U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

The Function of Repeating: The Relation Between Word class and Repetition Type in Developmental Stuttering

Anthony p. buhr.

Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University

Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Alabama.

Robin M. Jones

Edward g. conture, ellen m. kelly.

Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University.

Edward G. Conture [email protected] Mailing Address: Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Center, 1215 21st Ave. S., Medical Center East, South Tower, Nashville, TN 37232 Phone: 615-322-8780

Ellen M. Kelly [email protected] Mailing Address: Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Center, 1215 21st Ave. S., Medical Center East, South Tower, Nashville, TN 37232 Phone: (615) 936-5174

The purpose of the present study was to investigate repetitions associated with monosyllabic words in preschool-age children who stutter (CWS). Specifically, it was hypothesized that repetition type should vary according to word class in preschool-age CWS and children who do not stutter (CWNS).

Thirteen preschool-age CWS and 15 preschool-age CWNS produced age-appropriate narratives, which were transcribed and coded for part-word repetitions (PWR) and whole-word repetitions (WWR) occurring on monosyllabic words. Each repetition type location was also coded for word class (i.e., function vs. content).

Results indicated that although CWS and CWNS were significantly more likely to produce PWR on content words, this tendency did not differ between the two talker groups. Further, CWS and CWNS did not differ in their tendencies to produce PWR versus WWR overall, but the tendency to produce repetitions on function words was significantly greater for CWS versus CWNS.

Findings are taken to suggest that repetitions of monosyllabic words in young children are not easily explained from the perspective of phonological errors, but may instead be considered from an incremental planning of speech perspective.

1. Introduction

An issue that remains unresolved with respect to developmental (childhood) stuttering is the nature of a potential linguistic contribution to the occurrence of speech disfluencies (for review, see Ntourou, Conture, & Lipsey, 2011 ). As part of the traditional model of language production, it has been proposed that different repetition types are by-products of errors occurring at different levels of linguistic encoding (e.g., Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999 ), For example, part-word repetitions (PWR) are proposed to result from errors at the level of phonological encoding, and whole-word repetitions (WWR) are proposed to result from errors at the level of lexical planning ( Postma & Kolk, 1993 ). From the standpoint of the traditional model, content words are more susceptible to phonological encoding errors ( Garrett, 1975 ; Stemburger, 1984 ). Potential linguistic encoding problems can therefore be tested empirically by comparing the likelihood of producing PWR versus WWR on content versus function words. Using this framework, this study explored a potential phonological factor in childhood stuttering.

1.1 Part- and whole-word repetitions and linguistic encoding

According to the traditional model of language production, linguistic planning is thought to be a lexically driven process, in which both phonological and syntactic elements of a sentence are driven by the selection of a word ( Levelt, 1989 ; Levelt, et al. 1999 ). Accordingly, a phonological error can be detected prior to its occurrence in overt speech by monitoring internal speech. Once the error is detected, overt speech can be interrupted for error repair, and the speaker's retrace span, measured from the point of interruption to where the speaker restarts the utterance, is thought to reflect the level of planning at which the error occurred. For example, a speaker's correction of a syntactic error would involve a retrace span of a single phrase (e.g., “Go to the- to the left.”) or a single word (e.g., “Go to -to the left”). Correction of a phonological error would involve a retrace span of part of a word (e.g., “G -Go to the left”) 1 .

Speech errors are typically associated with content words such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives ( Fromkin, 1971 ). According to the traditional model of language production ( Levelt et al., 1999 ), speech errors can emerge during linguistic planning, as content words are assigned to their relevant slots within a syntactic structure (e.g., a noun phrase). During this process interference can occur between content words, resulting in anticipation, perseveration, or exchange errors ( Dell, 1986 ). Such interference is apparent in a tongue twister such as “she sells sea shells,” in which anticipation of sh in “shells” might result in the selection of sh at the syllable-initial position of the preceding word “sea”, resulting in an error. According to the traditional model, an interruption occurring within a content word would be assumed to originate at a phonological level. In contrast, function words such as conjunctions, prepositions, and determiners play a grammatical role in the serial ordering of content words, and are not thought to be associated with phonological errors ( Garrett, 1975 ).

1.2. Linguistic encoding

Among various language factors, a potential phonological factor has been theoretically identified as a core deficit that contributes to childhood stuttering (e.g., The Covert Repair Hypothesis, Postma & Kolk, 1993 ). Phonological factors have been empirically assessed in childhood stuttering in several ways, including 1) performance on standardized tests of phonological or articulation, 2) phonological complexity of stuttered words, and 3) phonological processing (for review, see Sasisekaran, 2014 ). First, CWS as a group do not appear to differ in terms of metaphonological abilities or on standardized measures of phonology (Bajaj, Paden, & Schommer-Aikins, 2004; Paden, Ambrose, & Yairi, 1999), although there may be a subgroup of CWS who exhibit poor performance on measures of phonology (e.g., Paden, Ambrose, & Yairi, 1999). Second, phonological complexity of the word spoken has not been found to influence the likelihood of stuttering ( Howell & Au-Yeung, 1995 ; Throneburg, Yairi, & Paden, 1994 ). However, Anderson (2007) reported that words associated with PWR were significantly lower in word frequency but not lower phonological density compared to fluently produced words. Third, empirical investigations have reported that stuttering frequency is not correlated to frequency of phonological process errors (e.g., Wolk, Blomgren, & Smith, 2000). However, Yaruss and Conture (1996) reported a correlation between stuttering frequency and slips of the tongue. As a result of equivocal evidence for a phonological factor in stuttering in the aforementioned studies, it has been speculated that a phonological factor does not contribute to stuttering (e.g., Nippold, 2002 ).

Given that the retrace span after repairing a phonological error is likely to occur within a content word ( Postma & Kolk, 1993 ), empirical studies regarding the tendency for children to stutter on function words presents a novel means to investigate a potential phonological factor in developmental stuttering. Although the traditional model would predict that phonological encoding problems should manifest on content words, young children tend to stutter on function words (e.g, Au-Yeung, Howell, & Pilgrim, 1998 ). Empirical studies have shown that stuttering is more likely to occur at the beginning of an utterance, and for preschool-age children, function words are more likely to occur at this initial position ( Buhr & Zebrowski, 2009 ; Richels, Buhr, Conture, & Ntourou, 2010 ). Although these findings suggest that sentence planning is related to the tendency to stutter on function words, factors related to the word itself could lead to stuttering on content words.

1.3 Purpose of study

To investigate a potential phonological factor in developmental stuttering, content and function words associated with whole-word repetitions (WWR) and part-word repetitions (PWR) were examined in young children who stutter (CWS) and young children who do not stutter (CWNS). The following three hypotheses were designed to test the relation between repetition type and word class:

First, it is hypothesized that both CWS and CWNS will tend to produce more PWR than WWR on content compared to function words. If this hypothesis is supported by empirical findings, it would suggest that each repetition type is more likely to manifest as a result of different aspects of sentence production.

Second, preschool-age CWS are hypothesized to be more likely than preschool-age CWNS to produce PWR relative to WWR overall, regardless of word class (function or content words). If this hypothesis is supported by empirical findings, it would lend support to the notion that phonological encoding difficulties play a prominent role in developmental stuttering.

Third, preschool-age CWS are hypothesized to be more likely than preschool-age CWNS to produce PWR relative to WWR on content words. If this hypothesis is supported by empirical findings, it would lend further support to the notion that phonological encoding difficulties play a prominent role in developmental stuttering.

2.1 Participants

Thirteen preschool-age children who stuttered (CWS; mean age = 48.0 months, range 37-60 months; 4 males) and 15 preschool-age children who did not stutter (CWNS; mean age = 49.0 months, range 37-59 months; 7 males) participated. All participants were monolingual English-speaking preschool-age children. Participants were paid volunteers whose parents learned of the study in a free local monthly parent magazine, were contacted from Tennessee State birth records or were referred to the Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Hearing and Speech Center for evaluation. None of the CWS had received treatment for their stuttering prior to or during the study. The protocol was approved by the Vanderbilt University Institutional Review Board. Parents provided informed consent and children assented to the study.

From an initial pool of 19 possible CWS and 21 possible CWNS, 5 CWS and 2 CWNS were eliminated because the low audio intensity of their audio-video recordings made it difficult to accurately transcribe their narratives. In addition, transcripts from 1 CWS and 4 CWNS were eliminated because stuttered repetitions (i.e., PWR and WWR) were too infrequent to permit meaningful assessment. Exclusion of these CWS and CWNS for the reasons stated resulted in the inclusion of 13 CWS, all of whom were Caucasians, and 15 CWNS, 14 of whom were Caucasians and one an African-American.

A child was considered a CWS if (a) three or more stuttered disfluencies (i.e., sound/syllable repetitions, monosyllabic whole-word repetitions, and sound prolongations) were produced per 100 words of conversational speech ( Clark, Conture, & Walden, 2013 ), and (b) received a score of 11 or higher (i.e., severity of at least “mild”) on the Stuttering Severity Instrument-3 (SSI-3; Riley, 1994 ). A child was considered a CWNS if (a) two or fewer stuttered disfluenices were produced per 100 words of conversational speech, and (b) received a score of 10 or lower (i.e., severity of less than “mild”) on the SSI-3. Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 .

Mean and standard deviation (SD) for preschool-age children who stutter (CWS; n = 13) and preschool-age children who do no stutter (CWNS; n = 15).

Note: SES = socioeconomic status; SSI score obtained from SSI-3; ST = stuttered disfluencies; NST = non-stuttered disfluencies. ST and NST rates obtained from transcriptions of narratives.

To minimize the possibility of stuttering being confounded by clinically significant concerns with speech or language abilities, children had to score above the 16 th percentile, or one standard deviation below the mean, on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition ( Dunn & Dunn, 1997 ), the Expressive Vocabulary Test ( Williams, 1997 ), the Test of Early Language Development-3 ( Hresko, Reid, & Hamill, 1999 ), and the “Sounds in Words” subtest of the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2; Goldman & Fristoe, 2000 ). Group means are presented in Table 2 .

Mean and standard deviation (SD) of standardized languages scores for preschool-age children who stutter (CWS; n = 13) and preschool-age children who do not stutter (CWNS; n = 15).

Note: TELD3-R = Test of Early Language Development-Receptive; TELD3-E = Test of Early Language Development-Expressive; PPVT3 = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; EVT = Expressive Vocabulary Test; GFTA2 = Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation.

2.2 Procedure

Upon arrival at the Vanderbilt University Developmental Stuttering Project laboratory, participants were led into a room and seated in a car safety seat situated directly in front of a computer monitor. This setup was designed to resemble a jeep to make participation more inviting for preschool-age children. Participants were asked to produce a narrative using one of four storybooks about a boy, a dog, and a frog, all by the author Mercer Mayer, including: Frog, Where Are You? (1969), A Boy, a Dog and a Frog (1967), Frog on his Own (1973) or A Boy, a Dog, a Frog and a Friend (1971) . The use of narratives provided a consistent speaking context across participants.

Audio/video recordings of these narratives were subsequently used to produce computer-based transcripts (SALT, Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts; Miller, & Iglesias, 2008 ). Utterance segmentation was based on identification of either: 1) a new independent clause, or 2) a pause of more than 1 second. Utterances containing singing, recitation, or unintelligible words, as well as those that were abandoned or interrupted were excluded from final data analysis. If a child listed information (e.g., “cow, dog, sheep...), items after the first two were placed in parentheses for exclusion from analysis. Finally, utterances consisting of a single word (e.g., yes, no) were included in the data set. Using these transcripts, the final data set consisted of 22,552 words within 4529 utterances across 15 CWNS and 13 CWS.

Several disfluency types were coded within each participant's transcribed narrative. Stuttered disfluencies included part-word repetitions (PWR), monosyllabic whole-word repetitions (WWR), and sound prolongations (PRO). Non-stuttered disfluencies included interjections (INT), multi-syllable word repetitions (MWR), phrase repetitions (PR), and revisions (REV). However, only repetitions occurring on monosyllabic words were used for the purposes of the present study, as noted earlier. Restricting analysis to only monosyllabic words allowed the researchers to control for potential differences in planning demand on multi- versus monosyllabic words.

A PWR was defined as the repetition of part of a single-syllable word, (e.g., “co- come over here”). A WWR was defined as a repetition of a single-syllable word (“come- come over here”). A PWR or WWR that occurred on a monosyllabic word associated with a phrase repetition was coded if the word within the repeated material also occurred in the final production (e.g., “g- go to go to the store”). Finally, monosyllabic words on which both a PWR and WWR occurred (i.e., a disfluency cluster) were not used for the present analysis. The occurrence of disfluency clusters was very infrequent. Word class was categorized using the Au-Yeung et al. (1998) scheme.

2.3 Measurement Reliability

One of the three narratives for each participant was chosen at random for assessment of measurement reliability and transcribed and coded by a second experimenter. At syllable locations, measurement reliability comparisons were made for fluency type (stuttered or fluent) and repetition type (PWR or WWR). For fluency type, agreement was 85% with a kappa coefficient of .70 and for repetition type, agreement was 91% with a kappa coefficient of .72.

2.4 Dependent measures

To assess between-group differences in frequency of repetition types of interest (i.e., PWR and WWR), repetition frequency was used as a dependent variable. Repetition frequency was defined as frequency of repetition type per 100 words. This measure was log-transformed on data combined from both groups to normalize its distribution ( Tumanova, Conture, Lambert & Walden, 2014 ). Finally, a regression model was used to evaluate potential between-group differences according to Age, Gender, and socioeconomic status (SES; Hollingshead, 1975 ).

To examine Hypotheses 1-3, a list of words on which either a PWR or a WWR occurred was first generated using SALT ( Miller & Iglesias, 2008 ) for both CWS and CWNS. For each word, the number of times a PWR versus a WWR occurred was recorded. As mentioned above, only monosyllabic words were used and each was identified as a content word or a function word. Content words included nouns, main verbs, adverbs, and adjectives. Function words included auxiliary verbs, pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions, and determiners. Thus, the words associated with PWR and WWR were coded as either a function or a content word for both CWS and CWNS.

2.5 Data Analysis

Demographic information and frequencies of pwr and wwr.

In addition to comparing CWS and CWNS on standardized language skills, log-transformed values of frequency of repetition type were used as dependent measures in independent samples t-tests to assess between-group differences in frequency of repetition type (i.e., PWR and WWR). Results of this analysis allowed researchers to confirm that CWS and CWNS significantly differed in their frequency per 100 words of PWR and WWR.

Relation between word class and repetition type: Within-group comparisons

Chi-square tests of independence were used to assess the first hypothesis, that is, whether CWS and CWNS were more likely to produce PWR than WWR on function vs. content words. Cramer's V is reported as the effect size, with a value of 0.1 indicating a small effect, a value of 0.3 indicating a moderate effect, and a value of 0.5 indicating a large effect.

Relation between word class and repetition type: Between-group comparisons

Chi-square tests of independence were used to assess between-group differences regarding the tendencies to produce 1) repetitions on function vs. content words (first hypothesis), 2) repetitions on PWR vs. WWR (second hypothesis), 3) PWR on content words (third hypothesis), and 4) WWR on function words. As with the above within-group comparison of the relation between word class and repetition type, Cramer's V was employed for this within-group assessment of the same relation.

3.1 Demographic information and frequencies of PWR and WWR

First, as might be expected based on SSI-3 group classification criteria, CWS produced significantly more stuttered repetitions, β = 1.071, t (22) = 4.602, p < 0.001. There were, however, no significant differences in repetition frequency according to Gender, β = −.366, t (22) = 1.479, p = .142, Age, β = .012, t (22) = 0.937, p = .351, or SES, β = .007, t (22) = 0.822, p = .413, and the interaction between Gender and SSI group classification was not significant, β = −.163, t (22) = 0.431, p = .667. Likewise, performance on standardized language tests did not significantly differ between CWS and CWNS (p > 0.10 for all comparisons). Finally, as shown in Table 3 , CWS produced a significantly greater repetition frequency of both PWR and WWR compared to CWNS.

Mean, standard deviation (SD), and range of frequency per 100 words, and between-group comparisons of frequency per 100 words (Welch t-tests), for preschool-age children who stutter (CWS; n = 13) and preschool-age children who do not stutter (CWNS; n = 15).

Note: PWR = part-word repetition; WWR = whole-word repetition.

3.2 Word class and repetition type: Within-group comparisons

First, regarding Hypothesis 1, results of chi-square tests of independence indicated that for CWS, the tendency to produce WWR versus PWR differed between function and content words, χ 2 (1, N = 448) = 7.143, p =.008, Cramer's V = 0.13. This tendency to produce WWR versus PWR also differed between function and content words for CWNS, χ 2 (1, N = 177) = 4.181, p = 0.041, Cramer's V = 0.15. As can be seen on the right hand side of Figure 1 , both groups tended to produce PWR on content words. And as can be seen on the left hand of Figure 1 , both groups tended to produce WWR on function words. These results suggest that each repetition type is more likely to manifest from different aspects of sentence production. Table 4 presents information regarding within-group comparisons.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms-694198-f0001.jpg

Interaction between word class (function vs. content) and repetition type for children who do stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS).

Results of within-group chi-square tests of independence used to assess if preschool-age children who stutter (CWS; n = 13) and preschool-age children who do not stutter (CWNS; n = 15) were more likely to produce PWR or WWR on content and function words.

3.3 Word class and repetition type: Between-group comparisons

First, results of chi-square tests of independence indicated that the tendency to produce repetitions on function compared to content words significantly differed between CWS and CWNS, χ 2 (1, N = 625) = 4.019, p = .045, Cramer's V = 0.08. Specifically, CWS exhibited a greater tendency than CWNS to produce repetitions on function versus content words. Second, regarding Hypothesis 2, CWS and CWNS did not differ in their tendency to produce PWR compared to WWR, regardless of word class, χ 2 (1, N = 625) = 0.959, p = .33. This finding does not support the notion that phonological encoding difficulties play a prominent role in developmental stuttering. Third, regarding Hypothesis 3, CWS and CWNS did not differ in their tendencies to produce PWR relative to WWR on content words, χ 2 (1, N = 625) = 0.117, p = .73. This finding also does not support the notion that phonological encoding difficulties play a prominent role in developmental stuttering. Finally, there was no significant difference between CWS and CWNS in their tendency to produce WWR on function words, χ 2 (1, N = 625) = 0.236, p = .63. Results of between-group comparisons are presented in Table 5 .

Results of between-group chi-square tests of independence used to assess if preschool-age children who stutter (CWS; n = 13) and preschool-age children who do not stutter (CWNS; n = 15) were more likely to produce PWR or WWR on content and function words.

3.4 Summary of results

The present study resulted in three main findings, relating to each of the three hypotheses. First, regarding Hypothesis 1, both CWS and CWNS produced more PWR than WWR on content words compared to function words. Second regarding Hypothesis 2, CWS were not more likely than CWNS to produce PWR than WWR overall. Rather, CWS were significantly more likely than CWNS to produce repetitions on function compared to content words. Third, regarding Hypothesis 3, CWS were not significantly more likely than CWNS to produce PWR on content words.

4. Discussion

The three main findings will be discussed first, followed by a more general discussion of the main findings.

4.1 Within-group comparisons of PWR relative to WWR on content and function words (Hypothesis 1)

First, regarding Hypothesis 1, CWS and CWNS both tended to produce WWR on function compared to content words ( Figure 1 ). Given that children tend to stutter at the beginning of an utterance, a position where function words are most likely to occur (e.g., Buhr & Zebrowski, 2009 ; Richels et al., 2010 ), this finding suggests that WWR occurring on function words is related to aspects of sentence planning. If the size of the retrace span is related to the linguistic unit being planned by the speaker ( Levelt et al., 1999 ), WWR may be related to increased planning requirements for lexical selection at the beginning of an utterance, the position where planning requirements are thought to be greatest (e.g., Clark & Wasow, 1998 ; Rispoli, Hadley, & Holt, 2008 ).

It can also be seen in Figure 1 that both CWS and CWNS did produce PWR on function words. Relevant to this finding, Wijnen (1992) reported that adults produced virtually no sound errors on what was defined as minor classes of words (i.e., adverbs, pronouns, determiners, prepositions), and his analysis of child speech samples revealed that only 24% of their sound errors were on minor classes of words. This suggests that PWR need not occur due to phonological encoding errors. Rather, other factors might have contributed to the occurrence of PWR for preschool-age children in the study.

4.2 Between-group comparison of PWR relative to WWR (Hypothesis 2)

Second, regarding Hypothesis 2, it was hypothesized that CWS would be more likely than CWNS to produce PWR than WWR, regardless of word class. To the extent that the phonological factors drive PWR, such a finding would indicate that phonological encoding difficulties play a prominent role in developmental stuttering. However, results did not reveal a difference between CWS and CWNS in their tendency to produce PWR versus WWR.

4.3 Between-group comparisons of PWR relative to WWR on content words (Hypothesis 3)

Finally, regarding Hypothesis 3, it was hypothesized that, if phonological factors contribute to developmental stuttering, CWS would be significantly more likely than CWNS to produce PWR on content words, as content words are thought to be most susceptible to phonological errors ( Garrett, 1975 ; Levelt, 1989 ; Stemberger, 1984 ). As can be seen in Figure 1 , of all repetitions produced on content words, the percentage that consisted of PWR (compared to WWR) was 61% for preschool-age CWNS and 56% for preschool-age CWS. Thus, although both groups tended to produce PWR on content words, this tendency did not distinguish CWS from CWNS. This finding does not support to the notion that phonological difficulties are involved in developmental stuttering.

4.4. Incremental Speech Production

In the present study, preschool-age CWS produced a greater frequency of repetitions than CWNS for both PWR and WWR, consistent with group classification and confirming that preschool-age CWS are less fluent in general ( Tumanova, et al., 2014 ). To the extent that different repetition types result from different levels of linguistic planning, a theoretical account is needed to explain this finding. One possibility is that WWR could also relate to phonological encoding errors. However, this would mean that the overt manifestation of a repetition would not reflect the origin of the original error (i.e., phonological or lexical), contrary to the traditional model ( Levelt et al, 1999 ).

A more parsimonious explanation of present findings is that repetition types relate to a single factor that can manifest in more than one way in overt speech (e.g., PWR or WWR). One possibility is the development of incremental speaking skills, or the ability to simultaneously articulate an utterance while planning what to articulate next ( Ferreira & Swets, 2002 ; Levelt et al., 1999 ). For example, a speaker who has made a syntactic commitment ( Clark & Wasow, 1998 ) may repeat the initial function word to “buy time” for planning (e.g., Au-Yeung, et al., 1998 ). To this end, a child's articulation of the phrase-initial function word of a noun phrase (e.g., The in “The boy...”) can begin if it agrees syntactically with the yet-to-be-selected content word (i.e., boy ), reflecting a speaker's commitment at a syntactic level of planning. Thus, the incremental nature of speech allows speakers to begin speaking while utterance planning is not yet completed.

With respect to the present study, the finding that preschool-age CWS are significantly more likely than preschool-age CWNS to produce repetitions on function words suggests that CWS may have greater difficulty with incremental speaking skills. This difficulty may result in a greater tendency to produce repetitions on function words as a tactic to “buy time” for further planning ( Au-Yeung et al., 1998 ). In addition, children for whom incremental speech is developing may take more time to plan what to articulate next, resulting in a greater likelihood of overt interruptions within an utterance. To this end, an interruption may occur either within or between words, and the retrace span may encompass one or multiple syllables.

4.5 Incremental Speech and Repetition Type

Children for whom incremental speaking skills are developing might produce a relatively greater frequency of PWR and WWR. The tendency to produce WWR on function words may be tied to utterance planning, and WWR on function words would therefore be expected to occur at the beginning of an utterance, where function words tend to occur for preschool-age children ( Buhr & Zebrowski, 2009 ; Richels et al., 2010 ). On the other hand, the greater frequency of PWR may relate to the ability to plan an interruption in advance . Clark and Wasow (1998) have suggested that speakers choose to interrupt between words and retrace back to a constituent boundary (e.g., “I wa- I want to go.”), thus facilitating formulation and comprehension for speaker and listener.

Empirical findings have shown that adult speakers choose to delay an interruption until planning has been completed ( Seyfeddinpur, Kita, & Indefrey, 2008 ), or to interrupt between words to make the resumption easier ( Tydgat, Stevens, Hartsuiker, & Pickering, 2011 ). To this end, children for whom incremental speech is developing may be less able to plan an interruption at syllable boundaries, resulting in a greater frequency of PWR. Thus, planning in advance an interruption to occur within a syllable versus between syllable boundaries is a possibility worth further empirical investigation.

4.6 Limitations

This present study has several limitations that would benefit from being addressed in future empirical investigations. One limitation is that only repetitions were examined. However, this made it possible to develop relatively straightforward hypotheses regarding the occurrence of PWR and WWR that could be tested in both CWS and CWNS. Subsequent empirical investigations in this area may also want to consider other disfluency types, for example, sound prolongations.

A second limitation is the gender distribution is unequal between CWS and CWNS and did not reflect the stuttering population at large. However, a regression model did not indicate any gender-related differences. It is worth considering that some children in the study were on different developmental tracks. For example, females are more likely to recover from stuttering ( Conture, 2001 ). However, it is unknown how different developmental tracks might have influenced the relation between word class and repetition type in preschool-age children.

A third limitation is that syllable stress was not examined. To the extent that young children are acquiring the prosodic patterns of their language, prosodic factors could also contribute to the occurrence of repetitions, particularly on content words (e.g., Natke, Sandrieser, van Ark, Pietrowski, & Kalvaram, 2004 ; Packman; Onslow, Richard, & van Doorn, 1996 ).

A fourth limitation is that narratives were used in the present study, whereas much previous literature has used conversational data. Although conversational and narrative tasks likely differ in some language production processes, it is not clear that this is true for phonological encoding, the primary focus of the study. Additionally, there is no empirical evidence that these authors are aware of that stuttering frequency significantly differs between conversations and narratives. This is an empirical question that must await future study.

A final limitation is that the design was descriptive rather than experimental in nature. Perhaps future studies may employ other speaking tasks that permit greater degree of control over factors relating to developing incremental speech production.

4.7 Conclusion

Findings of the present study revealed that, although both preschool-age CWS and CWNS were more likely to produce PWR than WWR on content words, this tendency did not differ between the two groups. This finding is inconsistent with the notion that stuttering of preschool-aged CWS is associated with phonological encoding difficulties. Although results of the present study do not rule out the possibility that at least some repetitions produced by preschool-age CWS could be by-products of phonological encoding errors, obtaining empirical evidence in support of this possibility is a challenge for future empirical studies. An alternative explanation of the present findings is that incremental planning of speech parsimoniously accounts for the occurrence of PWR and WWR on content versus function words.

What this paper adds

It is already known that preschool-age children who stutter tend to stutter on function words at the beginning of sentences. It is also known that this tendency exists for young children who do not stutter as well. However, the precise relation between word class and repetition type in preschool-age stuttering is unknown.

This study specifically adds key detail that preschool age children who stutter and preschool-age children who do not stutter both tend to produce whole-word repetitions on function words and part-word repetitions on content words. This study also adds the key detail that preschool-age children who stutter tend to stutter more on function words than content relative to preschool-age children who do not stutter. These findings suggest that the developing incremental speaking skills might better account for stuttering in preschool-age children compared to phonological processing accounts.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by an NICHD/NIH training grant T32-CH18921, NICHD Grant P30HD15052, NIDCD RO1 Grants 5RO1DC000523-16 and DC006477-01A2, and National Center for Research Resources (CTSA) grant (1 UL1 RR024975) to Vanderbilt University as well as a Vanderbilt University Discovery Grant. Special thanks to Dr. Tedra Walden for grant funding supporting this research. The research reported herein does not reflect the views of the NIH, NICHD, NIDCD, NCRR, Vanderbilt University, or the University of Alabama.

1 It should be noted that our definition of part-word repetition is an attempt at a particular utterance that involves the repetition of something less than the fully-formed syllable. To this end, vocalization that is produced with central tongue position (i.e., schwa) is not considered to be the vocalic portion of the syllable.

Contributor Information

Anthony P. Buhr, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University. Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Alabama.

Robin M. Jones, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University.

Edward G. Conture, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University.

Ellen M. Kelly, Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University.

  • Anderson JD. Phonological neighborhood and word frequency effects in the stuttered disfluencies of children who stutter. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007; 50 :229–247. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Au-Yeung J, Howell P, Pilgrim L. Phonological words and stuttering on function words. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1998; 41 :1019–1030. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bajaj A, Hodson B, Schommer-Aikins M. Performance on phonological and grammatical awareness metalinguistic tasks by children who stutter and their fluent peers. Journal of fluency disorders. 2004; 29 (1):63–77. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buhr AT, Zebrowski PM. Sentence position and syntactic complexity of stuttering in early childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2009; 34 :155–172. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clark E, Conture E, Walden T. Articulation abilities of preschool-age children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2013; 38 :324–41. PMCID: PMC3868004. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clark HH, Wasow T. Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology. 1998; 37 :201–242. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conture EG. Stuttering: Its nature, diagnosis, and treatment. Allyn & Bacon; Boston: 2001. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dell GS. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review. 1986; 93 :283–321. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed., PPVT-III American Guidance Service, Inc.; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feirreira F, Swets B. How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring computation of arithmetic sums. Journal of Memory and Language. 2002; 46 :57–84. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fromkin VA. The non-anomalous nature of anomalous sentences. Language. 1971; 47 :27–52. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Garrett MF. The analysis of sentence production. In: Bower GH, editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Academic Press; New York: 1975. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2) American Guidance Services, Inc.; Circle Pines, MN: 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hollingshead A. Four factor index of social status. Yale University; New Haven, CT.: 1975. Unpublished manuscript. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Howell P, Au-Yeung J. The association between stuttering, Brown's factors, and phonological categories in child stutterers ranging in age between 2 and 12 years. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1995; 20 :331–344. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hresko W, Reid D, Hammill D. Test of early language development (TELD) PRO-ED; Austin, TX: 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levelt WJM. Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition. 1983; 14 :41–104. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levelt WJM. Speaking: From intention to articulation. The MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 1989. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levelt WJM, Roelofs A, Meyers A. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999; 22 :1–38. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer M. A boy, a dog, and a frog. Dial Press; New York, NY: 1967. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer M. Frog, where are you? Dial Press; New York, NY: 1969. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer M. A boy, a dog, a frog and a friend. Dial Press; New York, NY: 1971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer M. Frog on his own. Dial Press; New York, NY: 1973. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller J, Iglesias A. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT). Research Version 2008 [Computer Software] SALT Software, LLC; 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Natke U, Sandrieser P, van Ark, Melanie, Pietrowsky R, Kalvaram KT. Linguistic stress, within-word position, and grammatical class in relation to early childhood stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2004; 29 :109–122. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nippold MA. Stuttering and phonology: Is there an interaction? American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. 2002; 11 (2):99–110. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ntourou K, Conture EG, Lipsey MW. Language abilities of children who stutter: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2011; 20 :163–179. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Packman A, Onslow M, Richard F, van Doorn J. Syllabic stress and variability: A model of stuttering. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 1996; 10 :235–263. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paden EP, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early Childhood Stuttering IIInitial Status of Phonological Abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999; 42 (5):1113–1124. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Postma A, Kolk H. The covert repair hypothesis: Prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993; 36 :472–487. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richels C, Buhr A, Conture E, Ntourou K. Utterance Complexity and Stuttering on Function Words in Preschool-Age Children who Stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2010; 35 :314–331. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Riley G. Stuttering Severity Instrument for Young Children-3. 3rd Ed. Pro-Ed.; Austin, TX: 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rispoli M, Hadley P, Holt J. Stalls and revisions: A developmental perspective on sentence production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2008; 51 :953–966. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sasisekaran J. Exploring the Link between Stuttering and Phonology: A Review and Implications for Treatment. Seminars in Speech and Language. 2014; 35 (2):95–113. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seyfeddinipur M, Kita S, Indefrey P. How speakers interrupt themselves in managing problems in speaking: Evidence from self-repairs. Cognition. 2008; 108 :837–942. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stemberger JP. Structural errors in normal and agrammatic speech. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 1984; 1 :281–313. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Throneburg RN, Yairi E, Paden EP. The relation between phonological difficulty and the occurrence of disfluencies in the early stage of stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994; 37 :504–509. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tumanova V, Conture E, Lambert W, Walden T. Speech disfluencies of preschool-age children who do and do not stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2014; 49 :25–41. PMCID: PMC4048759 [Available 2015/5/1] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tydgat I, Stevens M, Hartusuiker RJ, Pickering MJ. Deciding where to stop speaking. Journal of Memory and Language. 2011; 64 :359–380. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wijnen F. Incidental word and sound errors in young speakers. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992; 31 :734–755. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams KT. Expressive vocabulary test (EVT) American Guidance Service, Inc.; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wolk L, Blomgren M, Smith AB. The frequency of simultaneous disfluency and phonological errors in children: A preliminary investigation. Journal of fluency disorders. 2001; 25 (4):269–281. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yaruss JS, Conture EG. Stuttering and Phonological Disorders in ChildrenExamination of the Covert Repair Hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1996; 39 (2):349–364. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Effects of Time Pressure on Mechanisms of Speech Production and Self-Monitoring

  • Published: March 2001
  • Volume 30 , pages 163–184, ( 2001 )

Cite this article

  • Claudy C. E. Oomen 1 &
  • Albert Postma 1  

947 Accesses

51 Citations

Explore all metrics

The purpose of the present study was to examine effects of time pressure on mechanisms of speech production and self-monitoring. The most widely accepted monitoring theory (Levelt, 1989) suggests that monitoring proceeds through language perception, that is, speech error detection is primarily based on the parsing of one's own inner and overt speech. Twenty-four subjects described visual networks at two different rates (normal and fast). The time pressure manipulation affected a number of temporal characteristics: the error to cutoff and cutoff to repair times were shorter in the fast than in the normal condition. The results indicate that the monitor adjusts its speed of error detection and repair planning to the faster speech output rate. The time pressure manipulation did not affect the accuracy of error detection. The implications for the perception theory of monitoring are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Visual speech influences speech perception immediately but not automatically.

Holger Mitterer & Eva Reinisch

Rate dependent speech processing can be speech specific: Evidence from the perceptual disappearance of words under changes in context speech rate

Mark A. Pitt, Christine Szostak & Laura C. Dilley

repeating words in spontaneous speech

The effect of visually filled reproductions on the reproduced durations of auditory intervals

Miria N. Plastira & Marios N. Avraamides

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar  

Berg, T. (1986). The aftermath of error occurrence: psycholinguistic evidence from cut offs. Language and Communication, 6, 195-213.

Blackmer, E. R., & Mitton, J. L. (1991). Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech. Cognition, 39, 173-194.

Brédart, S. (1991). Word-interruption in self-repairing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 123-138.

Christenfeld, N. (1994). Options and ums. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13, 192-199.

Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. (1977). Psychology and Language . An Introduction to Psycholinguistics . Harcourt, New York.

Clark, H. H., & Wasow, W. (1998). Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 201-242.

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 124-142.

Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 313-349.

Dell, G. S., & Repka, R. J. (1992). Errors in inner speech. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition . New York: Plenum Press.

De Smedt, K., & Kempen, G. (1987). Incremental sentence production, self-correction and coordination. In G. Kempen (Ed.), Natural language generation (pp. 365-376). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

Feldstein, S, Crown, C. L., & Jaffe, J. (1991). Expectation and extraversion: influencing the perceived rate of tone-silence sequences. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 395-398.

Garnsey, S. M., & Dell, G. S. (1984). Some neurolinguistic implications of prearticulatory editing in production. Brain and Language, 23, 64-73.

Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech . New York: Academic Press.

Grosjean, F., & Lane, H. (1981). Temporal variables in the perception and production of spoken and sign languages. In P. D. Eimas & J. L. Miller(Eds.), Perspectives in the study of speech (pp. 207-238). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hartsuiker, R. J., & Kolk, H. H. J. (in press). Error monitoring in speech production: a computational test of the perceptual loop theory. Cognitive Psychology .

Henderson, A. I. (1974). Time patterns in spontaneous speech-cognitive stride or random walk? A reply to Jaffe et al . (1972). Language and Speech, 17, 119-125.

Howell, P., Kadi-Hanifi, K., & Joung, K. (1991). Phrase revisions in fluent and stuttering children. In H. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering . Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Jaffe, J., Anderson, S. W., & Rieber, R. W. (1973). Research and clinical approaches to disorders of speech rate. Journal of Communication Disorders, 6, 225-246.

Kempen, G. (submitted). Human Grammatical Coding. Book manuscript.

Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An incremental procedure grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive Science, 11, 201-258.

Kemper, S. (1992). Adults' sentence fragments: who, what, when, where, and why? Communication Research, 19, 444-458.

Klapp, S. T., Anderson, W. G., & Berrian, R. W. (1973). Implicit speech in reading, reconsidered. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 368-374.

Klapp, S. T., & Erwin, C. I. (1976). Relation between programming time and duration of the response being programmed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 59-598.

Kowall, S., O'Connell, D. C., & Sabin, E. J. (1975). Temporal patterning and vocal hesitations in spontaneous narratives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 195-204.

Lackner, J. R., & Tuller, B. H. (1979). Role of efference monitoring in the detection of selfproduced speech errors. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker(Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 281-294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Laver, J. D. M. (1980). Monitoring systems in the neurolinguistica control of speech production. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand (pp. 287-305). New York: Academic Press.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring in self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41-104.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1992). The perceptual loop theory not disconfirmed: A reply to MacKay. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 226-230.

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-37.

MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89, 483-506.

MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills . New York: Springer.

MacKay, D. G. (1992a). Errors, ambiguity, and awareness in language perception and production. In B. J. Baars(Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition . New York: Plenum Press.

MacKay, D. G. (1992b). Awareness and error detection: New theories and research paradigms. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 199-225.

Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. (1981). Central processes in speech understanding. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B259, 317-332.

Martin, N., Weisberg, R. W., & Saffran, E. M. (1989). Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: implications for models of lexical retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 462-485.

Miller, J. L., Grosjean, F., & Lomanto, C. (1984). Articulation rate and its variability in spontaneous speech: a reanalysis and some implications. Phonetica, 41, 215-225.

Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1995). Reduced resources and specific inpairments in normal and aphasic sentence comprehension. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 651-679.

Motley, M. T. (1980). Verification of "Freudian slips" and semantic prearticulatory editing via laboratory-induced spoonerisms. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand . New York: Academic Press.

Motley, M. T., Camden, C. T., & Baars, B. J. (1982). Covert formulation and editing of anomalies in speech production: Evidence from experimentally elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 578-594

Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwarts, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation, Vol. 4 (pp. 1-18). London: Wiley.

Postma, A. (2000). Detection of Errors during Speech Production: A Review of Speech Monitoring Models. Cognition, 77, 97-131.

Postma, A., & Kolk, H. H. J. (1992). The effects of noise masking and required accuracy on speech errors, disfluencies, and self-repairs. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 537-544.

Postma, A., & Kolk, H. H. J. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 472-487.

Postma, A., Kolk, H., & Povel, D. J. (1990). On the relation among speech errors, disfluencies, and self-repairs. Language and Speech, 33, 19-29.

Postma, A., & Noordanus, C. (1996). The production and detection of speech errors in silent, mouthed, noise-masked, and normal auditory feedback speech. Language and Speech, 39, 375-392.

Schlenck, K. J., Huber, W., & Willmes, K. (1987). Prepairs and repairs: different monitoring functions in aphasic language production. Brain and Language, 30, 226-244.

Shriberg, E. E. (1994). Preliminaries to a theory of speech disfluencies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.

Van Hest, G. W. C. M. (1996). Self-repair in L1 and L2 production. In R. Appel, G. Extra, K. Jaspaert, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Studies in multilingualism, Vol. 4, Tilburg: University Press.

Van Wijk, C., & Kempen, G. (1987). A dual system for producing self-repairs in spontaneous speech: evidence from experimentally elicited corrections. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 403-440.

Wheeldon, L. R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995). Monitoring the time course of phonological encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 311-334.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Psychological Laboratory, Utrecht University, The Netherlands

Claudy C. E. Oomen & Albert Postma

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Oomen, C.C.E., Postma, A. Effects of Time Pressure on Mechanisms of Speech Production and Self-Monitoring. J Psycholinguist Res 30 , 163–184 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010377828778

Download citation

Issue Date : March 2001

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010377828778

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • time pressure
  • speaking rate
  • speech monitoring
  • self-repair
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

 alt=

Thomas A Wasow

Clarence irving lewis professor in philosophy and professor of linguistics, emeritus and academic secretary to the university.

  • Print Profile
  • Email Profile
  • View Stanford-only Profile
  • Publications
  • (650) 723-4991 (office)

Additional Info

  • Mail Code: 2095
  • Other Names: Thomas Wasow

2023-24 Courses

  • Directed Reading LINGUIST 397 (Aut, Win, Sum)
  • Directed Research LINGUIST 398 (Aut, Win, Spr, Sum)
  • Dissertation Research LINGUIST 399 (Aut, Win, Spr, Sum)
  • Honors Research LINGUIST 198 (Win, Spr)
  • Independent Study LINGUIST 199 (Aut, Win, Spr, Sum)
  • Independent Study SYMSYS 196 (Spr)
  • Individual Work for Graduate Students PHIL 240 (Aut, Win, Spr)
  • Individual Work, Undergraduate PHIL 197 (Aut, Win, Spr, Sum)
  • M.A. Project LINGUIST 390 (Aut, Win)
  • Research Projects in Linguistics LINGUIST 396 (Win)
  • Tutorial, Senior Year PHIL 196 (Aut, Win, Spr)

2022-23 Courses

  • Governance, Culture, and Innovation in Oxford OSPGEN 47 (Sum)
  • Studying Stanford: Governance, Culture, and Innovation SYMSYS 176S (Spr)

Stanford Advisees

  • Doctoral Dissertation Reader (AC) Jiayi Lu

All Publications

We explore the consequences of letting the incremental and integrative nature of language processing inform the design of competence grammar. What emerges is a view of grammar as a system of local monotonic constraints that provide a direct characterization of the signs (the form-meaning correspondences) of a given language. This "sign-based" conception of grammar has provided precise solutions to the key problems long thought to motivate movement-based analyses, has supported three decades of computational research developing large-scale grammar implementations, and is now beginning to play a role in computational psycholinguistics research that explores the use of underspecification in the incremental computation of partial meanings.

View details for DOI 10.1007/s10936-014-9332-4

View details for PubMedID 25385276

View details for DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00236

View details for PubMedID 23637692

View details for DOI 10.1017/S0954394510000098

View details for Web of Science ID 000284051600002

View details for DOI 10.1215/00031283-2007-001

View details for Web of Science ID 000246711800001

View details for DOI 10.1016/j.lingua.2004.07.001

View details for Web of Science ID 000231722700002

View details for Web of Science ID 000230643300012

View details for DOI 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.006

View details for Web of Science ID 000221986600003

View details for Web of Science ID 000222202600007

View details for Web of Science ID 000086288300002

Speakers often repeat the first word of major constituents, as in, "I uh I wouldn't be surprised at that." Repeats like this divide into four stages: an initial commitment to the constituent (with "I"); the suspension of speech; a hiatus in speaking (filled with "uh"); and a restart of the constituent ("I wouldn't."). An analysis of all repeated articles and pronouns in two large corpora of spontaneous speech shows that the four stages reflect different principles. Speakers are more likely to make a premature commitment, immediately suspending their speech, as both the local constituent and the constituent containing it become more complex. They plan some of these suspensions from the start as preliminary commitments to what they are about to say. And they are more likely to restart a constituent the more their stopping has disrupted its delivery. We argue that the principles governing these stages are general and not specific to repeats.

View details for Web of Science ID 000077736900001

View details for PubMedID 9892548

View details for Web of Science ID A1997XB85400005

View details for Web of Science ID A1996VF67800004

View details for Web of Science ID A1995RD50600003

View details for Web of Science ID A1994PE76300003

View details for Web of Science ID A1991GX82700051

View details for Web of Science ID A1989U938600091

View details for Web of Science ID A1985AXC9500003

View details for Web of Science ID A1985AKH6800001

View details for Web of Science ID A1982PP57800004

View details for Web of Science ID A1979GR67600001

View details for Web of Science ID A1978FP60600002

View details for Web of Science ID A1977EK15500018

View details for Web of Science ID A1977EK15400003

View details for Web of Science ID A1976BY67600006

View details for Web of Science ID A1976CN58300004

View details for Web of Science ID A1975AS33900001

View details for Web of Science ID A1975AC73100008

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

Profile image of Thomas  Wasow

1998, Cognitive Psychology

Related Papers

Language and Cognitive Processes

Duane Watson

repeating words in spontaneous speech

Proceedings of LONDIAL

Scott Fraundorf

This study demonstrates that four common types of disfluency in discourse (fillers, silent pauses, repairs, and repeat- ed words) differ from one another on two dimensions related to language production processes: their temporal relation to speech production problems and the level of production at which those problems occurred. Participants' speech in a story- telling paradigm was coded for the four disfluency types. Comparisons between types in their relation to story events, to clause boundaries, to utterance length, to utterance position, and to other disfluencies suggest the four types reflect differ- ent difficulties in language production. Temporally, fillers, silent pauses, and repeats represent difficulties in upcoming speech, while repairs represent past difficulties. Fillers were most associated with discourse-level problems, while silent pauses were more associated with grammatical and phonological difficulty.

Michael Schnadt

Julie Belião

Nanette Veilleux

This study tests the hypothesis that three common types of disfluency (fillers, silent pauses, and repeated words) reflect variance in what strategies are available to the production system for responding to difficulty in language production. Participants' speech in a storytelling paradigm was coded for the three disfluency types. Repeats occurred most often when difficult material was already being produced and could be repeated, but fillers and silent pauses occurred most when difficult material was still being planned. Fillers were associated only with conceptual difficulties, consistent with the proposal that they reflect a communicative signal whereas silent pauses and repeats were also related to lexical and phonological difficulties. These differences are discussed in terms of different strategies available to the language production system.

Yasuharu Den

This paper compares, using our Japanese data, word repetitions with error repairs in terms of their temporal structures in order to examine whether or not the prolongation of first tokens in word repetitions, observed by Den and Clark (2000), is really an effect of the speaker&#39;s strategy. Analyses of 10 task-oriented Japanese dialogues reveal a difference between word repetitions and error repairs for the data involving cut-off in first tokens; in both types of disfluencies, the final phoneme of the first token is considerably prolonged, but the degree of the prolongation is much greater in word repetitions than in error repairs. These results support our view that prolonged first tokens in word rep-etitions are a product of a process under the speaker&#39;s control or intention.

Language and …

Heather Bortfeld , M. Schober , Susan Brennan

Jonathan Ginzburg , Raquel Fernández , David Schlangen , Raquel Fernandez

Although disfluent speech is pervasive in spoken conversation, disfluencies have received little attention within formal theories of grammar. The majority of work on disfluent language has come from psycholinguistic models of speech production and comprehension and from structural ap- proaches designed to improve performance in speech applications. In this paper, we argue for the inclusion of this phenomenon in the scope of formal grammar, and present a detailed formal account which: (a) unifies disfluencies (self-repair) with Clarification Requests, without conflating them, (b) offers a precise explication of the roles of all key components of a disfluency, including editing phrases and filled pauses, and (c) accounts for the possibility of self addressed questions in a disfluency.

RELATED PAPERS

iJSRED Journal

Giorgio Pivato

Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology

John Ohrvik

Anna Kristina Hultgren

Dyra Dzakirah

Bioorganic & Medicinal Chemistry Letters

Joseph Vacca

marcos tadeu

Applied Mathematical Sciences

Joseph Ackora-prah

Asia Pacific journal of clinical nutrition

Abdulkadir Eren

npj Clean Water

Muhammad Burhan

Débora Lúcia

achmad subhan

Military Medicine

Rubini Pasupathy

Osteoarthritis and Cartilage

Erika Lopes

Youcanprint

Pietro Ferrari

Journal of Mathematical Economics

Mauricio Bugarin

Journal of Applied Biosciences

Simon AHOUANSOU MONTCHO

FEBS Journal

Rune Forstrøm

veljko blagojevic

Biological Psychiatry

Gamal Salama

xavier luffin

Biophysical Journal

Ester Otarola

Journal of proteomics

Francesc Xavier Aviles

Revista Brasileira De Zootecnia

Marcello Augusto Dias da Cunha

WAFAE JARACHI

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

IMAGES

  1. Power Of Repeating Words

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

  2. (PDF) Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

  3. Spontaneous Speech: The Simple Guide To The Art Of "getting it right

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

  4. PPT

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

  5. PPT

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

  6. The Power of Repeated Words, Thoughts, Mental Pictures and Feelings

    repeating words in spontaneous speech

VIDEO

  1. Spontaneous worship and words of knowledge

  2. Direct Speech Interpreting

  3. permutation words repeating letters| part3 |by Niharika Panda

  4. Shukar Collective

  5. SPOKEN ENGLISH FOR NON-NATIVE SPEAKERS LECTURE 50

  6. Peek Behind the Words: Spontaneous Dialogues

COMMENTS

  1. PDF Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    REPEATING WORDS IN SPONTANEOUS SPEECH 203 The evidence we use comes from two large corpora of spontaneous speech, one American and one British. COMMIT-AND-RESTORE MODEL OF REPEATED WORDS Repeating a word is often treated as an unanalyzable event (e.g., Deese, 1984; Holmes, 1988), but is really a sequence of processes, each with its

  2. Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    Speakers often repeat the first word of major constituents, as in, "I uh I wouldn't be surprised at that.". Repeats like this divide into four stages: an initial commitment to the constituent (with "I"); the suspension of speech; a hiatus in speaking (filled with "uh"); and a restart of the constituent ("I wouldn't . . ."). An ...

  3. Repeating words in spontaneous speech.

    Speakers often repeat the first word of major constituents, as in, "I uh I wouldn't be surprised at that". Repeats like this divide into four stages: an initial commitment to the constituent (with "I"); the suspension of speech; a hiatus in speaking (filled with "uh"); and a restart of the constituent ("I wouldn't . . ."). An analysis of all repeated articles and pronouns in two large corpora ...

  4. Palilalia: What It Is, How It Presents, and More

    Palilalia is a rare speech disorder in which the speaker involuntarily repeats words, phrases, or sentences they have just spoken, often several times. The individual's speech typically decreases in audibility and often accelerates in speed with each iteration. It has also been observed that palilalia occurs in spontaneous speech, and rarely ...

  5. Palilalia

    Palilalia (from the Greek πάλιν (pálin) meaning "again" and λαλιά (laliá) meaning "speech" or "to talk"), a complex tic, is a language disorder characterized by the involuntary repetition of syllables, words, or phrases. It has features resembling other complex tics such as echolalia or coprolalia, but, unlike other aphasias, palilalia is based upon contextually correct speech.

  6. [PDF] Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech. H. H. Clark, T. Wasow. Published in Cognitive Psychology 1 December 1998. Linguistics. TLDR. It is argued that the principles governing these stages are general and not specific to repeats, and that speakers are more likely to make a premature commitment, immediately suspending their speech, as both the ...

  7. Assessment of Individuals with Primary Progressive Aphasia

    The Progressive Aphasia Language Scale (PALS) 19 also involves clinician ratings of speech-language features (motor speech and grammatical features in spontaneous speech, naming, single word repetition and comprehension, and sentence repetition and comprehension) but is based on signs observed during a prescribed set of speech-language tasks ...

  8. Diagnosing and managing post-stroke aphasia

    Spontaneous speech, naming, and repetition are often constrained to recurring utterances (e.g., "nuh, nuh, nuh"; parts of speech "I want to" etc.). ... the reliability of yes/no responses to ascertain if the patient has more reliable yes/no responses with gestures vs. speech. Repetition of words, phrases, and sentences should also be ...

  9. (PDF) Understanding spontaneous speech

    Spontaneous verbalizations are identified by: repeating words or phrases, complete or truncated preceding a restart; missing, unknown or mispronounced words as a possible gap in the semantics ...

  10. ERIC

    Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech. Clark, Herbert H.; Wasow, Thomas. Cognitive Psychology, v37 n3 p201-42 Dec 1998. Two large collections of spontaneous transcribed speech (over 2000 conversations), one U.S. and one British, were analyzed for word repetitions. Four stages of repeated words reflect different principles, and the principles ...

  11. PDF UNDERSTANDING SPONTANEOUS SPEECH

    Spontaneous speech contains a number of phenomena that cause problems for current systems. • filled pauses - noises made by the speaker that don't correspond to words (ah, uh, um, etc). • restarts - repeating a word or phrase. The original word or phrase may be complete or truncated.

  12. Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    An analysis of all repeated articles and pronouns in two large corpora of spontaneous speech shows that the four stages reflect different principles. Speakers are more likely to make a premature commitment, immediately suspending their speech, as both the local constituent and the constituent containing it become more complex.

  13. Repeated word production is inconsistent in both aphasia and apraxia of

    McNeil and colleagues (1995) compared four participants with a clinical diagnosis of AOS and mild or no aphasia to five participants with a clinical diagnosis of conduction aphasia and phonemic paraphasia. The participants' task was to repeat ten words, 2-5 syllables in length, three times sequentially (e.g. "butterfly, butterfly ...

  14. Herbert Clark's Profile

    Repeating words in spontaneous speech COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Clark, H. H., Wasow, T. 1998; 37 (3): 201-242. Abstract. ... An analysis of all repeated articles and pronouns in two large corpora of spontaneous speech shows that the four stages reflect different principles. Speakers are more likely to make a premature commitment, immediately ...

  15. [PDF] Speaking in time

    Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech. H. H. Clark T. Wasow. Linguistics. Cognitive Psychology. 1998; TLDR. It is argued that the principles governing these stages are general and not specific to repeats, and that speakers are more likely to make a premature commitment, immediately suspending their speech, as both the local constituent and the ...

  16. Repetitions

    Participants first saw a target word on a screen and then had to press a keyboard symbol on hearing it spoken, which occurred after a repetition in a recording of spontaneous speech. Tree established that target words following repetitions in spontaneous speech do not slow listener comprehension.

  17. Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking

    Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology (1998) J.E. Fox Tree et al. Pronouncing "the" as "thee" to signal problems in speaking. Cognition (1997) J.E. Fox Tree et al. Discourse markers in spontaneous speech: oh what a difference an oh makes. Journal of Memory and Language

  18. The Function of Repeating: The Relation Between Word class and

    4.5 Incremental Speech and Repetition Type. ... 1 It should be noted that our definition of part-word repetition is an attempt at a particular utterance that involves the repetition of something less than the fully-formed syllable. To this end, vocalization that is produced with central tongue position (i.e., schwa) is not considered to be the ...

  19. Effects of Time Pressure on Mechanisms of Speech Production ...

    Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 201-242. Google Scholar Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 124-142. Google Scholar Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors.

  20. Thomas A Wasow's Profile

    Repeating words in spontaneous speech COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Clark, H. H., Wasow, T. 1998; 37 (3): 201-242. Abstract. Speakers often repeat the first word of major constituents, as in, "I uh I wouldn't be surprised at that." Repeats like this divide into four stages: an initial commitment to the constituent (with "I"); the suspension of speech; a ...

  21. Spontaneous Speech

    Talkers' signaling of "new" and "old" words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language, 26 ( 5 ): 489 - 504. Google Scholar. Fox Tree, J. E. ( 1995 ). The Effects of False Starts and Repetitions on the Processing of Subsequent Words in Spontaneous Speech.

  22. Repeating Words in Spontaneous Speech

    REPEATING WORDS IN SPONTANEOUS SPEECH 203 The evidence we use comes from two large corpora of spontaneous speech, one American and one British. COMMIT-AND-RESTORE MODEL OF REPEATED WORDS Repeating a word is often treated as an unanalyzable event (e.g., Deese, 1984; Holmes, 1988), but is really a sequence of processes, each with its own options ...

  23. Repeating words in spontaneous speech.

    This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.