernandes et al., 2017 ). The main molecular processes that underlie the epigenetic mechanisms are the following: through DNA methylation, histone modifications and microRNA expression ( Fernandes et al., 2017 ).
DNA methylation is a chemical covalent modification on the cytosine of the double stranded DNA molecule. It has been recognized that DNA methylation plays a key role in long-term memory ( Deibel et al., 2015 ; Kim and Kaang, 2017 ). In particular, mechanisms related to DNA methylation relieve the repressive effects of memory-suppressor genes to favor the expression of plasticity-promoting and memory consolidation genes. Several evidences showed that PE is able to coordinate the action of the genes involved in synaptic plasticity that regulate memory consolidation ( Molteni et al., 2002 ; Ding et al., 2006 ).
Histone modifications are post-translational chemical changes in histone proteins. They include histone methylation/demethylation, acetylation/deacetylation, and phosphorylation, all due to the activity of specific enzymes, which modify the chromatin structure, thereby regulating gene expression. It has been demonstrated that histone acetylation is a requisite for long-term memory (LTM) ( Barrett and Wood, 2008 ; Fernandes et al., 2017 ). In animals, motor activity increases these genetic mechanisms in the hippocampus and the frontal cortex, improving memory performances in behavioral tasks ( Cechinel et al., 2016 ). Recently, following 4 weeks of motor exercise, it has been evidenced an increasing of the activity of enzymes involved in histone acetylation/deacetylation, the epigenetic mechanisms that determine an enhancing in the expression of BDNF ( Maejima et al., 2018 ).
MicroRNAs (miRNAs) are small, single stranded RNA molecules able to inhibit the expression of target genes. They are widely expressed in the brain, participating in epigenetic mechanisms and acting as regulators of numerous biological processes in the brain, ranging from cell proliferation, differentiation, apoptosis, synaptic plasticity, and memory consolidation ( Saab and Mansuy, 2014 ). Recent evidences demonstrate that PE can mitigate the harmful effects of traumatic brain injury and aging on cognitive function by regulating the hippocampal expression of miR21 ( Hu et al., 2015 ) and miR-34a ( Kou et al., 2017 ). Furthermore, PE contributes to attenuate the effects of stress-related increase in miR-124, involved in neurogenesis and memory formation ( Pan-Vazquez et al., 2015 ).
What Kind of Physical Exercise?
Sport psychology has suggested that the success or failure of PE programs depends on several factors such as the intensity, frequency, duration of the exercise, and whether the PE is done in group or alone ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ). These aspects are important in terms of maintenance of PE practice and in order to gain benefits for brain and behavior, and they are affected by individual characteristics. Although such aspects have to be taken into account when training is proposed, scientific reports have evidenced different effects on cognitive functioning and wellbeing if PE is performed in aerobic or anaerobic modality.
Aerobic exercise allows the resynthesis of adenosine—triphosphate (ATP) by aerobic mechanisms, adjusting intensity (from low to high intensity), duration (usually long), and oxygen availability. The intensity depends on the cardiorespiratory effort with respect to the maximum heart rate (HRmax) or the maximum oxygen consumption (Vo2max), which determines an increase in oxygen consumption with respect to the rest condition. Examples of aerobic PE are jogging, running, cycling, and swimming.
On the contrary, anaerobic exercise has high intensity, short duration and unavailability of oxygen, determining the depletion of the muscles' ATP and/or phosphocreatine (PCr) reserves, shifting the production of ATP, to anaerobic energy mechanisms, lactacid or alactacid. Examples of anaerobic exercises are weight lifting or sprint in 100 m.
Robust literature demonstrated that chronic aerobic exercise is associated with potent structural and functional neuroplastic changes, with an improvement in cognitive functions ( Colcombe et al., 2006 ; Hillman et al., 2008 ; Erickson et al., 2009 ; Mandolesi et al., 2017 ) and increased feeling of general wellbeing ( Berger and Tobar, 2011 ; Biddle et al., 2011 ) (Table 4 ).
Table 4 . Effects of physical aerobic exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing.
Recently, growing evidence showed that acute aerobic exercise, defined as a single bout of exercise, relates to improved cognitive functions, especially prefrontal cortex-dependent cognition ( Tomporowski, 2003 ; Lambourne and Tomporowski, 2010 ; Chang et al., 2011 ; Ludyga et al., 2016 ; Basso and Suzuki, 2017 ). However, the effects of a single session of exercise on cognitive functioning are generally small ( Chang et al., 2012 ). In this line, it was evidenced that even a single bout of moderate-intensity aerobic exercise enhances, mood and emotional states and improves the wellbeing in MDD individuals ( Bartholomew et al., 2005 ; Basso and Suzuki, 2017 ) (Table 4 ).
Beside frequency and duration over time, even the intensity is a parameter to be considered when evaluating the PE effects. It has been showed that moderate intensity exercise is related to increased performance in working memory and cognitive flexibility, whereas high-intensity exercise improves the speed of information processing ( Chang and Etnier, 2009 ). In this context, it has been reported that peripheral BDNF was significantly increased after high intensity exercise, but not after low-intensity exercise ( Hötting et al., 2016 ). In fact, it is evidenced that high-intensity exercise provides greater benefit to cognitive functions than low-intensity exercise in the elderly ( Brown et al., 2012 ).
With regard to the psychological beneficial effects related to PE, research has evidenced that major benefits in reduction of anxiety and depression are determined by longer training program (several months), as compared to shorter ones (some days) for training session lasting over 30 min. Moreover, anxiety and depression reduction after aerobic exercise may be achieved with exercise intensity between 30 and 70% of maximal heart rate ( Weinberg and Gould, 2015 ). To achieve positive mood changes, an important role is played even by anaerobic activity, such as yoga, or in all PEs in which there is rhythmic abdominal breathing, enjoyment, rhythmic, and repetitive movements and relative absence of interpersonal competition ( Berger and Motl, 2001 ).
PE determines positive biological and psychological effects that affect the brain and the cognitive functioning and promote a condition of wellbeing. PE plays an important role in counteract normal and pathological aging. Recent evidences have shown that PE triggers potent neuroplastic phenomena, partly mediated by epigenetic mechanisms. In fact, PE cause profound alterations in gene expression and its protein products in the form of epigenomic manifestations ( Fernandes et al., 2017 ).
A growing body of literature indicates that both chronic and aerobic PE can achieve similar benefits.
These results should lead to reflect on beneficial effects of PE and to promote its use as a modifiable factor for prevention, to improve cognitive abilities and to enhance mood.
Despite all these positive effects, it must be underlined that PE should be tailored to the individual. In fact, even PE, when excessive, can have a dark side, when PE becomes compulsive and facilitates addictive behaviors.
Author Contributions
LM, AP, SM, FF, GF, PS, and GS: designed the review; LM and GS: wrote the paper. All authors read, revised, and approved the final manuscript.
The present paper was supported by University of Naples Parthenope Ricerca Competitiva 2017 (D.R. 289/2017) to LM and GF.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Adlard, P. A., Perreau, V. M., Pop, V., and Cotman, C. W. (2005). Voluntary exercise decreases amyloid load in a transgenic model of Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurosci. 25, 4217–4221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0496-05.2005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Akhavan, M. M., Emami-Abarghoie, M., Safari, M., Sadighi-Moghaddam, B., Vafaei, A. A., Bandegi, A. R., et al. (2008). Serotonergic and noradrenergic lesions suppress the enhancing effect of maternal exercise during pregnancy on learning and memory in rat pups. Neuroscience 151, 1173–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.10.051
Aksu, I., Baykara, B., Ozbal, S., Cetin, F., Sisman, A. R., Dayi, A., et al. (2012). Maternal treadmill exercise during pregnancy decreases anxiety and increases prefrontal cortex VEGF and BDNF levels of rat pups in early and late periods of life. Neurosci. Lett. 516, 221–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.091
Andreazza, A. C., Wang, J. F., Salmasi, F., Shao, L., and Young, L. T. (2013). Specific subcellular changes in oxidative stress in prefrontal cortex from patients with bipolar disorder. J. Neurochem. 127, 552–561. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12316
Asensio, S., Morales, J. L., Senabre, I., Romero, M. J., Beltran, M. A., Flores-Bellver, M., et al. (2016). Magnetic resonance imaging structural alterations in brain of alcohol abusers and its association with impulsivity. Addict. Biol. 21, 962–971. doi: 10.1111/adb.12257
Barbas, H. (2000). Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices. Brain Res. Bull. 52, 319–330. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00245-2
Barrett, R. M., and Wood, M. A. (2008). Beyond transcription factors: the role of chromatin modifying enzymes in regulating transcription required for memory. Learn. Mem. 15, 460–467. doi: 10.1101/lm.917508
Bartholomew, J. B., Morrison, D., and Ciccolo, J. T. (2005). Effects of acute exercise on mood and well-being in patients with major depressive disorder. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 37, 2032–2037. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000178101.78322.dd
Bartlett, J. D., Close, G. L., MacLaren, D. P. M., Gregson, W., Drust, B., and Morton, J. P. (2011). High-intensity interval running is perceived to be more enjoyable than moderate-intensity continuous exercise: implications for exercise adherence. J. Sports Sci. 29, 547–553. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2010.545427
Basso, J. C., and Suzuki, W. A. (2017). The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology and neurochemical pathways: a review. Brain Plast. 2, 127–152. doi: 10.3233/BPL-160040
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., and Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 443–452. doi: 10.1038/nrn848
Berger, B., and Motl, R. (2001). “Physical activity and quality of life,” in Handbook of Sport Psychology , eds R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, and C. Janelle (New York, NY: Wiley), 636–670.
Google Scholar
Berger, B., and Tobar, D. (2011). “Exercise and quality of life,” in The NEW Sport and Exercise Psychology Companion , eds T. Morris and P. Terry (Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology), 483–505.
Biddle, S. J. H., Atkin, A. J., Cavill, N., and Foster, C. (2011). Correlates of physical activity in youth: a review of quantitative systematic reviews. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 4, 25–49. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2010.548528
Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A . 87, 5568–5572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.14.5568
Brown, B. M., Peiffer, J. J., Sohrabi, H. R., Mondal, A., Gupta, V. B., Rainey-Smith, S. R., et al. (2012). Intense physical activity is associated with cognitive performance in the elderly. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e191. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.118
Brown, J., Cooper-Kuhn, C. M., Kempermann, G., Van Praag, H., Winkler, J., Gage, F. H., et al. (2003). Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 2042–2046. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02647.x
Brunoni, A. R., Lopes, M., and Fregni, F. (2008). A systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical studies on major depression and BDNF levels: implications for the role of neuroplasticity in depression. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 11, 1169–1180. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708009309
Burkhalter, T. M., and Hillman, C. H. (2011). A narrative review of physical activity, nutrition, and obesity to cognition and scholastic performance across the human lifespan. Adv. Nutr. Int. Rev. J. 2, 201S−206S. doi: 10.3945/an.111.000331
Cabral, D. A., da Costa, K. G., Okano, A. H., Elsangedy, H. M., Rachetti, V. P., and Fontes, E. B. (2017). Improving cerebral oxygenation, cognition and autonomic nervous system control of a chronic alcohol abuser through a three-month running program. Addict. Behav. Rep. 6, 83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2017.08.004
Carek, P. J., Laibstain, S. E., and Carek, S. M. (2011). Exercise for the treatment of depression and anxiety. Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 41, 15–28. doi: 10.2190/PM.41.1.c
Cataldo, A. M., McPhie, D. L., Lange, N. T., Punzell, S., Elmiligy, S., Ye, N. Z., et al. (2010). Abnormalities in mitochondrial structure in cells from patients with bipolar disorder. Am. J. Pathol. 177, 575–585. doi: 10.2353/ajpath.2010.081068
Cechinel, L. R., Basso, C. G., Bertoldi, K., Schallenberger, B., de Meireles, L. C. F., and Siqueira, I. R. (2016). Treadmill exercise induces age and protocol-dependent epigenetic changes in prefrontal cortex of Wistar rats. Behav. Brain Res. 313, 82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.016
Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K. I., Holtrop, J. L., Voss, M. W., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2014). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater white matter integrity in children. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:584. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00584
Chang, M., Jonsson, P. V., Snaedal, J., Bjornsson, S., Saczynski, J. S., Aspelund, T., et al. (2010). The effect of midlife physical activity on cognitive function among older adults: ages—Reykjavik study. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 65A, 1369–1374. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq152
Chang, Y. K., and Etnier, J. L. (2009). Exploring the dose-response relationship between resistance exercise intensity and cognitive function. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 31, 640–656. doi: 10.1123/jsep.31.5.640
Chang, Y. K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I., and Etnier, J. L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. 1453, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068
Chang, Y. K., Tsai, C. L., Hung, T. M., So, E. C., Chen, F. T., and Etnier, J. L. (2011). Effects of acute exercise on executive function: a study with a tower of London task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33, 847–865. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.6.847
Chieffi, S., Messina, G., Villano, I., Messina, A., Valenzano, A., Moscatelli, F., et al. (2017). Neuroprotective effects of physical activity: evidence from human and animal studies. Front. Neurol. 8:188. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00188
Coelho, F. G., de Gobbi, S., Andreatto, C. A. A., Corazza, D. I., Pedroso, R. V., and Santos-Galduróz, R. F. (2013). Physical exercise modulates peripheral levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF): a systematic review of experimental studies in the elderly. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 56, 10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2012.06.003
Colberg, S. R., Somma, C. T., and Sechrist, S. R. (2008). Physical activity participation may offset some of the negative impact of diabetes on cognitive function. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 9, 434–438. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2008.03.014
Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P. E., Kim, J. S., Prakash, R., McAuley, E., et al. (2006). Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 61, 1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166
Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430
Craft, L. L. (2005). Exercise and clinical depression: examining two psychological mechanisms. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 6, 151–171. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2003.11.003
Craft, L. L., and Perna, F. M. (2004). The benefits of exercise for the clinically depressed. Prim. Care Companion J. Clin. Psychiatry 6, 104–111. doi: 10.4088/PCC.v06n0301
Cutuli, D., Berretta, E., Caporali, P., Sampedro-Piquero, P., De Bartolo, P., Laricchiuta, D., et al. (2018). Effects of pre-reproductive maternal enrichment on maternal care, offspring's play behavior and oxytocinergic neurons. Neuropharmacology. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.02.015. [Epub ahead of print].
Cutuli, D., Berretta, E., Pasqualini, G., De Bartolo, P., Caporali, P., Laricchiuta, D., et al. (2017). Influence of pre-reproductive maternal enrichment on coping response to stress and expression of c-Fos and glucocorticoid receptors in adolescent offspring. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:73. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00073
DeBoer, L. B., Powers, M. B., Utschig, A. C., Otto, M. W., and Smits, J. A. J. (2012). Exploring exercise as an avenue for the treatment of anxiety disorders. Expert Rev. Neurother. 12, 1011–1022. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.73
Deeny, S. P., Poeppel, D., Zimmerman, J. B., Roth, S. M., Brandauer, J., Witkowski, S., et al. (2008). Exercise, APOE, and working memory: MEG and behavioral evidence for benefit of exercise in epsilon4 carriers. Biol. Psychol. 78, 179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.02.007
Deibel, S. H., Zelinski, E. L., Keeley, R. J., Kovalchuk, O., and McDonald, R. J. (2015). Epigenetic alterations in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and hippocampus contribute to age-related cognitive decline. Oncotarget 6, 23181–23203. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.4036
De Moor, M. H., Beem, A. L., Stubbe, J. H., Boomsma, D. I., and De Geus, E. J. C. (2006). Regular exercise, anxiety, depression and personality: a population-based study. Prev. Med. 42, 273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.12.002
de Souza Moura, A. M., Lamego, M. K., Paes, F., Rocha, N. B. F., Simoes-Silva, V., Rocha, S. A., et al. (2015). Comparison among aerobic exercise and other types of interventions to treat depression: a systematic review. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 14, 1171–1183. doi: 10.2174/1871527315666151111120714
Dietrich, A., and McDaniel, W. F. (2004). Endocannabinoids and exercise. Br. J. Sports Med. 38, 536–541. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2004.011718
Ding, Q., Vaynman, S., Akhavan, M., Ying, Z., and Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2006). Insulin-like growth factor I interfaces with brain-derived neurotrophic factor-mediated synaptic plasticity to modulate aspects of exercise-induced cognitive function. Neuroscience 140, 823–833. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.02.084
Donnelly, J. E., Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D., Etnier, J. L., Lee, S., Tomporowski, P., et al. (2016). Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 48, 1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901
Douw, L., Nieboer, D., van Dijk, B. W., Stam, C. J., and Twisk, J. W. (2014). A healthy brain in a healthy body: brain network correlates of physical and mental fitness. PLoS ONE 9:e88202. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088202
Ehninger, D., and Kempermann, G. (2003). Regional effects of wheel running and environmental enrichment on cell genesis and microglia proliferation in the adult murine neocortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 845–851. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.8.845
Ekstrand, J., Hellsten, J., and Tingström, A. (2008). Environmental enrichment, exercise and corticosterone affect endothelial cell proliferation in adult rat hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 442, 203–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.085
Erickson, K. I., Miller, D. L., Weinstein, A. M., Akl, S. L., and Banducci, S. (2012). Physical activity and brain plasticity in late adulthood: a conceptual and comprehensive review. Ageing Res. 3, 34–47. doi: 10.4081/ar.2012.e6
Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S., et al. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19, 1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547
Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108
Farioli-Vecchioli, S., Sacchetti, S., di Robilant, N. V., and Cutuli, D. (2018). The role of physical exercise and omega-3 fatty acids in depressive illness in the elderly. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 16, 308–326. doi: 10.2174/1570159X15666170912113852
Fernandes, J., Arida, R. M., and Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2017). Physical exercise as an epigenetic modulator of brain plasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 443–456. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.012
Ferreira-Vieira, T. H., Bastos, C. P., Pereira, G. S., Moreira, F. A., and Massensini, A. R. (2014). A role for the endocannabinoid system in exercise-induced spatial memory enhancement in mice. Hippocampus 24, 79–88. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22206
Foster, P. P. (2015). Role of physical and mental training in brain network configuration. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:117. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00117
Fox, K. R. (2000). Self-esteem, self-perceptions and exercise. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 31, 228–240.
Fuss, J., Steinle, J., Bindila, L., Auer, M. K., Kirchherr, H., Lutz, B., et al. (2015). A runner's high depends on cannabinoid receptors in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 13105–13108. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514996112
Gelfo, F., Mandolesi, L., Serra, L., Sorrentino, G., and Caltagirone, C. (2018). The neuroprotective effects of experience on cognitive functions: evidence from animal studies on the neurobiological bases of brain reserve. Neuroscience 370, 218–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.065
Giesen, E. S., Deimel, H., and Bloch, W. (2015). Clinical exercise interventions in alcohol use disorders: a systematic review. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 52, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2014.12.001
Gomes da Silva, S., Araujo, B. H. S., Cossa, A. C., Scorza, F. A., Cavalheiro, E. A., Naffah-Mazzacoratti Mda, G., et al. (2010). Physical exercise in adolescence changes CB1 cannabinoid receptor expression in the rat brain. Neurochem. Int. 57, 492–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2010.07.001
Grazioli, E., Dimauro, I., Mercatelli, N., Wang, G., Pitsiladis, Y., Di Luigi, L., et al. (2017). Physical activity in the prevention of human diseases: role of epigenetic modifications. BMC Genomics 18:802. doi: 10.1186/s12864-017-4193-5
Grego, F., Vallier, J. M., Collardeau, M., Rousseu, C., Cremieux, J., and Brisswalter, J. (2005). Influence of exercise duration and hydration status on cognitive function during prolonged cycling exercise. Int. J. Sports Med. 26, 27–33. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-817915
Haasova, M., Warren, F. C., Ussher, M., Janse Van Rensburg, K., Faulkner, G., Cropley, M., et al. (2013). The acute effects of physical activity on cigarette cravings: systematic review and meta-analysis with individual participant data. Addiction 108, 26–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.04034.x
Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298
Hirase, H., and Shinohara, Y. (2014). Transformation of cortical and hippocampal neural circuit by environmental enrichment. Neuroscience 280, 282–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.031
Hollamby, A., Davelaar, E. J., and Cadar, D. (2017). Increased physical fitness is associated with higher executive functioning in people with dementia. Front. Public Heal. 5:346. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00346
Hötting, K., and Röder, B. (2013). Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2243–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.005
Hötting, K., Schickert, N., Kaiser, J., Röder, B., and Schmidt-Kassow, M. (2016). The effects of acute physical exercise on memory, peripheral bdnf, and cortisol in young adults. Neural Plast. 2016, 1–12. doi: 10.1155/2016/6860573
Hu, T., Zhou, F. J., Chang, Y. F., Li, Y. S., Liu, G. C., Hong, Y., et al. (2015). miR21 is associated with the cognitive improvement following voluntary running wheel exercise in TBI mice. J. Mol. Neurosci. 57, 114–122. doi: 10.1007/s12031-015-0584-8
Huang, P., Fang, R., Li, B. Y., and Chen, S.-D. (2016). Exercise-related changes of networks in aging and mild cognitive impairment brain. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:47. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00047
Ieraci, A., Mallei, A., Musazzi, L., and Popoli, M. (2015). Physical exercise and acute restraint stress differentially modulate hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor transcripts and epigenetic mechanisms in mice. Hippocampus 25, 1380–1392. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22458
Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., Black, J. E., and Greenough, W. T. (1992). Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis in the adult rat cerebellum after vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12, 110–119. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.14
Ji, E. S., Kim, Y. M., Shin, M. S., Kim, C. J., Lee, K. S., Kim, K., et al. (2015). Treadmill exercise enhances spatial learning ability through suppressing hippocampal apoptosis in Huntington's disease rats. J. Exerc. Rehabil. 11, 133–139. doi: 10.12965/jer.150212
Kim, S., and Kaang, B.-K. (2017). Epigenetic regulation and chromatin remodeling in learning and memory. Exp. Mol. Med. 49:e281. doi: 10.1038/emm.2016.140
Kleim, J. A., Cooper, N. R., and VandenBerg, P. M. (2002). Exercise induces angiogenesis but does not alter movement representations within rat motor cortex. Brain Res. 934, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02239-4
Knapen, J., Sommerijns, E., Vancampfort, D., Sienaert, P., Pieters, G., Haake, P., et al. (2009). State anxiety and subjective well-being responses to acute bouts of aerobic exercise in patients with depressive and anxiety disorders. Br. J. Sports Med. 43, 756–759. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2008.052654
Korb, A., Bonetti, L. V., Da Silva, S. A., Marcuzzo, S., Ilha, J., Bertagnolli, M., et al. (2010). Effect of treadmill exercise on serotonin immunoreactivity in medullary raphe nuclei and spinal cord following sciatic nerve transection in rats. Neurochem. Res. 35, 380–389. doi: 10.1007/s11064-009-0066-x
Kou, X., Li, J., Liu, X., Chang, J., Zhao, Q., Jia, S., et al. (2017). Swimming attenuates d-galactose-induced brain aging via suppressing miR-34a-mediated autophagy impairment and abnormal mitochondrial dynamics. J. Appl. Physiol. 122, 1462–1469. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00018.2017
Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., et al. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 400, 418–419. doi: 10.1038/22682
Kronenberg, G., Bick-Sander, A., Bunk, E., Wolf, C., Ehninger, D., and Kempermann, G. (2006). Physical exercise prevents age-related decline in precursor cell activity in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1505–1513. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.016
Lafenetre, P., Leske, O., Wahle, P., and Heumann, R. (2011). The beneficial effects of physical activity on impaired adult neurogenesis and cognitive performance. Front. Neurosci. 5:51. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00051
Lambourne, K., and Tomporowski, P. (2010). The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 1341, 12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091
Landers, D. M., and Arent, S. M. (2001). “Physical activity and mental health,” in Handbook of Sport Psychology , eds R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, and C. Janelle (New York, NY: Wiley), 740–765.
Langdon, K. D., and Corbett, D. (2012). Improved working memory following novel combinations of physical and cognitive activity. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 26, 523–532. doi: 10.1177/1545968311425919
Lees, C., and Hopkins, J. (2013). Effect of aerobic exercise on cognition, academic achievement, and psychosocial function in children: a systematic review of randomized control trials. Prev. Chronic Dis. 10:130010. doi: 10.5888/pcd10.130010
Lin, T. W., and Kuo, Y. M. (2013). Exercise benefits brain function: the monoamine connection. Brain Sci. 3, 39–53. doi: 10.3390/brainsci3010039
Lin, T. W., Shih, Y. H., Chen, S. J., Lien, C. H., Chang, C. Y., Huang, T. Y., et al. (2015). Running exercise delays neurodegeneration in amygdala and hippocampus of Alzheimer's disease (APP/PS1) transgenic mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 118, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.12.005
Lista, I., and Sorrentino, G. (2010). Biological mechanisms of physical activity in preventing cognitive decline. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 30, 493–503. doi: 10.1007/s10571-009-9488-x
Loprinzi, P. D., Frith, E., Edwards, M. K., Sng, E., and Ashpole, N. (2018). The effects of exercise on memory function among young to middle-aged adults: systematic review and recommendations for future research. Am. J. Heal. Promot. 32, 691–704. doi: 10.1177/0890117117737409
Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., and Pühse, U. (2016). Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: a meta-analysis. Psychophysiology 53, 1611–1626. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12736
Maejima, H., Kanemura, N., Kokubun, T., Murata, K., and Takayanagi, K. (2018). Exercise enhances cognitive function and neurotrophin expression in the hippocampus accompanied by changes in epigenetic programming in senescence-accelerated mice. Neurosci. Lett. 665, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.023
Mammen, G., and Faulkner, G. (2013). Physical activity and the prevention of depression: a systematic review of prospective studies. Am. J. Prev. Med. 45, 649–657. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.08.001
Mandolesi, L., Gelfo, F., Serra, L., Montuori, S., Polverino, A., Curcio, G., et al. (2017). Environmental factors promoting neural plasticity: insights from animal and human studies. Neural Plast. 2017, 1–10. doi: 10.1155/2017/7219461
Marsh, H. W., and Sonstroem, R. J. (1995). Importance ratings and specific components of physical self-concept: relevance to predicting global components of self-concept and exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 84–104. doi: 10.1123/jsep.17.1.84
Martinsen, E. W. (1990). Benefits of exercise for the treatment of depression. Sports Med. 9, 380–389. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199009060-00006
Meyer, J. D., Ellingson, L. D., Koltyn, K. F., Stegner, A. J., Kim, J. S., and Cook, D. B. (2016a). Psychobiological responses to preferred and prescribed intensity exercise in major depressive disorder. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 2207–2215. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001022
Meyer, J. D., Koltyn, K. F., Stegner, A. J., Kim, J.-S., and Cook, D. B. (2016b). Relationships between serum BDNF and the antidepressant effect of acute exercise in depressed women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 74, 286–294. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.022
Molteni, R., Ying, Z., and Gómez-Pinilla, F. (2002). Differential effects of acute and chronic exercise on plasticity-related genes in the rat hippocampus revealed by microarray. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 1107–1116. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02158.x
Niemann, C., Godde, B., Staudinger, U. M., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2014). Exercise-induced changes in basal ganglia volume and cognition in older adults. Neuroscience 281, 147–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.033
Nithianantharajah, J., and Hannan, A. J. (2006). Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn1970
Pahng, A. R., McGinn, M. A., and Paulsen, R. I. (2017). The prefrontal cortex as a critical gate of negative affect and motivation in alcohol use disorder. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 13, 139–143. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.004
Pan-Vazquez, A., Rye, N., Ameri, M., McSparron, B., Smallwood, G., Bickerdyke, J., et al. (2015). Impact of voluntary exercise and housing conditions on hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor, miR-124 and anxiety. Mol. Brain 8:40. doi: 10.1186/s13041-015-0128-8
Pedrinolla, A., Schena, F., and Venturelli, M. (2017). Resilience to Alzheimer's disease: the role of physical activity. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 14, 546–553.
Penedo, F. J., and Dahn, J. R. (2005). Exercise and well-being: a review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 18, 189–193. doi: 10.1097/00001504-200503000-00013
Pereira, A. C., Huddleston, D. E., Brickman, A. M., Sosunov, A. A., Hen, R., McKhann, G. M., et al. (2007). An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 5638–5643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611721104
Petrosini, L., De Bartolo, P., Foti, F., Gelfo, F., Cutuli, D., Leggio, M. G., et al. (2009). On whether the environmental enrichment may provide cognitive and brain reserves. Brain Res. Rev. 61, 221–239. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.07.002
Querido, J. S., and Sheel, A. W. (2007). Regulation of cerebral blood flow during exercise. Sport. Med. 37, 765–782. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200737090-00002
Raedeke, T. D. (2007). The relationship between enjoyment and affective responses to exercise. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 19, 105–115. doi: 10.1080/10413200601113638
Richter, H., Ambrée, O., Lewejohann, L., Herring, A., Keyvani, K., Paulus, W., et al. (2008). Wheel-running in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer's disease: protection or symptom? Behav. Brain Res. 190, 74–84. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.005
Robinson, A. M., Eggleston, R. L., and Bucci, D. J. (2012). Physical exercise and catecholamine reuptake inhibitors affect orienting behavior and social interaction in a rat model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 762–771. doi: 10.1037/a0030488
Rodgers, W. M., Markland, D., Selzler, A. M., Murray, T. C., and Wilson, P. M. (2014). Distinguishing perceived competence and self-efficacy: an example from exercise. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 85, 527–539. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2014.961050
Saab, B. J., and Mansuy, I. M. (2014). Neuroepigenetics of memory formation and impairment: the role of microRNAs. Neuropharmacology 80, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.01.026
Sachs, M. L. (1981). “Running addiction,” in Psychology of Running , eds M. H. Sachs and M. L. Sachs (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 116–127.
Schwellnus, M., Soligard, T., Alonso, J.-M., Bahr, R., Clarsen, B., Dijkstra, H. P., et al. (2016). How much is too much? (Part 2) International Olympic Committee consensus statement on load in sport and risk of illness. Br. J. Sports Med. 50, 1043–1052. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096572
Scully, D., Kremer, J., Meade, M. M., Graham, R., and Dudgeon, K. (1998). Physical exercise and psychological well being: a critical review. Br. J. Sports Med. 32, 111–120. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.32.2.111
Serra, L., Cercignani, M., Petrosini, L., Basile, B., Perri, R., Fadda, L., et al. (2011). Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive reserve in alzheimer disease. Rejuvenation Res. 14, 143–151. doi: 10.1089/rej.2010.1103
Short, A. K., Yeshurun, S., Powell, R., Perreau, V. M., Fox, A., Kim, J. H., et al. (2017). Exercise alters mouse sperm small noncoding RNAs and induces a transgenerational modification of male offspring conditioned fear and anxiety. Transl. Psychiatry 7:e1114. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.82
Sibley, B. A., and Etnier, J. L. (2003). The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 15, 243–256. doi: 10.1123/pes.15.3.243
Sim, Y. J. (2014). Treadmill exercise alleviates impairment of spatial learning ability through enhancing cell proliferation in the streptozotocin-induced Alzheimer's disease rats. J. Exerc. Rehabil. 10, 81–88. doi: 10.12965/jer.140102
Snigdha, S., de Rivera, C., Milgram, N. W., and Cotman, C. W. (2014). Exercise enhances memory consolidation in the aging brain. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:3. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00003
Steiner, B., Kronenberg, G., Jessberger, S., Brandt, M. D., Reuter, K., and Kempermann, G. (2004). Differential regulation of gliogenesis in the context of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Glia 46, 41–52. doi: 10.1002/glia.10337
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6
Stessman, J., Hammerman-Rozenberg, R., Cohen, A., Ein-Mor, E., and Jacobs, J. M. (2009). Physical activity, function, and longevity among the very old. Arch. Intern. Med. 169, 1476–1483. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.248
Swain, R. A., Harris, A. B., Wiener, E. C., Dutka, M. V., Morris, H. D., Theien, B. E., et al. (2003). Prolonged exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 117, 1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00664-4
Thomas, S. N., Schroeder, T., Secher, N. H., and Mitchell, J. H. (1989). Cerebral blood flow during submaximal and maximal dynamic exercise in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 67, 744–748. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1989.67.2.744
Thomson, D., Turner, A., Lauder, S., Gigler, M. E., Berk, L., Singh, A. B., et al. (2015). A brief review of exercise, bipolar disorder, and mechanistic pathways. Front. Psychol. 6:147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00147
Tiryaki-Sonmez, G., Vatansever, S., Olcucu, B., and Schoenfeld, B. (2015). Obesity, food intake and exercise: relationship with ghrelin. Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 7, 116–124. doi: 10.1515/bhk-2015-0018
Tomporowski, P. D. (2003). Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. 112, 297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8
Urso, M. L., and Clarkson, P. M. (2003). Oxidative stress, exercise, and antioxidant supplementation. Toxicology 189, 41–54. doi: 10.1016/S0300-483X(03)00151-3
van Praag, H. (2009). Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. Trends Neurosci. 32, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.007
van Praag, H., Christie, B. R., Sejnowski, T. J., and Gage, F. H. (1999a). Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 13427–13431.
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., and Gage, F. H. (1999b). Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 266–270.
van Praag, H., Shubert, T., Zhao, C., and Gage, F. H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J. Neurosci. 25, 8680–8685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005
Vatansever-Ozen, S., Tiryaki-Sonmez, G., Bugdayci, G., and Ozen, G. (2011). The effects of exercise on food intake and hunger: relationship with acylated ghrelin and leptin. J. Sports Sci. Med. 10, 283–291.
Vaynman, S., Ying, Z., and Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2004). Hippocampal BDNF mediates the efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2580–2590. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03720.x
Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2011). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater efficiency of the network underlying cognitive control in preadolescent children. Neuroscience 199, 166–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.10.009
Waddington, C. H. (1939). An Introduction to Modern Genetics . London: George Allen and Unwin Ltd.
Wang, J., Fan, Y., Dong, Y., Ma, M., Ma, Y., Dong, Y., et al. (2016). Alterations in brain structure and functional connectivity in alcohol dependent patients and possible association with impulsivity. PLoS ONE 11:e0161956. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161956
Weinberg, R. S., and Gould, D. (2015). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology, 6th Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Windle, G., Hughes, D., Linck, P., Russell, I., and Woods, B. (2010). Is exercise effective in promoting mental well-being in older age? A systematic review. Aging Ment. Health 14, 652–669. doi: 10.1080/13607861003713232
Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F. C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A., et al. (2007). High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 597–609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003
World Health Organization (2010). Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health . Geneva: WHO Press.
Yaffe, K., Fiocco, A. J., Lindquist, K., Vittinghoff, E., Simonsick, E. M., Newman, A. B., et al. (2009). Predictors of maintaining cognitive function in older adults: the Health ABC study. Neurology 72, 2029–2035. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a92c36
Young, S. N. (2007). How to increase serotonin in the human brain without drugs. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 32, 394–399.
Zamani Sani, S. H., Fathirezaie, Z., Brand, S., Pühse, U., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., Gerber, M., et al. (2016). Physical activity and self-esteem: testing direct and indirect relationships associated with psychological and physical mechanisms. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 12, 2617–2625. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S116811
Zanetti, M., Shigaeff, N., Menzes, A. H. T., and Takahashi, A. A. (2017). Cognitive reserve: evidence of delayed of dementia - a case report. J. Dement. 1:101.
Zubala, A., MacGillivray, S., Frost, H., Kroll, T., Skelton, D. A., Gavine, A., et al. (2017). Promotion of physical activity interventions for community dwelling older adults: a systematic review of reviews. PLoS ONE 12:e0180902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180902
Keywords: physical exercise, cognition, wellbeing, brain, epigenetic mechanisms
Citation: Mandolesi L, Polverino A, Montuori S, Foti F, Ferraioli G, Sorrentino P and Sorrentino G (2018) Effects of Physical Exercise on Cognitive Functioning and Wellbeing: Biological and Psychological Benefits. Front. Psychol . 9:509. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509
Received: 04 January 2018; Accepted: 26 March 2018; Published: 27 April 2018.
Reviewed by:
Copyright © 2018 Mandolesi, Polverino, Montuori, Foti, Ferraioli, Sorrentino and Sorrentino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Laura Mandolesi, [email protected]
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
- Undergraduate
- High School
- Architecture
- American History
- Asian History
- Antique Literature
- American Literature
- Asian Literature
- Classic English Literature
- World Literature
- Creative Writing
- Linguistics
- Criminal Justice
- Legal Issues
- Anthropology
- Archaeology
- Political Science
- World Affairs
- African-American Studies
- East European Studies
- Latin-American Studies
- Native-American Studies
- West European Studies
- Family and Consumer Science
- Social Issues
- Women and Gender Studies
- Social Work
- Natural Sciences
- Pharmacology
- Earth science
- Agriculture
- Agricultural Studies
- Computer Science
- IT Management
- Mathematics
- Investments
- Engineering and Technology
- Engineering
- Aeronautics
- Medicine and Health
- Alternative Medicine
- Communications and Media
- Advertising
- Communication Strategies
- Public Relations
- Educational Theories
- Teacher's Career
- Chicago/Turabian
- Company Analysis
- Education Theories
- Shakespeare
- Canadian Studies
- Food Safety
- Relation of Global Warming and Extreme Weather Condition
- Movie Review
- Admission Essay
- Annotated Bibliography
- Application Essay
- Article Critique
- Article Review
- Article Writing
- Book Review
- Business Plan
- Business Proposal
- Capstone Project
- Cover Letter
- Creative Essay
- Dissertation
- Dissertation - Abstract
- Dissertation - Conclusion
- Dissertation - Discussion
- Dissertation - Hypothesis
- Dissertation - Introduction
- Dissertation - Literature
- Dissertation - Methodology
- Dissertation - Results
- GCSE Coursework
- Grant Proposal
- Marketing Plan
- Multiple Choice Quiz
- Personal Statement
- Power Point Presentation
- Power Point Presentation With Speaker Notes
- Questionnaire
- Reaction Paper
Research Paper
- Research Proposal
- SWOT analysis
- Thesis Paper
- Online Quiz
- Literature Review
- Movie Analysis
- Statistics problem
- Math Problem
- All papers examples
- How It Works
- Money Back Policy
- Terms of Use
- Privacy Policy
- We Are Hiring
Physical Exercise and Mental Health, Essay Example
Pages: 3
Words: 925
Hire a Writer for Custom Essay
Use 10% Off Discount: "custom10" in 1 Click 👇
You are free to use it as an inspiration or a source for your own work.
Introduction
The value of physical exercise to the body cannot be overemphasized. Even though there are many perspectives pertaining to its appropriateness, physical exercise has numerous benefits, which cannot be outweighed by any other mechanism applied to the human body. This research embodies  ananalysis of the usefulness of physical exercise in maintaining mental health. First a reflection of characteristics that constitute physical exercise would be undertaken and second, perspectives of mental health will be outlined in relation to the topic. Finally,ananalysis relating the benefits of physical exercise  to mental will be explored.
Physical exercise
Physical exercise is a concept used to describe any activity the body is allowed to access that helps in maintaining or enhancing a person’s health and well-being. This includes strategies that provide vitality to organs in the body such the heart, lungs, muscular/ skeletal systems, kidneys, brain. Benefits are immense because this engagement allows athletesto master their skills through maintaining body weight, expiration and inspiration activities along with activating glands to produce perspiration. Perspiring is a significant excretory mechanism, which is often inadequately addressed in modern sciences due to attempts of inhibiting the odor (Hardman &Stensel, 2009).               .
The use of antiperspirants causes glands to limit perspiration production and sabotages the excretory process. Subsequently, many physical disturbances occur due to toxin built up Analysts have also confirmed that engaging in regular physical exercise greatly reduces incidences of heart disease, stroke, postpones aging, heart attacks, stimulating the immune system to function efficiently and reducing incidences of type 2 diabetes (Hardman &Stensel, 2009).
Mental health
Mental health relates to the extent psychological well-being is sustained. Essentially, from a non-scientific perspective mental health is the absencepersonality  disordersand brain dysfunction. When viewedfrom a holistic paradigm mental health includes a person’s ability to enjoy and appreciate the values of life. According to psychologists who investigate the human personality mental health could embrace attainment of lower level needs and reaching the highest self-actualization stage. Therefore, in reality mental health could also encompass one’s ability to appropriately cope with adversity and still remain balanced psychologically((Demyttenaere, Bruffaerts& Posada-Villa, 2004).
Further, World health Organization advances that a subjective wellbeing exists whereby a person’s equilibrium is related to the extent of autonomy is articulated; competence  expressed; intergenerational independence is executed and intellectual self-actualization is achieved. According to WHO, it also encompasses the person’s integration potential. This means a cohesiveability appropriatelyaligning one’s self to people, social and physical environments as well as staying connected. Ultimately, it must be clarified that there are diverse theories infirming mental health characteristics (Demyttenaere et.al, 2004).
The American psychological association has definite guidelines for interpreting and diagnosing mental disorders. However, scientists have admitted that it is difficult determining the extent to which mental health classifications are applicable across cultures and social environments. The primary concern of this discussion is analyzing the extent physical exercise promotes mental health or features of personality development consistent with adequate health mental. My theory relates to the perspective that it does promote mental health(Demyttenaere et.al, 2004).
If physical exercise is expected to enhance bodily functions; prevent disease and promote mental and social well-being, then, its role in mental health must be significant. Analysts have confirmed that it has immense impacts on depression; promotes self-esteem; augments a person’s body appeal, which stimulates physical attraction. Health care providers prescribe physical exercise in many of their health promotion strategies. It is often recognized as the miracle/wonder drug for a number of emotional issues. Therefore, if mental health relates to a person’s self-esteem, emotional equilibrium, physical exercise is then a major source of mental health maintenance.
For example, factors responsible for depression include low self-esteem, obesity, anxiety and stress. Research shows where endorphins are produced when people engage in physical activities. Further, studies reveal that this secretion initiates a response known as the runner high, linked to a euphoria created when physical activity is produced. Endorphin is an effective natural pain reliever and serves as an antidepressant when people experience various degree of the condition (Hardman Stensel, 2009).
Theories relating the release of other substances indicate thatanandamide is also a runner high initiator. Supporting researchconfirmedthatserotonin along with endorphin andanandamide levels are elevated in the blood stream. They remain that way days after physical exercise was experienced. Importantly, these endocrine secretions are responsible for mood stabilization weight management, which often affects mental health and ultimately improves self-esteem(Power, 2010).
It has been proven also that physical exercise alone combined with proper nutrition can maintain excellent mental health. There are more studies confirming successful application of physical exercise in depression prevention therapies. Analysts advanced that physical exercise is most effective on mental health when conducted as a group in gym settings or group walking therapies (Power, 2010).
The foregoing research highlighted perspectives relating the impact of physical exercise on mental health reflecting on differing viewpoints. Viewpoints on this subject differ based on classifications of mental health and physical exercise, which were clarified before an analysis was offered. Ultimately, in my point of view once a line of demarcation is drawn between mental health and mental disorder there is no need to differ regardingwhether physical exercise contributes to mental health. Physical exercise is necessary for the perpetuation of life itself.
Demyttenaere, K.Bruffaerts, R., & Posada-Villa, J. (2004). WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium. Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey. Journal of the American Medical Association . 291 (21); 2581–2590
Hardman, A., &Stensel, D. (2009). Physical Activity and Health: The Evidence Explained. London: Routledge
Power, A. (2010). Transforming the Nation’s Health: Next Steps in Mental Health Promotion. American Journal of Public Health 100 (12); 2343–6.
Stuck with your Essay?
Get in touch with one of our experts for instant help!
Richard the Lionhearted, Research Paper Example
Chinese Zodiac, Research Paper Example
Time is precious
don’t waste it!
Plagiarism-free guarantee
Privacy guarantee
Secure checkout
Money back guarantee
Related Essay Samples & Examples
Relatives, essay example.
Pages: 1
Words: 364
Voting as a Civic Responsibility, Essay Example
Words: 287
Utilitarianism and Its Applications, Essay Example
Words: 356
The Age-Related Changes of the Older Person, Essay Example
Pages: 2
Words: 448
The Problems ESOL Teachers Face, Essay Example
Pages: 8
Words: 2293
Should English Be the Primary Language? Essay Example
Pages: 4
Words: 999